Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Corticotropin-Releasing Factor Receptors CRF1 and CRF2 Exert Both Additive and Opposing Influences on Defensive Startle Behavior

Victoria B. Risbrough, Richard L. Hauger, Amanda L. Roberts, Wylie W. Vale and Mark A. Geyer
Journal of Neuroscience 21 July 2004, 24 (29) 6545-6552; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5760-03.2004
Victoria B. Risbrough
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Richard L. Hauger
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Amanda L. Roberts
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Wylie W. Vale
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mark A. Geyer
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) receptors (CRF1 and CRF2) are crucial mediators of physiological and behavioral responses to stress. In animals, CRF1 appears to primarily mediate CRF-induced anxiety-like responses, but the role of CRF2 during stress is still unclear. Here we report the effects of CRF1 and CRF2 on the magnitude and plasticity of defensive startle responses in mice. Startle plasticity is measured by inhibition of startle by sensory stimuli, i.e., prepulse inhibition (PPI), and is disrupted in patients with panic or posttraumatic stress disorders in which CRF neurotransmission may be overactive. Pharmacological blockade of CRF1 reversed both CRF-induced increases in startle and CRF-induced deficits in PPI. CRF2 blockade attenuated high-dose but not low-dose CRF-induced increases in startle and reduced PPI. Conversely, activation of CRF2 enhanced PPI. CRF had no effect on startle and increased PPI in CRF1 knock-out mice. These data indicate that CRF receptors act in concert to increase the magnitude of defensive startle yet in opposition to regulate the flexibility of startle. These data support a new model of respective CRF receptor roles in stress-related behavior such that, although both receptors enhance the magnitude of defensive responses, CRF1 receptors contravene, whereas CRF2 receptors enhance, the impact of sensory information on defensive behavior. We hypothesize that excessive CRF1 activation combined with reduced CRF2 signaling may contribute to information processing deficits seen in panic and posttraumatic stress disorder patients and support CRF1-specific pharmacotherapy.

  • CRH
  • antisauvagine-30
  • NBI-30775
  • R121919
  • urocortin
  • anxiety

Introduction

The neuropeptide corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mediates many neuroendocrine and behavioral responses to stress (Vale et al., 1981; Rivier et al., 1983; Koob and Heinrichs, 1999). CRF acts in the brain at two distinct G-protein-coupled receptors, CRF1 and CRF2 (Perrin and Vale, 1999; Dautzenberg and Hauger, 2002). Although both pharmacological and CRF1 knock-out (KO) mouse studies clearly support the anxiogenic effects of CRF1 activation, the influences of CRF2 activation on anxiety are less clear (Koob and Heinrichs, 1999; Bakshi and Kalin, 2000; Coste et al., 2001; Reul and Holsboer, 2002). Some evidence indicates that anxiety behaviors mediated by CRF1 activation are reduced when CRF2 is activated (Bale et al., 2000; Kishimoto et al., 2000; Coste et al., 2001; cf. Reul and Holsboer, 2002; Valdez et al., 2002). Nevertheless, other evidence indicates that CRF2 is also capable of inducing anxiety-like behavior (Radulovic et al., 1999; Takahashi et al., 2001; Pelleymounter et al., 2002) and increasing certain defensive behaviors, such as conditioned freezing (Bakshi et al., 2002) and the startle reflex (Risbrough et al., 2003).

The startle response consists of a series of involuntary reflexes elicited by a sudden, intense auditory stimulus and is considered to be a defensive behavior evolved to protect the body from impact during attack (Graham, 1975; Yeomans et al., 2002). The magnitude of the response is highly plastic: fear-inducing stimuli or CRF administration increase startle (Brown et al., 1951; Swerdlow et al., 1986; Davis et al., 1997), whereas threat-reducing stimuli (Lang et al., 1990) or sensory input in the case of prepulse inhibition (PPI) (Graham, 1975) reduce startle. Across species, presentation of a neutral, nonstartling acoustic “prepulse” 30-300 msec before the startling stimulus reduces startle magnitude, possibly by requiring the organism to allocate attentional resources to process the prepulse and hence filter or “gate” the subsequent startling stimulus (Graham, 1975; Hoffman and Ison, 1980; Norris and Blumenthal, 1996; Swerdlow et al., 1999). Clinically, PPI is used as an experimental measure of sensory and cognitive information-processing mechanisms that are deficient in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders (Braff et al. 2001).

Recently, patients suffering from panic disorder or posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) have been shown to have disruptions in PPI (Grillon et al., 1996, 1998; Ludewig et al., 2002). Brain CRF hypersecretion and CRF1 sensitization are hypothesized to contribute to some anxiety disorders (Dautzenberg and Hauger, 2002; Reul and Holsboer, 2002; Musselman and Nemeroff, 2003). Endogenous overexpression of CRF in transgenic mice or acute administration of CRF in rats reduces PPI (Conti et al., 2002; Dirks et al., 2002, 2003), although the mechanism whereby CRF receptor signaling modulates startle plasticity remains unknown. Understanding the mechanism of CRF-induced decreases in PPI may have important implications for describing specific brain system pathologies in anxiety disorder patients that exhibit disruptions in PPI. Hence, we examined the respective influences of CRF1 and CRF2 on the startle response and its sensitivity to inhibition by sensory information using both pharmacological techniques and CRF1 KO mice.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Male C57BL/6J mice from The Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME) and male 129S6/SvEvTac mice from Taconic (Germantown, NY) aged 6-8 weeks on arrival were housed four per cage in a temperature-controlled (21-22°C) room under a reverse 12 hr light/dark cycle (lights off at 8:00 A.M.). The reverse light/dark cycle was used to minimize interactions with the stress of disruptions in diurnal cycles associated with testing during the sleep phase. Initial studies [antisauvagine-30 (ASV30) and urocortin 2] were performed in C57BL/6J mice (Risbrough et al., 2003). Subsequent studies (NBI-30775 and urocortin 3) used 129SvEv mice because these mice exhibit more robust PPI performance, which allows for an optimal baseline performance for detection of both disruptions and improvements in PPI performance (Crawley et al., 1997). Heterozygously bred CRF1 receptor wild-type (WT) and KO mice (mixed C57BL/6J × 129SvEv background) (Smith et al. 1998) aged 4-5 months were housed similarly, except they were housed one per cage after surgery. All testing occurred from 10:00 A.M. to 6:00 P.M. and was conducted in accordance with the Principles of Laboratory Animal Care, National Institutes of Health guidelines, as approved by the University of California, San Diego, Veterans Affairs Medical Center, and Scripps Research Institute Animal Care Committees.

Apparatus

Startle chambers (SR-LAB; San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA) consisted of nonrestrictive Plexiglas cylinders 5 cm in diameter resting on a Plexiglas platform in a ventilated chamber. High-frequency speakers mounted 33 cm above the cylinders produced all acoustic stimuli, which were controlled by SR-LAB software. Piezoelectric accelerometers mounted under the cylinders transduced movements of the animal, which were digitized and stored by an interface and computer assembly. Beginning at startling stimulus onset, 65 consecutive 1 msec readings were recorded to obtain the peak amplitude of the animal's startle response. A dynamic calibration system was used to ensure comparable sensitivities across chambers. Sound levels were measured as described previously using the A weighting scale in units of decibels sound pressure level (Geyer and Dulawa, 2003). The house light remained off throughout all testing sessions.

Drugs

Peptide infusions. In all experiments other than with CRF1 receptor WT and KO mice, peptides were injected intracerebroventricularly using a free-hand method in lightly anesthetized mice as described previously (Pelleymounter et al., 2002; Risbrough et al., 2003). The injection volume was 5 μl. Pilot studies using dye injections instead of drug indicate that this method has ∼95% accuracy for the ventricles, with dye (2 μl injected) being found in lateral, third, and fourth ventricles. Any mouse with either (1) a cerebral hematoma or (2) an injection site >0.5 mm away from bregma and behavioral performance 2 SD from the group mean was removed from analysis (7 of 162 total mice injected).

Free-hand intracerebroventricular injections were not possible in CRF1 WT and KO mice because of their age and the thickness of their cranium. Hence, WT and KO mice were anesthetized using a 90 mg/kg ketamine-10 mg/kg xylazine mixture and prepared with a 23 gauge 7-mm-length unilateral guide cannula in the lateral ventricle (flat skull; anteroposterior, -0.1 mm; mediolateral, -1.0 mm; dorsoventral, -1.5 mm below dura). Cannulas were secured with one skull screw and dental cement. Drug injections and histologies were as described previously (Spina et al., 2000). In brief, injections were made 5-7 d after surgery in unanesthetized mice using a 30 gauge 8 mm injector (1 mm below the tip of the guide cannulas). Injection volume was 5 μl using gravity flow. Two weeks after testing, mice were anesthetized and 1 μl of dye was injected via the 8 mm injector using gravity flow. Mice were immediately killed, and the brains were removed. As the brains were removed, presence of the dye in the fourth ventricle was noted. A coronal cut was made along the guide tract to reveal lateral and third ventricles, which were also noted for presence of dye, and brains were frozen and stored with cut side on slides for digital scanning. Only animals with verification of dye in all four ventricles were included in the analysis (30 of 30 mice implanted).

Experiment 1, ASV30 versus human/rat-CRF. Human/rat (h/r)-CRF at 0.06 and 0.6 nmol (0.2 nmol = 1 μg of peptide; Bachem, Torrance, CA) and 3 nmol ASV30 (a gift from Neurocrine Biosciences, San Diego, CA) were diluted in water (Pelleymounter et al., 2002; Risbrough et al., 2003) and coadministered or given alone 1 hr before testing.

Experiment 2, NBI-30775 versus h/r-CRF. The selective CRF1 receptor antagonist NBI-30775 (also known as R-121919; a gift from Neurocrine Biosciences) was diluted in 5% cremophor-sterile water and a 20 mg/kg dose was administered intraperitoneally both 10 min before and 60 min after h/r-CRF or artificial CSF (aCSF) administration (i.e., 110 and 60 min before test). This double-injection method was used to ensure a lasting effect of NBI-30775 over the 2 hr pretest and testing period.

Experiment 3, h/r-CRF in CRF1 WT and KO mice. WT and KO CRF1 mice received aCSF vehicle or 0.2 nmol of h/r-CRF and were tested 1 hr after injection.

Experiment 4, urocortin 2 and urocortin 3. Human urocortin 2-stresscopin-related peptide (2 and 6 nmol; Bachem) was dissolved in aCSF vehicle and administered 1 hr before the first test session. The mice were tested 1, 2, 3, and 4 hr after administration. This time course was chosen because some reports indicate that urocortin 2 can have effects up to 4 hr after administration (Valdez et al., 2002). Mouse urocortin 3 (0.8 and 2.4 nmol; Bachem) was dissolved in aCSF vehicle. The mice were tested 2, 4, and 6 hr after administration, because the urocortin 2 data indicated that a longer time window may be needed to detect time-dependent effects of CRF2 receptor activation on PPI (see below).

Behavioral testing

For all acoustic startle sessions, the intertrial interval between stimulus presentations averaged 15 sec (range of 7-23 sec). A 65 dB background was presented continuously throughout the session. After placement into the startle chambers, a 5 min acclimation period preceded testing. Four to 10 d before drug testing, mice were tested briefly to measure baseline acoustic startle response and PPI. The mice were then assigned to drug groups (i.e., counterbalanced) so that all drug groups averaged similar startle response levels and PPI performance before drug testing. Startle pulses were 40 msec in duration, prepulses were 20 msec in duration, and prepulses preceded the pulse by 100 msec (onset-onset). In all prepulse trials, the interstimulus interval was 100 msec from onset of the prepulse to onset of the pulse. Testing parameters were varied slightly between each separate experiment to customize the parameters to keep baseline PPI performance consistent across different cohorts and strains (between 40 and 55% PPI on average). Thus, experiments in which the mice exhibited relative poor pretest performance, such as C57BL/6J mice, were presented some trials with louder prepulse stimuli and reduced startle stimuli than mice with relatively better pretest performance, such as 129SvEv mice (details below). Thus, in experiments using C57BL/6J (ASV30 vs h/r-CRF, urocortin 2) and CRF1 WT and KO mice, acoustic startle sessions included two blocks. The first block tested acoustic startle response only and included three different acoustic stimulus intensities: 90, 105, and 120 dB. The data from this block in C57BL/6J mice were presented previously (Risbrough et al., 2003). The second block (data presented here) consisted of six each of 105 or 120 dB startle pulse intensities and five each of four different prepulse trials (73 and 81 dB preceding either a 105 or 120 dB pulse). In experiments in 129SvEv mice, the test sessions consisted of nine 120 dB startle pulses and six each of 69, 73, and 81 dB (urocortin 3) prepulse trials or 10 120 dB startle pulses and eight each of 73 and 81 dB (NBI-30775) prepulse trials.

Data analysis

In all experiments, the average startle magnitude over the record window (i.e., 65 msec) was used for all data analysis. Two mice were removed because of startle responses of <10 units during the 120 dB trials, which most likely indicates hearing loss. Either a two-way ANOVA with treatment (h/r-CRF) and pretreatment (ASV30 or NBI-30775) or genotype (WT or KO) or a one-way ANOVA with treatment (urocortin 2 or urocortin 3) as the between-subject factor, and prepulse and pulse intensity as within-subject factors, were used. Post hoc analysis followed significant main or interaction effects as appropriate. Unless specified otherwise, PPI data presented were collapsed across prepulse intensities, and startle reactivity data are shown at the 120 dB startle intensity (for detailed effects of CRF receptors on startle reactivity, see Risbrough et al., 2003).

Results

Experiment 1, ASV30 versus h/r-CRF: CRF2 antagonism reduces PPI and decreases startle

Administration of h/r-CRF resulted in an inverted U-shaped dose-effect curve, with the low dose (0.06 nmol) of h/r-CRF decreasing PPI (p < 0.05; Dunnett's test), whereas a 10 times higher dose (0.6 nmol) had no effect on PPI (Fig. 1a) (h/r-CRF, F(2,53) = 7.65; p < 0.01). The CRF2 antagonist ASV30 reduced PPI when coadministered with the high 0.6 nmol dose of h/r-CRF (p < 0.05; Tukey's test) but had no effect when administered either alone or with the low dose of h/r-CRF (Fig. 1a) (ASV30 × h/r-CRF, F(2,53) = 4.13; p < 0.05). In contrast to the effects on PPI, h/r-CRF dose dependently (p < 0.01; Tukey's test) increased acoustic startle magnitude (Fig. 1b) (h/r-CRF, F(2,53) = 19.96; p < 0.001). Despite having no effects when given alone, ASV30 attenuated the increases in startle from the high but not low dose of h/r-CRF (Fig. 1b) (ASV30 × h/r-CRF, F(2,53) = 5.53; p < 0.01).

  Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Effect of CRF on startle and prepulse inhibition during CRF1 or CRF2 receptor blockade. a, b, Vehicle (VEH), 0.06 or 0.6 nmol of h/r-CRF, and vehicle or 3 nmol of ASV30 were coadministered (5 μl, i.c.v.) in C57BL/6J mice 1 hr before prepulse inhibition (a) and startle (b) testing. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 versus vehicle/vehicle group; ##p < 0.01 versus 0.6 nmol of h/r-CRF/vehicle group. c, d, Vehicle or 20 mg/kg (intraperitoneally) NBI-30775 (NBI) was administered 10 min before vehicle and 60 min after vehicle or 0.2 nmol of h/r-CRF (5 μl, i.c.v.) in 129SvEv mice. Prepulse inhibition (c) and startle (d) were tested 2 hr after h/r-CRF injection. *p < 0.05 versus vehicle/vehicle group; #p < 0.05 versus 0.2 nmol of h/r-CRF/vehicle group; +p < 0.05 main effect of NBI-30775 and main effect of h/r-CRF. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM.

The observation that the maximal effect of h/r-CRF on startle did not correspond with any effect on PPI indicates that the effects of CRF on startle and PPI are independent, thus confirming previous reports of CRF effects on startle plasticity (Conti et al., 2002; Dirks et al., 2002, 2003). To further evaluate the independence of the startle and PPI effects, we compared PPI across prepulse trials with different startle pulse intensities, thus more closely matching baseline startle values across prepulse performance (Brody et al., 2004). Comparisons of the PPI trials using a low-intensity startle stimulus (105 dB) in the 0.06 h/r-CRF group to PPI trials with a higher startle intensity stimuli (120 dB) in the vehicle group further confirmed the significant reduction in PPI at the 0.06 h/r-CRF dose group, even when the CRF group had a lower startle baseline relative to the vehicle group (t19 = 1.8; p < 0.05; vehicle, percentage PPI of 29 ± 5, 120 dB startle of 172 ± 21; 0.06 h/r-CRF, percentage PPI of 12 ± 9, 105 dB startle of 109 ± 32).

In vitro saturation binding studies indicate that 125I-ASV30 binds to <0.1% of human CRF1 recombinantly expressed in HEK cells (Higelin et al., 2001). In competitive binding experiments, ASV30 exhibits a 340- to 500-fold greater selectivity for CRF2 over CRF1 (Brauns et al., 2001; Dautzenberg et al., 2001). In vivo effects (some exerted by similar doses to the dose of ASV30 used in our experiments) are in sharp contrast to and/or do not interfere with CRF1-mediated effects (Pelleymounter et al., 2002; Rivier et al., 2002; Hammack et al., 2003; Lu et al., 2003; Overstreet et al., 2004). Thus, current in vitro and in vivo data support the selectivity of ASV30 at the doses used for CRF2 and not CRF1 activity. In vitro studies indicate that h/r-CRF binds at CRF2 with a Ki value that is 4- to 20-fold greater than the Ki value at CRF1 (Dautzenberg et al., 1999; Perrin and Vale, 1999). Accordingly, the most parsimonious explanation for the present data are that the low h/r-CRF dose did not activate an observable CRF2-mediated behavioral response, because ASV30 pretreatment did not alter the effects of this dose on PPI or startle. In contrast, ASV30 pretreatment prevented the effects of the high dose of h/r-CRF on PPI and startle. Thus, blockade of CRF2 in the presence of high doses of agonist that no longer affect PPI may unmask a CRF1 effect similar to that seen at lower CRF doses at which PPI deficits are observed. When tested with similar parameters in 129SvEv mice, which exhibit higher PPI performance (and are thus less likely to exhibit floor effects of CRF on PPI), ASV30 significantly potentiated 0.2 nmol h/r-CRF-induced deficits in PPI (percentage PPI: vehicle/vehicle, 72 ± 5; vehicle/CRF, 57 ± 5; ASV30/vehicle, 82 ± 5; ASV30/CRF, 32 ± 7; p < 0.01, vehicle/CRF vs ASV30/CRF). These data led us to hypothesize that the CRF1 receptor is likely to mediate the low-dose CRF-induced deficits in PPI, although CRF2 receptor activation may increase PPI. Accordingly, to test these hypotheses, we asked whether CRF1 receptor blockade would attenuate or block CRF-induced decreases in PPI. To answer this question, we selected the 129SvEv strain of mouse, because their baseline PPI performance is more robust and consistent across testing (Crawley et al., 1997). We decided to use the 0.2 nmol dose in all additional experiments, because this appeared to be a reliable dose in both strains for startle effects (Risbrough et al., 2003) that would likely activate both CRF1 and CRF2.

Experiment 2, NBI-30775 versus h/r-CRF: CRF1 antagonism increases PPI and decreases startle

Because these findings indicated that activation of CRF2 increased PPI, we hypothesized that CRF1 activation was responsible for the CRF-induced deficits in PPI and predicted that a CRF1 antagonist would block CRF-induced decreases in PPI. As predicted, h/r-CRF decreased and NBI-30775 increased PPI (Fig. 1c) (h/r-CRF, F(1,40) = 19.07, p < 0.001; NBI-30775, F(1,40) = 11.17, p < 0.01). The main effect of NBI-30775 appeared to be attributable to NBI-30775 significantly increasing PPI in the h/r-CRF-treated group (Fig. 1c) (p < 0.05 h/r-CRF/vehicle vs h/r-CRF/NBI-30775; a priori Dunn's test). A similar test in C57BL6/J mice, although not statistically significant (possibly because of a smaller sample size), confirms that NBI-30775 attenuates the PPI disruption induced by 0.2 nmol h/r-CRF (percentage PPI: vehicle/vehicle, 38 ± 3; vehicle/CRF, 22 ± 5; NBI-30775/vehicle, 43 ± 5; NBI-30775/CRF, 34 ± 4). Startle reactivity was significantly increased after h/r-CRF treatment (Fig. 1d) (h/r-CRF, F(1,40) = 7.35; p < 0.01), albeit more weakly than in experiment 1 (Fig. 1b), possibly attributable to the difference in pretest administration times chosen (experiment 1, 1 hr before test; experiment 2, 2 hr before test) (Risbrough et al. 2003). NBI-30775 treatment had the opposite effect, significantly reducing startle reactivity (Fig. 1d) (NBI-30775, F(1,40) = 6.01; p < 0.05), confirming the effects of NBI-30775 on CRF-induced increases in startle in C57BL/6J mice (Risbrough et al., 2003). To further confirm the hypothesis that CRF1 activation reduces PPI, we tested whether the effect of CRF on startle and PPI would be absent in CRF1 gene mutation (CRF1 KO) mice.

Experiment 3, CRF increases PPI but not startle in CRF1 KO mice

CRF treatment (0.2 nmol) reduced PPI in WT mice (Fig. 2a), as seen previously. Strikingly, however, h/r-CRF treatment significantly increased PPI in the CRF1 KO mice (Fig. 2a) (p < 0.05; Dunn's test) (h/r-CRF × gene, F(1,24) = 17.99; p < 0.001). Thus, relative to its effect in WT mice, CRF had the opposite effect on PPI when CRF1 had been genetically deleted. CRF also increased startle reactivity in the WT mice, and this effect was completely absent in the KO mice (Fig. 2b) (Dunn's test) (h/r-CRF × gene, F(1,24) = 7.86; p < 0.01). In conjunction with the CRF2 antagonist data, these data indicate that activation of CRF2 increases startle inhibition by sensory input. The occurrence of increased PPI in CRF1 KOs injected with CRF compared with WT mice could, however, result in part from compensatory changes in brain CRF and urocortin systems. For example, increased expression of CRF in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus has been observed in CRF1 KO mice (Smith et al., 1998). To confirm that CRF2 activation increases PPI in the normal system, we activated CRF2 acutely in normal mice using the recently discovered peptides urocortin 2 and 3 that act preferentially at CRF2 receptors.

  Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Effect of CRF on prepulse inhibition of the startle response and startle reactivity in CRF1 gene deletion mice. CRF1 WT and KO mice received vehicle or 0.2 nmol of h/r-CRF (5 μl, i.c.v.) 1 hr before prepulse inhibition (a) and startle (b) testing. *p < 0.05 versus respective vehicle group. #p < 0.05 versus h/r-CRF WT group. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM.

Experiment 4, urocortin 2 and 3: CRF2 agonism increases startle and PPI

Both urocortin 2 and 3 produced long-lasting effects on PPI, inducing significant increases in PPI across all time points and prepulse trials tested (Fig. 3) (data collapsed across time; urocortin 2, F(2,28) = 3.84, p < 0.05; urocortin 3, F(2,28) = 3.79, p < 0.05). These data confirm other reports of long-acting effects of CRF2 activation on behavior (Valdez et al., 2002) and also confirm that CRF2 activation increases PPI in both 129SvEv and C57BL/6J strains, although the baseline performance is markedly different across the two strains. Together, these data indicate the robustness of CRF2-mediated effects across behavioral baseline, strain, and agonist used. Dunnett's post hoc analysis revealed that the 6 nmol dose of urocortin 2 and the 0.8 and 2.4 nmol doses of urocortin 3 significantly increased PPI relative to vehicle (Fig. 3). As shown in Table 1, the 2 nmol dose of urocortin 2 increased startle reactivity up to the second hour after injection (p < 0.05; Dunnett's test) (urocortin 2, F(2,28) = 4.64, p < 0.05; time × urocortin 2, F(6,84) = 3.02, p < 0.01). These data again confirm the dissociation between startle and PPI effects of CRF receptor activation, because the effect of urocortin 2 on PPI lasted at least 4 hr, whereas effects on startle were gone by the second hour after administration. Unlike urocortin 2 however, urocortin 3 did not affect startle responding at any of the doses or time points tested (Table 2).

  Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Effect of selective CRF2 activation on prepulse inhibition of the startle response. a, Vehicle or urocortin 2 (2 and 6 nmol) was administered (5 μl, i.c.v.) in C57BL/6J mice. Data are collapsed across tests at 1, 2, 3, and 4 hr after administration. b, Vehicle or urocortin 3 (0.8 and 2.4 nmol) was administered (5 μl, i.c.v.) in 129SvEv mice. Data are collapsed across tests at 2, 4, and 6 hr after administration. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 versus vehicle. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM of 83 dB prepulse trials.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Effect of intracerebroventricular urocortin 2 on acoustic startle responding to 120 dB pulse in C57BL/6J mice

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Effect of intracerebroventricular urocortin 3 on acoustic startle responding to 120 dB pulse in 129SvEv mice

Discussion

The present studies demonstrate that CRF1 activation increases startle reactivity and reduces PPI. In contrast, CRF2 activation increases startle reactivity (although only in combination with CRF1 activation) but simultaneously enhances PPI. Hence, the extant hypotheses of additive or opposing functions of CRF receptors on behavior are not necessarily contradictory. Rather, we suggest a model in which the two CRF receptors exert complementary influences on the magnitude of defensive behaviors and opposing influences on the flexibility of the behavior in response to sensory input. Perhaps the most novel of the present findings is that CRF1 and CRF2 have opposing influences on information-processing mechanisms that regulate responses to stressors, findings that may have important implications for clinical anxiety and stress-induced psychiatric disorders.

The present data show that selective blockade of CRF1 via NBI-30775 reverses CRF-induced deficits in PPI. In contrast, selective CRF2 antagonism via ASV30 appears to potentiate CRF-induced deficits in PPI. Conversely, selective activation (via urocortin 2 or 3 or in CRF1 KO mice) of CRF2 increased PPI. Thus, both the pharmacological and genetic manipulations in the present study give strong evidence for the opposing influences of CRF1 and CRF2 on startle inhibition. It also seems clear that CRF1 is the obligatory receptor for CRF effects on startle, because CRF1 KO mice exhibited no effects of CRF on startle nor did the selective CRF2 agonist urocortin 3. Although in vitro recombinant studies indicate similar selectivity, urocortin 2 may be less selective than urocortin 3 in vivo (Lewis et al., 2001; Valdez et al., 2003). For example, in rat pituitary cells endogenously expressing CRF1, urocortin 2 was shown to increase cAMP at very high concentrations (Lewis et al., 2001). Thus, it is possible that the increase in startle reactivity by urocortin 2 may reflect a combination of strong CRF2 activation with weak CRF1 activation. ASV30 reduced the effects of high doses of CRF on startle but did not alter the effects of low doses of CRF on startle. This pattern of effects does not seem to support the possibility that ASV30 effects on startle are attributable to weak CRF1 antagonism alone, because that should be expected to occur at low doses of competitive agonist, not just at the high dose of agonist. Thus, together, these data indicate that CRF1 is obligatory for CRF effects on startle, and CRF2 appears to have an additive influence only in the presence of CRF1 activation.

The defensive nature of the startle response may be critical to understanding the biological relevance of CRF effects on startle plasticity. Possible functions of the startle reflex are reduction of the latency for defensive flight (Pilz and Schnitzler, 1996) or protection during unexpected predator attack from behind (Yeomans and Frankland, 1995; cf. Fendt, 1998). If CRF-urocortin released during threat produces more robust startle responses, perhaps it is not surprising that CRF1 activation, in accordance with its activation of defensive behaviors, may also act to block any inhibition of the startle response from sensory input (e.g., from the prepulse). Thus, CRF1 activation is “protecting” the behavior when it is theoretically needed most, during threat. On the other hand, CRF2 activation increases the startle response further while enhancing the ability of sensory information to inhibit the response. Thus, with CRF2 activation, startle responsiveness remains high if needed, but it can now be modulated by other external or internal influences. The idea that CRF2 increases the flexibility of CRF1-activated behaviors also supports the hypothesized function of this receptor to modulate the initial effects of CRF1 activation during stress (Coste et al., 2001). Interestingly, both CRF administration and stress also reduce other forms of sensory inhibition or gating (Miyazato et al., 2000), which are also disrupted in PTSD patients (Gillette et al., 1997; Skinner et al., 1999). Thus, it is possible that CRF may be affecting information processing mechanisms per se, which in turn could modulate many responses other than just startle. Nevertheless, the benefits for the organism of CRF-induced reductions in gating remain unclear. We are currently exploring the possibility that CRF does not produce global “deficits” in gating but instead alters the tuning of gating mechanisms, such that gating is optimized for different stimulus conditions with CRF release compared with without it. For example, as with manipulations of dopamine (Swerdlow et al., 2003), CRF alters the temporal response function of PPI (e.g., prepulse to pulse intervals) (V. Risbrough, R. Hauger, and M. Geyer, unpublished observations).

Two extant hypotheses exist currently about the relative function of CRF receptors in anxiety-like responding, with data supporting either additive (Takahashi et al., 2001; Bakshi et al., 2002; Pelleymounter et al., 2002) or opposing (Radulovic et al., 1999; Bale et al., 2000; Kishimoto et al., 2000; Valdez et al., 2003) influences of CRF2 on CRF1 activation-induced behavior. The reports that activation of septal CRF2 receptors specifically increases freezing and reduces exploration indicate that the influences of CRF2 on anxiety-like behaviors may be region specific (Radulovic et al., 1999; Bakshi et al., 2002). The present findings of complementary effects of CRF1 and CRF2 on startle yet opposing effects on inhibition of startle (i.e., PPI) in the same animals may indicate that both models are possible within a given behavior, depending on the stimuli (e.g., prepulse or pulse alone) driving the behavior. CRF2 activity on startle was not measurable in the present studies unless there was putative CRF1 activity as well, which was not the case for CRF2 effects on PPI. Therefore, measurable CRF2 effects on behavior may sometimes depend on a minimal level of concomitant CRF1 activity. On the other hand, it may be that CRF2 effects on startle are dependent on more CRF2 activation than are effects on PPI; perhaps urocortin 3 and 0.2 nmol of h/r-CRF in CRF1 KO mice were not potent enough to increase startle via CRF2 receptors alone. Thus, how much and in particular where endogenous CRF-urocortin agonist(s) are released during various stressors may be critical to understanding respective CRF receptor functions across various anxiety models.

Previous studies have indicated that CRF effects on startle responding are mediated by the hippocampus and bed nucleus stria terminalis (Davis et al., 1997), both of which express CRF1 and CRF2 (Van Pett et al., 2000). The anatomical substrates mediating the effects of CRF on startle inhibition, however, are unknown. Regions that modulate PPI and express CRF receptors include the amygdala, hippocampus, raphe nuclei, and the nucleus reticularis pontis caudalis (Van Pett et al., 2000; Swerdlow et al., 2001). Interestingly, dopamine infusions into the amygdala reduce PPI (for review, see Swerdlow et al., 2001), and dopamine terminals overlap with CRF-expressing neurons in the central amygdala (Eliava et al., 2003). Antipsychotic medications reduce the PPI deficits observed in CRF-overexpressing mice (Dirks et al., 2003), which is likely attributable to dopamine or serotonin receptor antagonism. It is also possible that the CRF effects on PPI are mediated by the modulation of monoamine release in other brain regions. For example, CRF infusion into the ventral tegmentum significantly increased dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens (Kalivas et al., 1987), a brain region in which increased dopamine release disrupts PPI (Swerdlow et al., 2001). CRF also influences serotonin transmission during stress via CRF receptors in the dorsal and median raphe (Price et al., 1998; Kirby et al., 2000; Lowry et al., 2000; Hammack et al., 2002; Pernar et al., 2004). Modulation of serotonin transmission certainly affects PPI, because 8-OHDPAT [8-hydroxy-2-(di-N-propylamino)-tetralin], a 5-HT1A agonist, reduces PPI when delivered to the median or dorsal raphe (Sipes and Geyer, 1995). Therefore, it is possible that CRF receptors modulate PPI via indirect effects on serotonergic or dopaminergic mechanisms.

The endogenous ligands mediating these effects on startle are also unknown. Urocortin-deficient mice exhibit reduced startle threshold but blunted responses to high-intensity stimuli (Wang et al., 2002), indicating that urocortin may mediate CRF1 and/or CRF2 effects on startle. Unlike urocortin-deficient mice, unstressed (vehicle) CRF1 receptor KO mice had no baseline differences in startle or PPI, indicating that loss of CRF1 does not alter startle responses during conditions of presumably low CRF release. Thus, it is possible that the reduced startle exhibited in urocortin-deficient mice is attributable to loss of urocortin activity at CRF2 specifically. Future studies of CRF2, urocortin 2, and urocortin 3 KO mice are warranted (as they become available) to answer whether loss of CRF2 or its specific ligands results in patterns of startle behavior similar to those observed in urocortin KO mice.

The biologically active concentrations administered in the present studies (0.06-0.6 nmol/5 μl) are within the intracerebroventricular dose range for h/r-CRF effects on feeding, activity, and anxiety behaviors (Jones et al., 1999; Reyes et al., 2001). The exogenous CRF dose range required for efficacy is many-fold higher than stress-induced CRF release in the amygdala (11.5 fmol/50 μl) (Merlo Pich et al., 1995; Merali et al., 1998), calling into question the physiological relevance of exogenous CRF-induced behaviors. Unfortunately, comparisons of exogenous peptide versus endogenous peptide concentrations via microdialysis are difficult to interpret. For example, exogenous CRF concentrations that reach the potential brain regions of interest at the time point (e.g., 0.5-2 hr) in which behavioral measures are made are unknown and are likely to be much lower than the original concentration administered. Furthermore, the CRF dialysis recovery is 3% in vitro (for review, see Merali et al., 1998), indicating the in vivo dialysis measures are a great deal lower than the actual endogenous CRF concentrations at the synapse. The effects of exogenous CRF on startle and PPI, however, do mimic stress-induced effects on startle behavior, lending support to the physiological relevance to the present data. Acute or chronic stress has been shown to increase startle magnitude and reduce PPI in rats (Brake et al., 2000; Sipos et al., 2000; Faraday, 2002) and mice (Risbrough, Hauger, and Geyer, unpublished observations). Moreover, alcohol withdrawal, which induces CRF release (Merlo Pich et al., 1995), increases startle and reduces PPI in rodents (Rassnick et al., 1992) and humans (Keedwell et al., 2001).

Clinical studies have revealed gating deficits in patients with panic disorder (Ludewig et al., 2002) and PTSD (Grillon et al., 1996; Gillette et al., 1997). Hence, we suggest that CRF1 receptor supersensitivity (possibly attributable to a deficiency in homologous desensitization mechanisms) (Dautzenberg and Hauger, 2002) may override CRF2 receptor modulation of startle plasticity as indicated by reduced PPI. CRF has been implicated in particular in the production of panic attacks in patients, whereas high CRF levels in CSF and dysregulation of CRF-induced neuroendocrine responses occur in PTSD and possibly panic disorder (Holsboer et al., 1987; Bremner et al., 1997; Arborelius et al., 1999). Indeed, a recent preliminary report indicates that CSF concentrations of CRF are highest in PTSD patients that also exhibit pronounced disease severity and psychosis (Sautter et al., 2003). Thus, whether the PPI deficits incurred after stress in rodents or specifically in anxiety disorders in humans are mediated in part by CRF and pathophysiological presynaptic and postsynaptic CRF neurotransmission, respectively, remains to be determined. Schizophrenia patients also exhibit PPI deficits (Geyer and Braff, 1987; Braff and Geyer, 1990), and antipsychotic drugs block PPI deficits in CRF-overexpressing mice (Dirks et al., 2003), but there are conflicting data on whether disruption of the CRF system is an aspect of the pathophysiology particular to schizophrenia (Banki et al., 1987; Frederiksen et al., 1991; Lammers et al., 1995; Xing et al., 2004). Because so many different genetic (Geyer et al., 2002), pharmacological (Geyer et al., 2001), and neuroanatomical (Swerdlow et al., 2001) manipulations as well as disease states other than the anxiety disorders mentioned above (e.g., Tourette's syndrome, Huntington's disease, and obsessive compulsive disorder) (for review, see Braff et al., 2001) exert effects on sensorimotor gating and startle, it seems unlikely that all of these effects are attributable to one system (e.g., CRF) alone. Nevertheless, it is possible that stress exacerbates sensorimotor gating deficits in many patients, regardless of the specific underlying pathophysiology of information processing disturbances. Together with the present study, however, the converging data of CRF pathophysiology and PPI deficits in panic disorder and PTSD patients support the potential clinical use of CRF1 antagonists in these particular clinical populations.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the United States Veterans Affairs (VA) Veterans Integrated Service Network 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center (M.A.G. and R.L.H.), a VA Merit Review grant (R.L.H.), a Merck NEP fellowship (V.B.R.), National Institute of Mental Health National Research Service Award MH68133 (V.B.R.), National Institute on Drug Abuse Grants DA02925 (M.A.G.) and DA13769 (R.L.H.), and National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grant AA13523 (A.L.R.). We thank Drs. Dimitri Grigoriadis and Alan Foster for gifts of NBI-30775 and antisauvagine-30 and Drs. George Koob and Frank Dautzenberg for advice and assistance. We also thank James Doherty, Sorana Caldwell, Coree Levy, and Mahálah Buell for technical assistance and Dr. Susan Powell and Ginny Masten for comments.

  • M.A.G. holds an equity interest in San Diego Instruments (San Diego, CA).

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Mark Geyer, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093-0804. E-mail: mgeyer{at}ucsd.edu.

  • Copyright © 2004 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/04/246545-08$15.00/0

References

  1. ↵
    Arborelius L, Owens MJ, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB (1999) The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in depression and anxiety disorders. J Endocrinol 160: 1-12.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  2. ↵
    Bakshi VP, Kalin NH (2000) Corticotropin-releasing hormone and animal models of anxiety: gene-environment interactions. Biol Psychiatry 48: 1175-1198.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Bakshi VP, Smith-Roe S, Newman SM, Grigoriadis DE, Kalin NH (2002) Reduction of stress-induced behavior by antagonism of corticotropin-releasing hormone 2 (CRH2) receptors in lateral septum or CRH1 receptors in amygdala. J Neurosci 22: 2926-2935.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    Bale TL, Contarino A, Smith GW, Chan R, Gold LH, Sawchenko PE, Koob GF, Vale WW, Lee KF (2000) Mice deficient for corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor-2 display anxiety-like behaviour and are hypersensitive to stress. Nat Genet 24: 410-414.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Banki CM, Bissette G, Arato M, O'Connor L, Nemeroff CB (1987) CSF corticotropin-releasing factor-like immunoreactivity in depression and schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 144: 873-877.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  6. ↵
    Braff DL, Geyer MA (1990) Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia. Human and animal model studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47: 181-188.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR (2001) Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156: 234-258.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Brake WG, Flores G, Francis D, Meaney MJ, Srivastava LK, Gratton A (2000) Enhanced nucleus accumbens dopamine and plasma corticosterone stress responses in adult rats with neonatal excitotoxic lesions to the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 96: 687-695.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Brauns O, Liepold T, Radulovic J, Spiess J (2001) Pharmacological and chemical properties of astressin, antisauvagine-30 and alpha-helCRF: significance for behavioral experiments. Neuropharmacology 41: 507-516.
    OpenUrl
  10. ↵
    Bremner JD, Licinio J, Darnell A, Krystal JH, Owens MJ, Southwick SM, Nemeroff CB, Charney DS (1997) Elevated CSF corticotropin-releasing factor concentrations in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 154: 624-629.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. ↵
    Brody SA, Dulawa SC, Conquet F, Geyer MA (2004) Assessment of a prepulse inhibition deficit in a mutant mouse lacking mGlu5 receptors. Mol Psychiatry 9: 35-41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Brown JS, Kalish HI, Farber IE (1951) Conditioned fear as revealed by magnitude of startle response to an auditory stimulus. J Exp Psychol 41: 317-328.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Conti LH, Murry JD, Ruiz MA, Printz MP (2002) Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor on prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response in two rat strains. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 161: 296-303.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Coste SC, Murray SE, Stenzel-Poore MP (2001) Animal models of CRH excess and CRH receptor deficiency display altered adaptations to stress. Peptides 22: 733-741.
    OpenUrl
  15. ↵
    Crawley JN, Belknap JK, Collins A, Crabbe JC, Frankel W, Henderson N, Hitzemann RJ, Maxson SC, Miner LL, Silva AJ, Wehner JM, Wynshaw-Boris A, Paylor R (1997) Behavioral phenotypes of inbred mouse strains: implications and recommendations for molecular studies. Psychopharmacology 132: 107-124.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Dautzenberg FM, Hauger RL (2002) The CRF peptide family and their receptors: yet more partners discovered. Trends Pharmacol Sci 23: 71-77.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Dautzenberg FM, Kilpatrick GJ, Wille S, Hauger RL (1999) The ligand-selective domains of corticotropin-releasing factor type 1 and type 2 receptor reside in different extracellular domains: generation of chimeric receptors with a novel ligand-selective profile. J Neurochem 73: 821-829.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Dautzenberg FM, Py-Lang G, Higelin J, Fischer C, Wright MB, Huber G (2001) Different binding modes of amphibian and human corticotropin-releasing factor type 1 and type 2 receptors: evidence for evolutionary differences. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 296: 113-120.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Davis M, Walker DL, Lee Y (1997) Roles of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in fear and anxiety measured with the acoustic startle reflex. Possible relevance to PTSD. Ann NY Acad Sci 821: 305-331.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Dirks A, Groenink L, Schipholt MI, van der Gugten J, Hijzen TH, Geyer MA, Olivier B (2002) Reduced startle reactivity and plasticity in transgenic mice overexpressing corticotropin-releasing hormone. Biol Psychiatry 51: 583-590.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Dirks A, Groenink L, Westphal KG, Olivier JD, Verdouw PM, Van Der Gugten J, Geyer MA, Olivier B (2003) Reversal of startle gating deficits in transgenic mice overexpressing corticotropin-releasing factor by anti-psychotic drugs. Neuropsychopharmacology 28: 1790-1798.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Eliava M, Yilmazer-Hanke D, Asan E (2003) Interrelations between monoaminergic afferents and corticotropin-releasing factor-immunoreactive neurons in the rat central amygdaloid nucleus: ultrastructural evidence for dopaminergic control of amygdaloid stress systems. Histochem Cell Biol 120: 183-197.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Faraday MM (2002) Rat sex and strain differences in responses to stress. Physiol Behav 75: 507-522.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Fendt M (1998) Different regions of the periaqueductal grey are involved differently in the expression and conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle. Eur J Neurosci 10: 3876-3884.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Frederiksen SO, Ekman R, Gottfries CG, Widerlov E, Jonsson S (1991) Reduced concentrations of galanin, arginine vasopressin, neuropeptide Y and peptide YY in the temporal cortex but not in the hypothalamus of brains from schizophrenics. Acta Psychiatr Scand 83: 273-277.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Geyer MA, Braff DL (1987) Startle habituation and sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia and related animal models. Schizophr Bull 13: 643-668.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    Geyer MA, Dulawa SC (2003) Assessment of murine startle reactivity, prepulse inhibition, and habituation. In: Current protocols in neuroscience (Crawley JN, Skolnick P, eds), pp 8.17.11-18.17.15. New York: Wiley.
  28. ↵
    Geyer MA, Krebs-Thomson K, Braff DL, Swerdlow NR (2001) Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: a decade in review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156: 117-154.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Geyer MA, McIlwain KL, Paylor R (2002) Mouse genetic models for prepulse inhibition: an early review. Mol Psychiatry 7: 1039-1053.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Gillette GM, Skinner RD, Rasco LM, Fielstein EM, Davis DH, Pawelak JE, Freeman TW, Karson CN, Boop FA, Garcia-Rill E (1997) Combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder exhibit decreased habituation of the P1 midlatency auditory evoked potential. Life Sci 61: 1421-1434.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Graham FK (1975) Presidential Address, 1974. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimulation. Psychophysiology 12: 238-248.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Grillon C, Morgan CA, Southwick SM, Davis M, Charney DS (1996) Baseline startle amplitude and prepulse inhibition in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res 64: 169-178.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Grillon C, Morgan III CA, Davis M, Southwick SM (1998) Effects of experimental context and explicit threat cues on acoustic startle in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 44: 1027-1036.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Hammack SE, Richey KJ, Schmid MJ, LoPresti ML, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2002) The role of corticotropin-releasing hormone in the dorsal raphe nucleus in mediating the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress. J Neurosci 22: 1020-1026.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Hammack SE, Schmid MJ, LoPresti ML, Der-Avakian A, Pellymounter MA, Foster AC, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2003) Corticotropin releasing hormone type 2 receptors in the dorsal raphe nucleus mediate the behavioral consequences of uncontrollable stress. J Neurosci 23: 1019-1025.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Higelin J, Py-Lang G, Paternoster C, Ellis GJ, Patel A, Dautzenberg FM (2001) 125I-Antisauvagine-30: a novel and specific high-affinity radioligand for the characterization of corticotropin-releasing factor type 2 receptors. Neuropharmacology 40: 114-122.
    OpenUrl
  37. ↵
    Hoffman HS, Ison JR (1980) Reflex modification in the domain of startle. I. Some empirical findings and their implications for how the nervous system processes sensory input. Psychol Rev 87: 175-189.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Holsboer F, von Bardeleben U, Buller R, Heuser I, Steiger A (1987) Stimulation response to corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in patients with depression, alcoholism and panic disorder. Horm Metab Res Suppl 16: 80-88.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  39. ↵
    Jones DN, Kortekaas R, Hatcher PD, Middlemiss DN, White A, Hagan JJ (1999) Influence of peptide CRF receptor antagonists upon the behavioural effects of human/rat CRF. Eur J Pharmacol 373: 141-145.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Kalivas PW, Duffy P, Latimer LG (1987) Neurochemical and behavioral effects of corticotropin-releasing factor in the ventral tegmental area of the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 242: 757-763.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Keedwell PA, Kumari V, Poon L, Marshall EJ, Checkley SA (2001) Information processing deficits in withdrawing alcoholics. Addict Biol 6: 239-245.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Kirby LG, Rice KC, Valentino RJ (2000) Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor on neuronal activity in the serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuropsychopharmacology 22: 148-162.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Kishimoto T, Radulovic J, Radulovic M, Lin CR, Schrick C, Hooshmand F, Hermanson O, Rosenfeld MG, Spiess J (2000) Deletion of crhr2 reveals an anxiolytic role for corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor-2. Nat Genet 24: 415-419.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Koob GF, Heinrichs SC (1999) A role for corticotropin releasing factor and urocortin in behavioral responses to stressors. Brain Res 848: 141-152.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Lammers CH, Garcia-Borreguero D, Schmider J, Gotthardt U, Dettling M, Holsboer F, Heuser IJ (1995) Combined dexamethasone/corticotropin-releasing hormone test in patients with schizophrenia and in normal controls. II. Biol Psychiatry 38: 803-807.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (1990) Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychol Rev 97: 377-395.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    Lewis K, Li C, Perrin MH, Blount A, Kunitake K, Donaldson C, Vaughan J, Reyes TM, Gulyas J, Fischer W, Bilezikjian L, Rivier J, Sawchenko PE, Vale WW (2001) Identification of urocortin III, an additional member of the corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) family with high affinity for the CRF2 receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 7570-7575.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    Lowry CA, Rodda JE, Lightman SL, Ingram CD (2000) Corticotropin-releasing factor increases in vitro firing rates of serotonergic neurons in the rat dorsal raphe nucleus: evidence for activation of a topographically organized mesolimbocortical serotonergic system. J Neurosci 20: 7728-7736.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    Lu L, Liu Z, Huang M, Zhang Z (2003) Dopamine-dependent responses to cocaine depend on corticotropin-releasing factor receptor subtypes. J Neurochem 84: 1378-1386.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Ludewig S, Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Hell D, Vollenweider FX (2002) Prepulse inhibition deficits in patients with panic disorder. Depress Anxiety 15: 55-60.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Merali Z, McIntosh J, Kent P, Michaud D, Anisman H (1998) Aversive and appetitive events evoke the release of corticotropin-releasing hormone and bombesin-like peptides at the central nucleus of the amygdala. J Neurosci 18: 4758-4766.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    Merlo Pich E, Lorang M, Yeganeh M, Rodriguez de Fonseca F, Raber J, Koob GF, Weiss F (1995) Increase of extracellular corticotropin-releasing factor-like immunoreactivity levels in the amygdala of awake rats during restraint stress and ethanol withdrawal as measured by microdialysis. J Neurosci 15: 5439-5447.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  53. ↵
    Miyazato H, Skinner RD, Garcia-Rill E (2000) Locus coeruleus involvement in the effects of immobilization stress on the p13 midlatency auditory evoked potential in the rat. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 24: 1177-1201.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Musselman DL, Nemeroff CB (2003) Neuropeptides and hypothalamic releasing factors in psychiatric illness. In: Psychoneuroendocrinology (Wolkowitz OM, Rothschild AJ, eds), pp 29-82. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
  55. ↵
    Norris C, Blumenthal T (1996) A relationship between inhibition of the acoustic startle response and the protection of prepulse processing. Psychobiology 24: 160-168.
    OpenUrl
  56. ↵
    Overstreet DH, Knapp DJ, Breese GR (2004) Modulation of multiple ethanol withdrawal-induced anxiety-like behavior by CRF and CRF1 receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 77: 405-413.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Pelleymounter MA, Joppa M, Ling N, Foster AC (2002) Pharmacological evidence supporting a role for central corticotropin-releasing factor(2) receptors in behavioral, but not endocrine, response to environmental stress. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 302: 145-152.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    Pernar L, Curtis AL, Vale WW, Rivier JE, Valentino RJ (2004) Selective activation of corticotropin-releasing factor-2 receptors on neurochemically identified neurons in the rat dorsal raphe nucleus reveals dual actions. J Neurosci 24: 1305-1311.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    Perrin MH, Vale WW (1999) Corticotropin releasing factor receptors and their ligand family. Ann NY Acad Sci 885: 312-328.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    Pilz PK, Schnitzler HU (1996) Habituation and sensitization of the acoustic startle response in rats: amplitude, threshold, and latency measures. Neurobiol Learn Mem 66: 67-79.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Price ML, Curtis AL, Kirby LG, Valentino RJ, Lucki I (1998) Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor on brain serotonergic activity. Neuropsychopharmacology 18: 492-502.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Radulovic J, Ruhmann A, Liepold T, Spiess J (1999) Modulation of learning and anxiety by corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) and stress: differential roles of CRF receptors 1 and 2. J Neurosci 19: 5016-5025.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    Rassnick S, Koob GF, Geyer MA (1992) Responding to acoustic startle during chronic ethanol intoxication and withdrawal. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 106: 351-358.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Reul JM, Holsboer F (2002) Corticotropin-releasing factor receptors 1 and 2 in anxiety and depression. Curr Opin Pharmacol 2: 23-33.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Reyes TM, Lewis K, Perrin MH, Kunitake KS, Vaughan J, Arias CA, Hogenesch JB, Gulyas J, Rivier J, Vale WW, Sawchenko PE (2001) Urocortin II: a member of the corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) neuropeptide family that is selectively bound by type 2 CRF receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 2843-2848.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    Risbrough VB, Hauger RL, Pelleymounter MA, Geyer MA (2003) Role of corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) receptors 1 and 2 in CRF-potentiated acoustic startle in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 170: 178-187.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    Rivier J, Spiess J, Vale W (1983) Characterization of rat hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 80: 4851-4855.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    Rivier J, Gulyas J, Kirby D, Low W, Perrin MH, Kunitake K, DiGruccio M, Vaughan J, Reubi JC, Waser B, Koerber SC, Martinez V, Wang L, Tache Y, Vale W (2002) Potent and long-acting corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) receptor 2 selective peptide competitive antagonists. J Med Chem 45: 4737-4747.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    Sautter FJ, Bissette G, Wiley J, Manguno-Mire G, Schoenbachler B, Myers L, Johnson JE, Cerbone A, Malaspina D (2003) Corticotropin-releasing factor in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) with secondary psychotic symptoms, nonpsychotic PTSD, and healthy control subjects. Biol Psychiatry 54: 1382-1388.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    Sipes TA, Geyer MA (1995) 8-OH-DPAT disruption of prepulse inhibition in rats: reversal with (+)WAY 100,135 and localization of site of action. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 117: 41-48.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    Sipos ML, Bauman RA, Widholm JJ, Kant GJ (2000) Behavioral effects Of 8-OH-DPAT in chronically stressed male and female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 66: 403-411.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    Skinner RD, Rasco LM, Fitzgerald J, Karson CN, Matthew M, Williams DK, Garcia-Rill E (1999) Reduced sensory gating of the P1 potential in rape victims and combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress Anxiety 9: 122-130.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    Smith GW, Aubry JM, Dellu F, Contarino A, Bilezikjian LM, Gold LH, Chen R, Marchuk Y, Hauser C, Bentley CA, Sawchenko PE, Koob GF, Vale W, Lee KF (1998) Corticotropin releasing factor receptor 1-deficient mice display decreased anxiety, impaired stress response, and aberrant neuroendocrine development. Neuron 20: 1093-1102.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    Spina MG, Basso AM, Zorrilla EP, Heyser CJ, Rivier J, Vale W, Merlo-Pich E, Koob GF (2000) Behavioral effects of central administration of the novel CRF antagonist astressin in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 22: 230-239.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Vale WW, Koob GF (1986) Corticotropin-releasing factor potentiates acoustic startle in rats: blockade by chlordiazepoxide. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 88: 147-152.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Blumenthal T, Hartman PL (1999) Effects of discrete acoustic prestimuli on perceived intensity and behavioral responses to startling acoustic and tactile stimuli. Psychobiology 27: 547-556.
    OpenUrl
  77. ↵
    Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL (2001) Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156: 194-215.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    Swerdlow NR, Shoemaker JM, Auerbach PP, Pitcher L, Goins J, Platten A (2003) Heritable differences in the dopaminergic regulation of sensorimotor gating. II. Temporal, pharmacologic and generational analyses of apomorphine effects on prepulse inhibition. Psychopharmacology (Berl). May 21, in press.
  79. ↵
    Takahashi LK, Ho SP, Livanov V, Graciani N, Arneric SP (2001) Antagonism of CRF(2) receptors produces anxiolytic behavior in animal models of anxiety. Brain Res 902: 135-142.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    Valdez GR, Inoue K, Koob GF, Rivier J, Vale W, Zorrilla EP (2002) Human urocortin II: mild locomotor suppressive and delayed anxiolytic-like effects of a novel corticotropin-releasing factor related peptide. Brain Res 943: 142-150.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    Valdez GR, Zorrilla EP, Rivier J, Vale WW, Koob GF (2003) Locomotor suppressive and anxiolytic-like effects of urocortin 3, a highly selective type 2 corticotropin-releasing factor agonist. Brain Res 980: 206-212.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    Vale W, Spiess J, Rivier C, Rivier J (1981) Characterization of a 41-residue ovine hypothalamic peptide that stimulates secretion of corticotropin and beta-endorphin. Science 213: 1394-1397.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  83. ↵
    Van Pett K, Viau V, Bittencourt JC, Chan RK, Li HY, Arias C, Prins GS, Perrin M, Vale W, Sawchenko PE (2000) Distribution of mRNAs encoding CRF receptors in brain and pituitary of rat and mouse. J Comp Neurol 428: 191-212.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    Wang X, Su H, Copenhagen LD, Vaishnav S, Pieri F, Shope CD, Brownell WE, De Biasi M, Paylor R, Bradley A (2002) Urocortin-deficient mice display normal stress-induced anxiety behavior and autonomic control but an impaired acoustic startle response. Mol Cell Biol 22: 6605-6610.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  85. ↵
    Xing GQ, Russell S, Webster MJ, Post RM (2004) Decreased expression of mineralocorticoid receptor mRNA in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 7: 143-153.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  86. ↵
    Yeomans JS, Frankland PW (1995) The acoustic startle reflex: neurons and connections. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 21: 301-314.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    Yeomans JS, Li L, Scott BW, Frankland PW (2002) Tactile, acoustic and vestibular systems sum to elicit the startle reflex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26: 1-11.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 24 (29)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 24, Issue 29
21 Jul 2004
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Corticotropin-Releasing Factor Receptors CRF1 and CRF2 Exert Both Additive and Opposing Influences on Defensive Startle Behavior
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Corticotropin-Releasing Factor Receptors CRF1 and CRF2 Exert Both Additive and Opposing Influences on Defensive Startle Behavior
Victoria B. Risbrough, Richard L. Hauger, Amanda L. Roberts, Wylie W. Vale, Mark A. Geyer
Journal of Neuroscience 21 July 2004, 24 (29) 6545-6552; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5760-03.2004

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Corticotropin-Releasing Factor Receptors CRF1 and CRF2 Exert Both Additive and Opposing Influences on Defensive Startle Behavior
Victoria B. Risbrough, Richard L. Hauger, Amanda L. Roberts, Wylie W. Vale, Mark A. Geyer
Journal of Neuroscience 21 July 2004, 24 (29) 6545-6552; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5760-03.2004
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • The Laminar Development of Direction Selectivity in Ferret Visual Cortex
  • Individual Differences in Amygdala-Medial Prefrontal Anatomy Link Negative Affect, Impaired Social Functioning, and Polygenic Depression Risk
  • Influence of Reward on Corticospinal Excitability during Movement Preparation
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.