Abstract
Under some conditions, synaptically released glutamate can exert long-range actions in the cortical microcircuitry. To what extent glutamate spillover leads to direct cross talk among individual synapses remains unclear. We recorded NMDAR-mediated EPSCs in acute hippocampal slices at 35°C by stimulating two independent pathways that converge on the same CA1 pyramidal cell. Activation of a conditioning pathway in the presence of the use-dependent blocker dizocilpine maleate (MK801) resulted in partial NMDA receptor (NMDAR) blockade in the other, silent pathway. This was accompanied by an increase in the rise time of the EPSCs in the conditioning (although not the silent) pathway, implying an increase in diffusional distance from release site to NMDARs. We estimated that up to ∼30% of NMDARs contributing to EPSCs were activated by glutamate released from multiple synaptic sources; however, NMDAR-mediated synaptic cross talk was undetectable when NR2B subunit-containing receptors were blocked (but could be rescued by blocking glutamate uptake). We propose that NR2B-containing NMDARs can detect glutamate arising from multiple synapses, whereas NR2A-containing NMDARs only normally mediate direct synaptic transmission. These NMDAR isoforms thus play complementary roles in sensing global and local glutamate signals, respectively.
Introduction
Recent evidence suggests that synaptically released glutamate not only acts locally at intrasynaptic receptors, but it also escapes from the synaptic cleft to exert remote actions. Glutamate spillover, for instance, modulates GABA release in the cerebellum (Mitchell and Silver, 2000; Satake et al., 2000) and hippocampus (Min et al., 1998; Semyanov and Kullmann, 2000; Cossart et al., 2001; Semyanov and Kullmann, 2001). Repeated activation of cerebellar parallel fibers not only activates extrasynaptic NMDA receptors (NMDARs) (Clark and Cull-Candy, 2002) but also receptors on interneurons that are relatively far from the active synapses (Carter and Regehr, 2000). In the hippocampus, glutamate escape is implicated in the activation of extrasynaptic glutamate receptors and in heterosynaptic modulation of mossy fiber transmission (Vogt and Nicoll, 1999; Schmitz et al., 2001).
It is much less clear to what extent direct NMDAR-mediated cross talk occurs among small excitatory synapses (Kullmann, 1994; Asztely et al., 1997; Franks et al., 2002; Lehre and Rusakov, 2002). Diamond (2001) showed recently that a rapidly dissociating competitive NMDAR antagonist selectively reduced the slow component of NMDAR EPSCs. Although this implies that glutamate persists for a long time in the extracellular space and may activate extrasynaptic NMDARs, it does not necessarily follow that cooperation between neighboring synapses normally occurs. Arnth-Jensen et al. (2002) showed that increasing the density of release sites prolongs the decay of NMDAR EPSCs. Because responses of independent synapses were assumed to summate linearly, the increased decay suggested synaptic cooperation; however, because the extracellular space in CA1 hosts on average ∼0.2 mm high-affinity glutamate transporters (Lehre and Danbolt, 1998), glutamate originating from multiple sources could summate supralinearly in the extracellular space (see Materials and Methods). This suggests a scenario in which glutamate released from any individual site does not reach NMDARs in a recorded cell, whereas synchronous release from multiple sites partly overcomes the transporters, leading to activation of remote NMDARs. Because these receptors contribute to the slow current component, the observations of Arnth-Jensen et al. (2002) do not necessarily indicate that direct synaptic cross talk normally occurs.
Here we use an alternative approach to estimate the degree of NMDAR-mediated cross talk between two separately activated pathways. We also ask how different NMDAR subtypes contribute to this phenomenon. We find that, at a near-physiological temperature, up to 30–35% of NMDARs (depending on the density of active synapses and the efficiency of glutamate uptake) can be activated by more than one synaptic release site. Surprisingly, blockade of NR2B-containing NMDARs completely abolishes this intersynaptic cross talk, although it can be rescued by blocking glutamate uptake. The results argue for complementary roles of NR2A- and NR2B-containing NMDARs in sensing local versus global glutamate release in the cortex.
Materials and Methods
Male Sprague Dawley rats, 4–8 weeks old, were killed by cervical dislocation followed by decapitation. Hippocampal slices (350 μm thick) were transferred to a submersion-type recording chamber. The perfusion medium contained (in mm): 119 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1 MgCl2, 2 CaCl2, 26.2 NaHCO3, 1 NaH2PO4, 22 glucose, and 0.1 picrotoxin, and was bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2. All experiments were performed at 35–36°C. Stimuli (100 μsec square pulses) were delivered via bipolar stainless steel electrodes positioned in stratum radiatum. Whole-cell recordings were made with patch pipettes containing (in mm): 117.5 Cs gluconate, 17.5 CsCl, 10 HEPES, 0.2 EGTA, 8 NaCl, 2 Mg-ATP, 0.3 GTP, and 5 QX314 Br, pH 7.2, 290 mOsm. The series resistance was monitored throughout the experiment using a –3 mV step command and was in the range of 7–14 MΩ; cells showing an unstable (>30% change) series resistance or holding current were rejected. Evoked NMDAR EPSCs were recorded at Vm=+40 mV in 25 μm 2,3-dioxo-6-nitro-1,2,3,4-tetrahydrobenzo[f]quinoxaline-7-sulfonamide disodium, collected in series of 20–50 trials (>5 sec apart) of 150–500 msec duration using a 2 kHz low-pass filter (Axopatch 200B, Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA), and analyzed off-line (LabView, National Instruments); the intertrial interval and the number of trials in the presence of dizocilpine maleate (MK801) were set so that the total exposure to MK801 was similar (∼15 or 30 min) in all experiments in which the matching was required. Five minutes were allowed for wash-in and washout of MK801. NMDAR EPSC amplitudes were measured as the average current over a 100 msec interval after onset; this measure was selected to reduce the noise-related error of individual EPSC measurements. Application of MK801 reduced substantially the decay time constant of EPSCs, consistent with a reduction in the average NMDAR open time (Jahr, 1992; Rosenmund et al., 1993). This effect was only partly reversed after 5 min washout of MK801, indicating that washout was incomplete. To reduce possible bias caused by incomplete washout, we used only the first eight responses recorded after restarting stimulation. The amplitude variability of these responses was comparable with that of baseline responses: SEM <3–5% of the average amplitude. [This differed slightly from the approach of Carter and Regehr (2000), who measured a single response without washing out MK801.] The EPSC latency was measured at 70% of peak, which coincides roughly with the end of a near-linear segment in the onset of the average EPSC. The shortening of the EPSC rise time by MK801 (Rosenmund et al., 1993) was undetectable, probably because it was masked by reduced local shunting as the synaptic conductance decreased (see below). Chemicals were purchased from Sigma (St. Louis, MO), except for receptor antagonists and dl-threo-β-benzyloxyaspartate (TBOA), which were purchased from Tocris Cookson.
Supralinear summation of remotely activated NMDARs: kinetic estimates. To assess whether remote glutamate transients scale up linearly in the presence of glutamate transporters, we first used a simple approximation. Because the wave of glutamate released from a remote site can be considered quasi steady state within a few milliseconds of release (at least on a millisecond scale of changes), the kinetics of reversible binding to transporters leads to a quadratic equation that relates the concentrations of total and free glutamate, Gtot and G: (1) where Ttot (∼200 μm) and Kd (∼20 μm) stand for the known total extracellular concentration of (glial) transporters and their effective dissociation constant, respectively, in the CA1 neuropil (Danbolt, 2001). The NMDAR kinetics implies significant activation when glutamate is in the micromolar range (Lester and Jahr, 1992). It then follows from Equation 1 that, to reach G = 2.0 μm in the extracellular space, the total glutamate should be in the region of Gtot = 20 μm; this is also consistent with a detailed model of glutamate release and diffusion (Lehre and Rusakov, 2002). In these conditions, a fourfold increase in the number of release sites can be represented by substituting Gtot = 4 × 20 μm = 80 μm in Equation 1, which then yields G = 10.6 μm. This implies that the free glutamate transient increases 33% above what is expected from linear summation, to 10.6 μm instead of the expected 4 × 2.0 μm = 8.0 μm. Addressing the same question with a more explicit modeling experiment (see below), in which glutamate transients generated at 0.5–1.0 μm from the release sites are added in the presence of glutamate transporters, gives qualitatively similar results (data not shown).
Perisynaptic diffusion of glutamate. To simulate release, diffusion, and uptake of glutamate in the synaptic vicinity, we represented a “typical” CA1 excitatory synapse and its microenvironment with a three-dimensional (3D) compartmental model (with space partitioned both radially and tangentially) (Rusakov, 2001; Lehre and Rusakov, 2002). Briefly, the synaptic geometry included a flat cleft (width, 20 nm) between two hemispheres (consistent obstacles to diffusion; radius, 0.11 μm) separated from a homogeneous porous medium by a 20 nm gap. The perisynaptic medium had an extracellular fraction α = 0.12, tortuosity λ = 1.4, and a diffusion coefficient in the interstitial space D = 0.4 μm2/msec. This value of D is the upper limit estimate based on direct physical measurements of ion diffusivity in a narrow extracellular cleft (Kiessling et al., 2000). Compared with previous models (Rusakov and Kullmann, 1998; Barbour, 2001), this approach avoided the simplifying limitations of spherical symmetry and could incorporate glutamate transporters unevenly, in accordance with quantitative electron microscopy data (Lehre and Rusakov, 2002). Glutamate binding and uptake were represented by a simplified kinetic scheme (reflecting the upper limit of uptake and therefore a conservative estimate for extrasynaptic glutamate escape): (2a) where Glu, T, and GluT indicate free glutamate, free transporter, and the glutamate-transporter complex, respectively, k1 = 104m–1/msec–1, and k–1 = 0.2 msec–1; and the cycling rate set at its upper limit at 36°C, k2 = 0.1 msec–1 (Bergles and Jahr, 1998). This kinetics was translated into a set of finite-difference equations computed for each space compartment [note typographical errors in Eq. 3 (Rusakov, 2001)]: (2b) (2c) (2d) where the brackets indicate concentrations, time points are denoted by subscripts, and [Ttot] is the total extracellular concentration of transporters.
At each given space compartment, the NMDAR current was computed from the diffusion profiles of glutamate and a multistep NMDAR kinetic scheme (Lester and Jahr, 1992), as described previously (Rusakov and Kullmann, 1998). Because the extracellular concentration of high-affinity glutamate transporters is much higher than that of ionotropic glutamate receptors (Lehre and Danbolt, 1998; Nusser et al., 1998; Takumi et al., 1999; Racca et al., 2000), the effect of NMDARs on the glutamate concentration profile was considered negligible compared with that of transporters. Within the synaptic cleft, which is devoid of glial transporters, the same logic applies, simply because the total number of available NMDARs (20–40) is small compared with the number of released glutamate molecules (3000–5000). The EPSC latency changes (see Fig. 4C,D) were assessed using a single synapse model, with a cutoff distance of 5 μm.
To compute the average glutamate transient after synchronous release from several synapses, we adopted a conservative estimate of glutamate spillover. Spatial profiles of glutamate released from individual synapses scattered randomly within a 5-μm-sided cube were summated, and possible supralinear summation (attributable to the lower glutamate–transporter ratio after single, as opposed to multiple, synaptic release; see Supralinear summation above) was ignored. For simplicity, and to account for the arbitrary choice of presynaptic and postsynaptic sides at each synapse, glutamate concentrations at different distances from individual release sites were computed for space compartments coplanar to the synaptic cleft. The number of molecules released from an individual site was set at 5000. This value may be in excess of the glutamate contents of an individual vesicle with dimensions estimated from a recent ultrastructural study (Schikorski and Stevens, 1997), although it is consistent with other estimates (Harris and Sultan, 1995), including one based on rapid freeze-fracture and negative-staining electron microscopy (Zampighi and Fisher, 1997). Moreover, it represents a conservative estimate of the amount of glutamate exocytosed if multivesicular release occurs (Oertner et al., 2002). The total number of modeled synapses (250) matched the experimental synaptic density in area CA1 of 2.0 μm–3 (Rusakov et al., 1998). “Active” synapses in the scatter were selected randomly within the 5-μm-wide cube (with a 1 μm minus-sampling margin).
Synaptic current shunt. In the whole-cell configuration, the simplified recording circuit includes a pipette access resistance Rpip, an equivalent cell membrane resistance Rm and capacitance Cm, and a variable synaptic resistance Rsyn. When the voltage is clamped, the total circuit impedance Ztot at the input of the amplifier is Ztot = Rpip + (Rsyn–1 + Rm–1 + XC–1)–1, where XC = ωCm is the reactance of the capacitor Cm at a cycling frequency ω. The typical parameters of this circuit in the conditions of our experiments are as follows: (1) in the resting conditions, XC =∞, Rsyn =∞, and Rtot is 300–500 MΩ, (2) the pipette resistance is ∼2.5 MΩ, and (3) a 5 mV command voltage step (in the absence of synaptic activation, Rsyn = ∞) shows Ztot as an equivalent series resistance of <10–12 MΩ for the fastest current components and ∼20 MΩ for current components with a rise time constant of 8–15 msec, which is characteristic for NMDAR EPSCs. When the NMDAR synaptic conductance is activated, Rsyn can be approximated by Rsyn(t)–1 = A{–exp(–τ1/t) + exp(–τ2/t)}, where τ1 ∼8 msec, τ2 ∼80 msec, and A is an amplitude factor. In these conditions, a simple formula relates the time-dependent synaptic resistance Rsyn(t), and the conductance Ztot sampled by the amplifier: (3) where Ze = RsynXC/(Rsyn + XC) is the equivalent membrane impedance representing Rm and Cm combined. See Results and Figure 4 for additional details.
Results
Independence of afferent pathways in stratum radiatum
We made a 1.5- to 2-mm-long cut in stratum radiatum, parallel to stratum pyramidale, in rat hippocampal slices to separate two populations of axons (Fig. 1A). To test for presynaptic interactions between the pathways, we used a paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) protocol. When two stimuli were applied to one pathway with a 50 msec interval, both AMPA receptor (AMPAR) and NMDAR EPSCs exhibited PPF (EPSC2/EPSC1, mean ± SEM: 184 ± 7 and 145 ± 7%, n = 16 and n = 18, respectively) (Fig. 1B,C). In contrast, when the different pathways were alternately stimulated with the same interval, no cross-facilitation could be detected (101 ± 2 and 93 ± 4% for AMPAR- and NMDAR-mediated responses, respectively). The results imply that stimulation of one pathway did not recruit the fibers of the other pathway. Comparison of the mean PPF confidence limits for AMPAR responses indicates that, with a probability of >0.98, this cross-pathway contamination was <5%. [In the experiments in which two stimuli were applied to the same pathway, the facilitation of AMPAR-mediated EPSCs was 1.9-fold larger than that of NMDAR-mediated responses (see above; p < 0.001). Among possible mechanisms are that “NMDAR-only” synapses may have a different release probability (Poncer and Malinow, 2001) and that NMDA receptors occupied by glutamate released on the first stimulus are unavailable to mediate an incremental response to the second stimulus. We cannot exclude, however, imperfect recovery of the synaptic current arising from local shunting.] We observed a small but significant depression of the NMDAR responses when the two stimuli were applied to the different pathways (Fig. 1C) (cross-pathway stimulation; 93 ± 4%; p < 0.05). This depression was consistent with the hypothesis that high-affinity NMDARs (in contrast to low-affinity AMPARs) at one pathway bind glutamate released from the other pathway, thus resulting in partial occlusion of the response 50 msec after release. This temporal pooling is unlikely to apply to AMPARs because they have a faster off-rate (which is also evident from the AMPAR EPSC traces returning to the baseline long before the second stimulus) (Fig. 1B). An additional explanation for the small occlusion of the second NMDAR response is local shunting of synaptic currents (see below).
Heterosynaptic activation of NMDARs detected with MK801
Having established the independence of the two pathways converging on the same cell, we used the use-dependent NMDAR blocker MK801 (4 μm) to look for cross talk among the NMDARs mediating EPSCs in the two pathways (Carter and Regehr, 2000). The blocker was bath applied after recording a sample of baseline NMDAR EPSCs (Vm =+40 mV) elicited by stimulating the two pathways alternately. Single stimuli (25–55) 10–15 sec apart were then applied to one, conditioning pathway (CP) in the continued presence of MK801 until the EPSCs had decreased by 70–85% (on average, to ∼22% of baseline). Between stimuli, cells were held at Vm = –70 mV to reduce spontaneous activation of NMDARs. The choice of CP and silent pathway (SP) was random. After 5 min washout of MK801, we resumed stimulation of the SP to determine the degree of reduction in NMDAR-mediated EPSCs caused by MK801 blockade. Only the first eight responses after washout were used for analysis, because MK801 washout was usually incomplete and responses often continued to decrease (see Materials and Methods). Their mean amplitude was reduced by 56 ± 6% relative to baseline (n = 8; p < 0.001) (Fig. 2A,C).
To test whether this reduction can be explained by the spontaneous activation of NMDARs while MK801 was present in the bath, we interleaved these experiments with others in which neither pathway was stimulated in the presence of MK801. The protocol was otherwise identical. The average reduction of EPSCs in these “no-stimuli” control experiments was 28 ± 6% (n = 15; p < 0.001) (Fig. 2 B), half of the 56% EPSC reduction in the SP after CP stimulation (p < 0.02). This implies that neurotransmitter molecules escaping from CP synapses activate NMDARs in the SP.
Because glycine is an obligatory coagonist of NMDARs, the NMDAR-mediated cross talk could occur, in principle, through the release and spillover of glycine rather than glutamate. To test for this possibility, we applied a saturating concentration of the glycine-binding site agonist d-serine (100 μm). This had no effect on either the EPSC amplitude (98 ± 7% of baseline; n = 10) or its decrease in MK801 in no-stimuli control experiments (69 ± 5% of baseline compared with 72 ± 6% of baseline in the previous experiments; n = 6 and n = 15, respectively; exposure to MK801 was similar in all experiments). These data indicated that the activity of glycine-binding sites had no effect on NMDAR activation in our experiments.
If the MK801 blockade of NMDARs in the SP was caused by spillover of glutamate from the CP, this blockade should be enhanced when glutamate release is enhanced or when glutamate uptake is inhibited (Asztely et al., 1997; Carter and Regehr, 2000; Diamond, 2001). We applied trains of five stimuli at 20 Hz instead of single stimuli to the CP while maintaining the same duration of exposure of NMDARs to MK801. In this protocol, the release probability increase factor, estimated in separate experiments as the average amplitude of five AMPAR-mediated responses (AMPAR responses were required to exclude temporal pooling of NMDAR EPSCs) relative to the single (first) response amplitude in the train, was 1.22 ± 0.08 (n = 9; p < 0.03) (Fig. 2C, inset). The EPSC amplitude in this pathway was reduced to 10–15% of baseline (on average, to ∼12% of baseline). Accordingly, the SP responses were reduced to 28 ± 6% of baseline (p < 0.001; n = 8), indicating an approximately twofold enhancement of the NMDAR blockade in the SP compared with that seen with single stimuli. The duration of the exposure to MK801 and the voltage steps applied to the postsynaptic cells were as for the previous experiments using single pulses, implying that the same no-stimuli control data apply.
Clearly, any synaptic cross talk should depend on the density of release sites or activated synapses, or both. Does the measure of spillover used here (reduction in the SP NMDAR responses post-MK801) reflect this? Figure 2D shows that the decrease post-MK801 in the SP EPSCs (the measure of spillover) was indeed positively correlated (r = 0.60; p < 0.03) with the baseline EPSC amplitude in the CP (the measure of synaptic activation leading to spillover). This indicates that the extent of cross talk does increase, as expected, with the density of activated synapses. This prompts the question, however, of whether the densities of activated synapses in our experiments fall within the physiological range. Although it is impossible to reproduce the normal activity of the hippocampal circuitry in vitro, we took a simple approach by asking how the stimuli compared with the firing threshold of CA1 pyramidal cells (Arnth-Jensen et al., 2002). To estimate the action potential generation threshold, we recorded in cell-attached mode (without glutamate receptor blockers in the bath) and gradually increased the extracellular stimulus strength until an action potential could be detected (Fig. 2D, inset). Once the threshold was established, the membrane was broken, and NMDAR EPSCs were then recorded in whole-cell mode, using the same stimulus strength (with AMPA–kainate receptors blocked as in the cross-talk experiments described above). The average amplitude of the NMDAR EPSCs that corresponded to the cell firing threshold was 257 ± 82 pA (n = 9) (Fig. 2D, arrow). This was almost twice the average baseline amplitude of the CP NMDAR responses in the cross-talk experiments (140 ± 29 pA; n = 9; single stimulus experiments only). Thus, assuming linear scaling, the number of synapses activated in the CP corresponded, on average, to 140 of 257 = 54% of the synaptic density required to fire a pyramidal cell. Although this protocol cannot fully reproduce the conditions of spike generation in vivo (not the least because the interneuron network may alter the excitability of the circuitry), the expected asynchrony of firing in vivo might require an even higher density of synapses activated within short time intervals for the postsynaptic cell to fire.
Blocking glutamate uptake enhances cross talk
We then tested the effect of the glutamate uptake blocker TBOA (50 μm) on the extent of cross talk. To match the conditions of these experiments to those of the original experiments, we applied TBOA only during perfusion with MK801. NMDAR EPSCs in the SP were decreased to 14 ± 4% of baseline, whereas the CP responses were decreased to 8 ± 2% of baseline (n = 8) (Fig. 3). To test whether the more profound reduction was caused by the enhanced spontaneous MK801 blockade of NMDARs in the presence of TBOA rather than by enhanced spillover, we conducted the corresponding no-stimuli control experiments. After washout of TBOA plus MK801, EPSCs in these experiments were decreased to 64 ± 7% of baseline (Fig. 3C) (n = 6; same exposure to MK801 as in the preceding experiments). This was not significantly different from the effect of MK801 in the no-stimuli experiments without TBOA (72 ± 6%) (Fig. 2). We then further tested whether any long-term effects of TBOA alone (after washout) result in an increase in EPSCs. This could undermine the validity of the observed changes post-MK801. When TBOA alone was applied and, after 15 min, washed out again, NMDARs EPSCs showed a modest reduction (to 79 ± 8% of baseline; n = 6) rather than an increase. This indicated that, if anything, the discrepancy between CP and no-stimuli control could have under-estimated (by ∼20%) the extent of synaptic cross talk in the presence of TBOA.
Thus, either increasing the amount of glutamate released or inhibiting glutamate uptake led to an enhanced blockade of the NMDA receptors mediating the SP EPSCs.
MK801 increases the diffusional distance from release sites to available NMDARs in the conditioning pathway
Because the concentration of glutamate after release decreases sharply outside the synaptic cleft, MK801 should block a decreasing proportion of NMDARs with increasing distance from the release site. When the synapse is activated repeatedly in the presence of MK801, the average diffusional distance from the release site to the remaining NMDARs, and hence the latency of the EPSCs, should therefore increase progressively. This increase is not expected, however, at synapses where NMDARs are activated by glutamate spillover from remote synapses (Fig. 4A).
Is it biophysically plausible to detect such an effect? To assess this, we used a detailed 3D model of a “typical” CA1 synapse (Fig. 4B). First, we simulated the extracellular glutamate concentration transient at several distances from the release site, inside and outside the cleft (Fig. 4C) (see Materials and Methods for details). This transient was then used to estimate the open probability of NMDARs at the corresponding loci. These simulations predict (Fig. 4D) that the NMDAR EPSC latency (70% peak point) near the synaptic cleft edge in area CA1 should be 1.0–1.5 msec greater than that near the synaptic cleft center (the release site). At a distance of 200–250 nm, which is compatible with the half-distance between nearest neighbors in the CA1 neuropil (Rusakov and Kullmann, 1998), the latency increase should be 2.0–2.5 msec. These time intervals are well within the resolution of electrophysiological recordings. Indeed, an increase in the NMDAR EPSC rise time has been reported to occur after application of MK801 (Arnth-Jensen et al., 2002).
The 70% peak latency of CP EPSCs should thus increase after application and washout of MK801. In keeping with the prediction of an increased diffusion distance, we observed a latency increase of 3.18 ± 0.49 msec in the CP (p < 0.001; n = 10) (Fig. 4E, top bars). In these experiments, however, the SP EPSCs also showed a latency increase (1.13 ± 0.39 msec) (Fig. 4E, top bars), albeit three times smaller than the increase in the CP (p < 0.001; n = 10). This increase could not be readily explained by voltage-clamp escape because smaller responses are expected to be better clamped and therefore exhibit faster, rather than slower, rise times. Moreover, because the MK801 blockade should not affect the spatial pattern of release sites, dendritic filtering is also unlikely to affect the EPSC shape.
Another phenomenon that can affect the measurement of the early part of the NMDAR EPSC is incomplete washout of MK801. The EPSC decay time constant was markedly shortened by MK801, as expected from a shortening of the average NMDAR open time, and this effect was only partially reversed after washout, implying that MK801 was still present (Fig. 4E, traces). This is predicted to have the indirect effect of shortening the rise time of the synaptic conductance, because it should peak earlier than in the absence of MK801 (Jahr, 1992; Rosenmund et al., 1993); however, this effect is opposite to the observed change, which was an increase in the 70% peak latency (∼3 msec in the CP and ∼1 msec in the SP). Moreover, although the expected small shortening in the rise time of the conductance waveform could lead to an underestimate of the EPSC latency increase, it should affect both pathways equally (because it is a function not of cumulative blockade but of the acute effect of MK801 persisting at the end of the experiment). Thus, this phenomenon should not introduce any bias to the comparison between pathways.
An alternative phenomenon must therefore underlie the increase in latency in the SP. A possible explanation for the nonspecific latency increase is that the peak of the NMDAR EPSC was distorted by local shunting of the synaptic current through open NMDA receptors.
To address this further, we performed an equivalent circuit analysis of the voltage-clamp configuration representative of our experiments (Fig. 4F) (see Materials and Methods for derivations and parameter values; for the purposes of shunt analysis, ideal voltage clamp and no dendritic filtering were assumed). The analysis predicts that, given a characteristic synaptic conductance of 2–3nS(20–30 activated NMDARs at each synapse) and given the typical conditions of our experiments (50–100 activated synapses or 100–300 nS total conductance), a threefold decrease in NMDAR EPSCs corresponds to a ∼1 msec increase in its 70% peak latency (Fig. 4G). (In this range of amplitudes, the NMDAR EPSC amplitude error attributable to sublinear summation is <10–15%.) This change in latency is in excellent agreement with the experimental data for the nonspecific increase in latency in the SP.
The question arises, however, whether this shunting could then account for the differential latency increases in the two pathways simply because the EPSC amplitude decreased more in the CP than in the SP. To test for this possibility, we applied the NMDAR antagonist R(–)-3-(2-carboxy-piperazin-4-yl)-propyl-1-phosphonic acid (CPP) in a nonsaturating concentration (2.5–5 μm) that reduced the baseline responses to a similar extent as MK801 in the CP (17 ± 4%) (Fig. 4E, bottom graph). The latency increase in these responses was half that seen in the post-MK801 CP responses of similar size (1.48 ± 0.48 msec, n = 12; compared with 3.18 ± 0.49 msec, n = 10; p < 0.04). Thus, although local synaptic current shunting may explain part or all of the nonspecific latency increase seen in the SP, the additional latency increase observed in the CP can be attributed to an increased diffusional distance from the release site to the remaining unblocked receptors.
To further verify that the difference between the actions of MK801 and CPP was not caused by differential penetration of the drugs into the synaptic cleft, we also measured the latency of EPSCs after application of MK801 together with TBOA (see above). TBOA should increase the proportion of remote NMDARs activated by glutamate (Lozovaya et al., 1999; Diamond, 2001) and therefore subsequently blocked by MK801. Thus, when TBOA plus MK801 are washed out, a relatively higher proportion of intrasynaptic versus extrasynaptic receptors should be available to synaptically released glutamate. This implies a smaller increase in EPSC latency compared with the case of MK801 application without TBOA. In accordance with this prediction, the latency increase of the CP EPSCs was substantially smaller than that observed without application of TBOA (Fig. 4E, top bar graph) (1.13 ± 0.28 msec; n = 7; p < 0.001; latency increase in SP: 1.72 ± 0.59 msec; data not shown), whereas the EPSC amplitude was reduced to the same extent (Fig. 4E, bottom bar graph) (14 ± 4% of baseline). These data therefore support the hypothesis that the MK801 blockade increases the average distance between release sites and NMDARs at the directly activated (CP) but not spillover-activated (SP) synapses. On the basis of the predictions of modeling (Fig. 4D), the observed difference in NMDAR EPSC latency of 1.5–2 msec that cannot be accounted for by local shunting should correspond to a difference of 120–200 nm in the typical diffusion distance.
NR2A- and NR2B-containing NMDARs in intersynaptic signaling in CA1
In the cortex, functional NMDARs must contain either a NR2A or a NR2B receptor subunit (Kutsuwada et al., 1992). The expression of NR2A-containing receptors, which are thought to be almost exclusively intrasynaptic, increases sharply with synaptic maturation (Tovar and Westbrook, 1999), with the NR2A/NR2B ratio reaching its maximum at postnatal day 28 (Kew et al., 1998). Several studies have argued that extrasynaptic receptors tend to contain NR2B subunits, whereas synaptic receptors can contain either subtype (Stocca and Vicini, 1998; Momiyama, 2000; Steigerwald et al., 2000; Dalby and Mody, 2003; de Armentia and Sah, 2003). Although NR2B receptors have a two- to threefold higher sensitivity for glutamate compared with NR2A receptors (Kutsuwada et al., 1992), the progress of MK801 blockade in the presence of glutamate is indistinguishable between NR2A-containing, NR2B-containing, and the “endogenous mixture” of NMDARs expressed in cultured neurons (Prybylowski et al., 2002). In the conditions of the present MK801 experiments, therefore, the selective NR2B blocker ifenprodil (Williams et al., 1993) is a useful tool for dissecting the relative contribution of these receptor subtypes to intersynaptic cross talk.
Application of ifenprodil (10 μm) decreased the NMDAR EPSC amplitude to 58 ± 5% of baseline (p < 0.001; n = 16) (Fig. 5A). This decrease was in agreement with the effect of the selective NR2B antagonist Ro 25-6981 on NMDAR EPSCs in CA1 pyramidal cells reported recently in mice (Kawakami et al., 2003): decrease to 64 and 63% of baseline, respectively, in the left and right hemisphere. The decrease in EPSC amplitude produced by ifenprodil application was accompanied by a reduction in the monoexponential decay time constant from 181 ± 17 to 122 ± 13 msec (p < 0.002; n = 16) (Fig. 5B). This is consistent with the difference between decay time constants of NMDAR-mediated currents in NR2B- and NR2A-transfected cerebellar granule cells: 225 msec and 116 msec, respectively (Prybylowski et al., 2002).
We repeated the two-pathway cross-talk experiment in the continued presence of ifenprodil, with the same MK801 exposure protocol as in previous experiments. The progress of NMDAR EPSC blockade in the CP appeared slower compared with the experiments without ifenprodil (reduction to ∼40%, n = 6, compared with ∼22% of baseline, respectively). This was consistent with the substantially lower activation sensitivity of the remaining NR2A-containing NMDARs (Kutsuwada et al., 1992); however, we cannot fully exclude the possibility that NR2B-containing NMDARs are more sensitive to MK801. The similarity in MK801 sensitivity between the two isoforms reported in culture (Prybylowski et al., 2002) might not fully capture the behavior of the receptors in hippocampal slices. An additional contributing factor could be a direct consequence of synaptic cross talk. If the higher-affinity NR2B receptors sense glutamate released from more remote sites, these receptors will be activated in a higher number of trials (because the release probability at CA1 synapses is substantially lower than unity) compared with local NR2A receptors.
This gives rise to a paradox. Although NR2A-containing NMDARs show a lower cumulative rate of blockade by MK801 than do NR2B-containing receptors (Fig. 5), the diffusional distance increases in MK801 (Fig. 4), implying a higher occupancy of intrasynaptic versus extrasynaptic receptors. A likely resolution of this paradox is that a significant proportion of NR2B-containing receptors occur close to synapses. Such receptors should be blocked by MK801 more rapidly than either local NR2A-containing or remote NR2B-containing receptors because they are exposed to both local (high occupancy, low frequency) and spillover-mediated (low occupancy, high frequency) glutamate release. The rapid blockade of these receptors by MK801 therefore would increase the relative contribution of remotely activated NR2B NMDARs to the synaptic signal. The latter is consistent with the increased 70% peak latency of EPSCs (Fig. 4).
To allow the CP EPSCs to decrease to a level comparable with that seen in experiments without ifenprodil (15–20%), we increased the exposure to MK801 from 15 to 30 min. With the longer exposure to MK801, the residual EPSC amplitude in the SP was 53 ± 11% of baseline (n = 8) (Fig. 5C,E). Surprisingly, the NMDAR EPSCs underwent a similar average reduction in the corresponding no-stimuli experiments (52 ± 4% of baseline; n = 8; same exposure to MK801) (Fig. 5D,E). This implies that intersynaptic cross talk mediated by NMDARs was undetectable in the presence of 10 μm ifenprodil.
Because 10 μm ifenprodil potentially blocks a small fraction of NR2A receptors, we repeated these experiments in the presence of 3 μm ifenprodil. This data set again yielded no evidence for cross talk. NMDAR EPSCs in the SP and no-stimulus control were reduced to 38 ± 6% of baseline (post-MK801; n = 3) and 36 ± 3% of baseline (n = 6; data not shown), respectively. The greater reduction compared with that in 10 μm ifenprodil may reflect more rapid trial-to-trial progress of MK801 blockade under conditions in which fewer NR2B receptors are blocked.
In the presence of ifenprodil, only NR2A-containing NMDARs remain available to detect released glutamate. Because these receptors, unlike NR2B-containing NMDARs, appear to be restricted to synapses, one might also expect that there would be no specific latency increase after MK801 blockade in these conditions. The 70% peak latency of the post-MK801 EPSCs in the CP was increased only by 0.88 ± 0.30 msec relative to baseline (n = 6). In accordance with our predictions, this increase was actually smaller than the nonspecific increase of 1.13 ± 0.39 msec in similar experiments with CPP (although this difference fell short of statistical significance). Importantly, the latency increase was three times smaller than that without ifenprodil (3.18 ± 0.49; p < 0.005).
Because blocking glutamate uptake with TBOA substantially increased the extent of synaptic cross talk (Fig. 3), we tested whether this manipulation could rescue, at least partly, the cross talk abolished by ifenprodil. When TBOA was applied together with ifenprodil, the discrepancy between the average EPSC amplitude at the SP and at the no-stimuli control was indeed partly restored (18 ± 10 and 37 ± 7%, n = 7 and n = 8, respectively; p < 0.03; the CP EPSC amplitude was 15 ± 10%) (Fig. 5E).
Discussion
The extent of spillover in CA1
The main results of this study are, first, that a proportion of NMDARs in CA1 pyramidal cells recorded at a near-physiological temperature mediate excitatory signaling from more than one synapse and, second, that such receptors mainly, if not exclusively, contain the NR2B subunit.
These results allow a quantitative estimate for the extent of spillover. In the no-stimuli experiments the NMDAR EPSCs were reduced to 71% of the baseline, whereas this reduction was to 44% of baseline in the SP (Fig. 2C). Spillover can therefore account for an MK801-dependent blockade of 100 · (1–44/71) ∼ 38% of the SP NMDARs. This reduction occurred after n = 25–55 stimuli to the CP. Because low-frequency stimulation is likely to involve principally synapses with a high release probability (Rosenmund et al., 1993), the proportion p of NMDARs blocked by MK801 (in either pathway) per stimulus should not depend on n. Thus, the baseline-normalized amplitude of NMDAR EPSCs after the first stimulus will be A(1) = (1 – p) and the amplitude after the nth stimulus will be A(n) = (1 – p)n. Allowing indexes c and s for the CP and SP, respectively, one then obtains: (4) Given Ac = 0.3 (22 over 71%) and As = 0.62 (44 over 71%), the formula yields a ratio close to 3. In other words, there is an ∼30% chance that glutamate released from one pathway will successfully activate NMDARs in the other pathway. When groups of five stimuli were applied in our experiments, the value of pc/ps became close to 2.0 (Ac = 12/71 = 0.17; As = 24/71 = 0.39; total stimulus number n ∼ 120), indicating an increased probability (50% chance) of intersynaptic cross talk in CA1. In fact, because low-probability synapses may be recruited in this experiment, 50% is likely to be an underestimate. In TBOA (Fig. 3), cross talk was even more probable: pc/ps was close to 1.35 (Ac = 8/64 = 0.125; As = 14/64 = 0.219; stimulus number n ∼ 40).
Sparseness of activated synapses and glutamate spillover
Our results therefore predict that, on average, 25–35% of NMDARs in CA1 pyramidal cells sense glutamate released at more than one synapse, with the stimulus intensities and frequencies used here. Is this estimate biophysically plausible? To address this, it is necessary to estimate the density of activated synapses. Before applying CNQX to block AMPA receptors, we observed evoked EPSCs in the range of Aevok = 0.5–1 nA and spontaneous EPSCs in the range of Aspon = 10–15 pA. Given that blocking action potentials with TTX has little effect on the amplitude of spontaneous EPSCs in CA1 pyramidal neurons (Manabe et al., 1992), this implies that a single stimulus activates Aevok/Aspon = 50–100 synapses per cell. In area CA1, apical dendrites of pyramidal cells receive 5,000–10,000 synaptic inputs (Trommald et al., 1995), implying that up to 2% of excitatory synapses were activated. Because the volume density of all excitatory synapses in CA1 is ∼2 μm–3 (Rusakov and Kullmann, 1998), the density of activated synapses will be NV ∼ 0.04 μm–3. This corresponds to a nearest-neighbor distance among activated synapses of 0.554 NV–1/3 = 1.62 μm (Rusakov and Kullmann, 1998).
Is this synaptic density consistent with the 25–35% chance of NMDARs being “shared” among synapses? To address this, we performed simulation experiments that would mimic the experimental protocol. We generated a 3D scatter of synapses in a 5-μm-wide cube of neuropil (Fig. 6A, schematic). The size, spatial density, and typical geometry of synaptic environment matched the experimental estimates obtained using electron microscopy (Rusakov and Kullmann, 1998; Lehre and Rusakov, 2002). A subset of activated synapses was sampled randomly (Fig. 6A, red and yellow spheres), and 5000 glutamate molecules (see Materials and Methods) were released synchronously at each of the selected synapses. The resulting spatial profile of glutamate at a given time point could then be visualized using a “probing plane” that crossed the cube (Fig. 6A,B). Conversely, the glutamate concentration transient and the corresponding NMDAR opening time course [estimated from the kinetic scheme of Lester and Jahr (1992)] could be simulated at a given location within the cube. To mimic the two-pathway experiments, we collected such recordings at an arbitrary point, whereas the subset of active synapses was randomly resampled at each trial (see Materials and Methods). Figure 6, C and D, depicts a sample (n = 20) of glutamate transients and the corresponding NMDAR activation curves, respectively, generated by the 5% subsets of synapses. In such simulations, the ith trial would correspond to an NMDAR maximum opening probability Pio,max (i = 1,...,20) in response to glutamate release from the ith subset of synapses. The probability that the same NMDARs will be activated by both of any two independent synaptic subsets could then be calculated as the product Pij = Pio,maxPjo,max (i ≠ j), with indices i and j representing two independent pathways. Figure 6E shows the results of such simulations depicting the expected average 〈Pij〉 plotted against the proportion of activated synapses.
These data suggest that ∼30% of NMDARs will be shared among independent synaptic subsets when 7–8% of all local synapses are activated. This compares with the estimate, obtained from the electrophysiological data, that only 2% of synapses were active (see above). The latter estimate, however, reflects the average value throughout the neuropil, whereas in the acute slice functional synapses are likely to be distributed unevenly (for instance because some dendrites were cut during slicing), thus giving higher local densities. The predictions of the modeling are thus in reasonable agreement with the electrophysiology.
The role of NR2B- and NR2A-containing receptors in spillover
Our data predict that blocking the NR2B-containing NMDARs with ifenprodil effectively abolishes activation of NMDARs by escaping glutamate. The lower sensitivity of the NR2A-containing NMDARs to glutamate (Kutsuwada et al., 1992) provides one possible explanation; however, this could still give rise to a paradox. Because there are many inactive synapses (∼95% of the total) scattered between active ones, some NR2A-containing receptors could be in relatively close proximity to release sites. One major distinction between the two receptor subunits, however, is that the NR2A-containing NMDARs are thought to occur close to the release site, perhaps exclusively within the synaptic cleft (Tovar and Westbrook, 1999; Momiyama, 2000; Steigerwald et al., 2000; Dalby and Mody, 2003; de Armentia and Sah, 2003). It is conceivable therefore that these receptors are surrounded by high-affinity receptors or transporters, or both, expressed in the adjacent neuronal or glial membranes. [The function of postsynaptic transporters was probably compromised by holding the cell at a positive voltage, arguing instead for a role of presynaptic transporters (Danbolt, 2001; Mathews and Diamond, 2003).] In this case, the intracleft NR2A-containing receptors would be protected from low-concentration waves of glutamate spilling in (Diamond, 2001). In agreement with this hypothesis, blocking glutamate uptake with TBOA partly rescued the discrepancy between the CP and no-stimuli control (Fig. 5E), thus removing, at least in part, the barrier for intersynaptic cross talk mediated by NR2A-containing NMDARs.
Finally, the finding that synaptic cross talk depends on NR2B receptors argues against major contribution of glutamate released from glia (Araque et al., 1999) to the observed spillover phenomena. A substantial proportion of synaptic clefts (and therefore NR2A-containing NMDARs) in CA1 are in the immediate proximity of glial protrusions (Ventura and Harris, 1999).
Functional implications of intersynaptic cross talk
The conclusion that receptors activated by transmitter release at one synapse can also be activated by another synapse implies a significant loss of information capacity in the network; however, this issue is not straightforward. First, low-affinity AMPARs can still mediate fast, point-to-point synaptic transmission without degradation of spatiotemporal “bandwidth.” Second, even among high-affinity NMDARs, the present study implies that it is NR2B-containing receptors that are shared among multiple afferent inputs. This is in line with the notion that the extrasynaptic NMDARs, which are supposed to contain the NR2B subunit, are distributed over a 100-fold larger area (although at a much lower concentration) (Takumi et al., 1999; Racca et al., 2000) of cell membranes compared with the synaptic cleft (Rusakov et al., 1998).
Interestingly, NR2A- and NR2B-containing NMDARs have been linked to different intracellular cascades (Steigerwald et al., 2000; Zheng et al., 2001; Krapivinsky et al., 2003), also leading to preferential triggering of long-term potentiation and long-term depression, respectively (Lu et al., 2001; Hardingham et al., 2002). Thus, the results of the present study suggest a possible novel role for NR2B receptors. Because they detect glutamate released from multiple sources, they may exist primarily to monitor the overall level of activity in the network (Dalby and Mody, 2003) and thus to regulate the density and strength of glutamatergic synapses (Turrigiano, 1999). Indeed, the present data suggest a simple scenario in which a proportion of NR2B-containing NMDARs are activated by glutamate spilled over from synapses on neighboring cells. In contrast, NR2A-containing NMDARs do not appear to share such spillover signals, thus suggesting a distinction between “global” and “local” signaling sensed by distinct subpopulations of NMDARs.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust, and the European Commission (DECG QLG3-CT-2001-02004). We are grateful to Drs. L. Trussell, T. V. P. Bliss, N. J. Emptage, D. Attwell, C. Reid, and M. Walker for helpful comments.
Correspondence should be addressed to D. A. Rusakov, Institute of Neurology, University College London, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK. E-mail: d.rusakov{at}ion.ucl.ac.uk.
DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0364-04.2004
Copyright © 2004 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/04/244767-11$15.00/0