Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Featured ArticleBehavioral/Systems/Cognitive

The S6KII (rsk) Gene of Drosophila melanogaster Differentially Affects an Operant and a Classical Learning Task

Gabriele Putz, Franco Bertolucci, Thomas Raabe, Troy Zars and Martin Heisenberg
Journal of Neuroscience 3 November 2004, 24 (44) 9745-9751; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3211-04.2004
Gabriele Putz
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Franco Bertolucci
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thomas Raabe
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Troy Zars
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Martin Heisenberg
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

In an attempt to dissect classical and operant conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster, we have isolated the gene for ribosomal S6 kinase II (S6KII). This enzyme is part of a family of serine-threonine kinases that in mammals have been implicated in the MAPK (mitogen-activated protein kinase) signaling cascade controlling (among other processes) synaptic plasticity (long-term potentiation/long-term depression) and memory formation. The human homolog rsk2 has been linked to mental retardation (Coffin-Lowry syndrome). Mutant analysis in Drosophila shows that S6KII serves different functions in operant place learning and classical (pavlovian) olfactory conditioning. Whereas in the null mutant only pavlovian olfactory learning is affected, a P-element insertion mutant reducing the amount of S6KII only affects operant place learning. A mutant lacking part of the N-terminal kinase domain and performing poorly in both learning tasks is dominant in the operant paradigm and recessive in the pavlovian paradigm. The behavioral defects in the pavlovian task can be rescued by the genomic S6KII transgene. Overexpression of S6KII in wild type has a dominant-negative effect on the operant task that is rescued by the null mutant, whereas in the pavlovian task overexpression may even enhance learning performance.

  • Drosophila melanogaster
  • associative learning
  • heat box
  • operant conditioning
  • classical conditioning
  • p90 ribosomal S6 kinase

Introduction

In associative pavlovian (classical) conditioning, the organism exploits the temporal relationships of events in the environment; in operant conditioning, the animal adapts its actions to their consequences. In search of genes in Drosophila melanogaster that would differentially affect the two forms of learning, we have isolated the gene S6KII that encodes ribosomal S6 kinase II (Wassarman et al., 1994).

Ribosomal S6 kinases [RSKs; also known as p90rsk, S6KI and S6KII, or mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK)-activated protein kinase-1] are a family of serine-threonine kinases that become activated by and are mediators of the Ras-extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) signaling pathway (Frodin and Gammeltoft, 1999). Four RSK isoforms (RSK1-4) are known in vertebrates, whereas in Drosophila only a single isoform has been described (Wassarman et al., 1994). (We will keep the name S6KII for the Drosophila gene and refer to the Drosophila enzyme as S6KII.) RSKs are characterized by two kinase domains. The C-terminal kinase domain contributes through autophosphorylation to full activation of the N-terminal kinase domain, which in turn phosphorylates substrates such as the cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB), c-Fos, NFκB (nuclear factor κB)/IκBα (inhibitor of nuclear factor-κB), and estrogen receptor (Bjorbaek et al., 1995; Nebreda and Gavin, 1999).

RSKs have diverse functions. They are important players in cell cycle progression, cell survival, and cytostatic-factor arrest. Additional roles of RSKs include the feedback inhibition of the Ras-ERK pathway and the regulation of protein synthesis by phosphorylation of polyribosomal proteins and glycogen synthase kinase-3 (Frodin and Gammeltoft, 1999; Nebreda and Gavin, 1999; Ballif and Blenis, 2001).

Mutations in the human rsk2 gene are associated with the Coffin-Lowry syndrome (CLS) (Lowry et al., 1971), an X-linked disorder characterized by severe psychomotor retardation, digit and facial dysmorphisms, and progressive skeletal deformations (Young, 1988; Trivier et al., 1996). In CLS fibroblasts, a drastic attenuation in the induced Ser-133 phosphorylation of transcription factor CREB was detected in response to epidermal growth factor stimulation (De Cesare et al., 1998).

The ERK/MAPK cascade is involved in neuronal as well as behavioral plasticity (English and Sweatt, 1997; Impey et al., 1998; Sgambato et al., 1998; Orban et al., 1999), and studies assign the RSKs a role in these processes as the direct link between MAPKs and CREB (Xing et al., 1996). Near to nothing is known directly about functions of RSKs in invertebrates (Rintelen et al., 2001). However, the MAPK cascade has been implicated in neuronal plasticity in molluscs (Martin et al., 1997), and in Drosophila, a role of the MAPK cascade in pavlovian memory formation was indicated by a mutant of the leonardo (leo) gene encoding a 14-3-3 family protein (Skoulakis and Davis, 1996).

We report here the first functional study of the rsk homolog in Drosophila, the S6KII gene. Unlike in CLS patients, null mutations in the S6KII gene cause no visible external morphological defects but, in accordance with the human condition and the rsk2 knock-out mouse (Dufresne et al., 2001), impair cognitive functions in that operant place learning and pavlovian olfactory conditioning are affected. Yet, the gene plays distinctly different roles in the two forms of behavioral plasticity.

Materials and Methods

Flies. A collection of X-chromosomal insertion lines of the P[lacW] element was provided by Dr. Ulrich Schäfer (Max-Planck Institute for Biophysical Chemistry, Göttingen, Germany). The insertion site of the P[lacW] element in ignP1 was determined by in situ hybridization on polytene chromosomes. Genomic fragments flanking the P insertion site were recovered by plasmid rescue and sequenced. Precise and imprecise excision lines of P-element line ignP1 were generated by remobilization of the P[lacW] with introduction of a stable transposase source (Robertson et al., 1988). The lines were screened by Southern blot for precise excision of the P-element and by PCR for deletions of the S6KII gene locus. Deletions of interesting candidates were characterized by sequencing. Lines were converted to the genetic background of wild-type Berlin by repeatedly outcrossing them to w1118 flies on a wild-type Berlin background. For additional analysis, the w+ gene was recombined onto the X chromosome. Selection for recombination events was done by PCR. Subsequently, the strains were also crossed to wild-type Canton S (WT-CS) for six generations and made homozygous. According to the genetic background of the tested flies, Drosophila strain w1118 or WT-CS was used as a control line. Flies were reared on cornmeal/molasses medium (Guo et al., 1996) in a 16/8 hr light/dark cycle at 60% humidity and 25°C. Adults were studied at 2-7 d after eclosion.

Operant learning in the heat box. The conditioning apparatus (see Fig. 1) consists of an array of 15 chambers (26 × 4 × 2 mm) operating in parallel, each with Peltier elements on top and bottom allowing for fast heating and cooling (Putz and Heisenberg, 2002). The standard experiment includes a 30 sec pretest, 4 min training, and 3 min test period. (In some experiments, the pretest period is extended to 5 min, a measure that may slightly reduce variation in the data.) During the experiment, one half of the chamber is defined as the “punished” side and the other as the “unpunished” side. Designations are altered for every experiment. In the pre-test, the fly can explore the chamber at a constant temperature of 20°C. During the training period, the whole chamber is heated to 40°C whenever the fly enters the punished side and is cooled down to 20°C when it enters the unpunished side. In the test period following immediately, the chamber is constantly at 20°C. A performance index (PI) is calculated as the difference between the time the fly spent in the unpunished versus punished half of the chamber divided by the total time. For analysis, training and test phases were binned into 1 or 2 min blocks. Training values refer to the last minute of training and test values to the first minute of the memory test. Because tests for normal distribution of performance indices yield varying results, nonparametrical tests were used for statistical evaluation. Two independent groups were compared by Mann-Whitney U tests. For comparison of three and more groups, Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA tests were used. Thermo-sensitivity tests were performed as described by Zars (2001).

  Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schematic diagram of one of the 15 modified heat boxes operated in parallel.

Olfactory learning. Olfactory memory was tested with a modified version of the conditioning apparatus of Tully and Quinn (1985). In this assay, groups of 50-100 flies are sequentially exposed to two odorants, one of which is paired with electric shocks as a negative reinforcer. During the procedure, wild-type flies learn to prefer the unpunished odorant. The olfactory discrimination learning paradigm, control experiments for shock reactivity and odor perception, as well as calculations, are performed after that described by de-Belle and Heisenberg (1994), with slight modifications. Pure benzaldehyde (100 μl, in cups of 5 mm diameter, 7 mm depths) and 3-octanol (900 μl, in cups of 16 mm diameter, 10 mm depths) are used as odorants. Negative reinforcement consists of 12 130 V DC electric shocks over 60 sec (one every 5 sec) with each shock lasting 1.25 sec. Repeated-measures ANOVAs were used for comparison between groups; when warranted, the Duncan post hoc test was performed.

Reverse transcription-PCR. Isolation of poly(A)+ mRNA from total RNA was obtained by using the Oligotex mRNA Mini kit from Qiagen (Hilden, Germany). SUPERSCRIPT First Strand Synthesis System from Invitrogen (San Diego, CA) was optimized to synthesize first-strand cDNA from varying amounts of purified poly(A)+ or total RNA. RNA was reverse transcribed using random hexamers. The quality of isolated cDNA was tested by amplification of the rp49 ribosomal gene.

Transgenic lines. The 6.5 kb genomic fragment used for rescue experiments was derived from BACCR05K22 (AC011760) (Hoskins et al., 2000) and was cloned into the pW8 transformation vector (Klemenz et al., 1987). Transgenic lines were generated by injecting Qiagen-purified plasmid DNA into w1118 embryos. Six independent transgenic lines were established and cantonized for six generations. The site of plasmid insertion was characterized.

Antibodies and Western blot analysis. A peptide (ADLD-SARKRPTHRTLC) corresponding to amino acids 119-134 of the S6KII protein was synthesized and used to immunize rabbits by a commercial supplier. For Western blot analysis, protein lysates from 10 heads of male or female adult flies of the indicated genotype were separated by SDS-PAGE. As a control, protein extracts from five heads of flies expressing the UAS: S6KII transgene under hs-Gal4 control were used. [Transgenic lines expressing the S6KII gene under Gal4/UAS control were kindly provided by E. Hafen (Zurich, Switzerland) (Rintelen et al., 2001)]. After transfer to nitrocellulose, the blot was incubated overnight at 4°C with the affinity-purified anti-S6KII serum (1:500), then probed with an HRP-conjugated secondary antibody and developed using the ECL kit (Amersham Biosciences, Braunschweig, Germany).

Results

Behavioral and molecular characterization of mutant ignP1

Operant place learning in the heat box (Putz and Heisenberg, 2002) was used to screen 1221 homozygous viable, X-chromosomal P-element insertion (P[lacW]) lines. Flies were trained to avoid part of an elongated, narrow chamber that was quickly heated from 20 to 40°C if the fly entered this part (Fig. 1). In one line called ignorantP1 (ignP1), males showed reduced performance during the training (tr) and in the subsequent memory test (te) (U test; tr: Z = 2.70, p < 0.01; te: Z = 2.70, p < 0.01). No significant defect was observed in females on any genetic background (against w1118 on a wild-type Berlin background; U test; tr: Z = 0.13, p = NS; te: Z = 0.35, p = NS) (Fig. 2a). After recombining the chromosomal region carrying the P-element onto a WT-CS X chromosome carrying the w+ gene and extensive additional backcrossing of the autosomes to WT-CS, a robust male-specific defect during the training phase remained (U test; Z = 3.57, p < 0.001), whereas performance in the memory test was close to normal in both genders (U test; males: Z = 0.87, p = NS; females: Z = -1.43, p = NS) (Fig. 2b). Thermo-sensitivity was not impaired in ignP1 mutant males (repeated-measures ANOVA; males: F = 0.93, p = NS; females: F = 0.77, p = NS; see supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org). Because the memory phenotype of ignP1 seemed to be under polygenic control and was not apparent in the WT-CS genetic background, we decided to refer, for the rest of this report, only to the performance deficit during training, although in some of the figures first-minute memory scores will be shown as well.

  Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

ignP1 mutant shows a male-specific behavioral phenotype in heat-box conditioning. Performance indices of ignP1 versus control flies in the standard experiment are shown. The last minute of training (hatched bars) and 1 min memory test (empty bars) are shown separately for males and females. a, Performance of w1118ignP1 mutant in a wild-type Berlin genetic background compared with w1118 (Berlin) control flies. b, Performance of ignP1 mutant after recombining the S6KII gene with the P-element insertion onto the X chromosome carrying the w+ gene and backcrossing to WT-CS (designated as WT in all subsequent figures). n, Number of flies. The error bars indicate SEMs. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Molecular characterization of ignP1 and excision lines

Sequencing of DNA flanking the P-element in the ignP1 line showed that it was inserted in the 5′ untranslated region of the first exon of the gene coding for S6KII (CG17596) (Fig. 3). The P-element insertion is located 355 bp in front of the predicted translational start site. Sequencing of expressed-sequence tag clones SD05277, GH08264, and GH21818 (Stapleton et al., 2002) confirmed Flybase information about the structure of S6KII. The P-element insertion is located at position 27/28 of clone SD05277. To test whether the P-element insertion in ignP1 flies is responsible for the behavioral defect, and second, to obtain additional S6KII mutants, P-element excision lines of ignP1 flies were generated. The P-element was remobilized crossing ignP1 to a source of transposase (Robertson et al., 1988). Based on the loss of the mini-w+ marker of P[lacW], we obtained 128 jump-out lines from F1 females and 227 from F1 males (Engels et al., 1990). In a first screen of jump-outs in females, 13 lines were found by Southern blot to be candidate precise jump-out lines. Two lines, ignΔ1P1 and ignΔ2P1, restored wild-type sequence at the P-element insertion site. Line ignΔ1P1 does not show any nucleotide changes close to this site, whereas line ignΔ2P1 has several nucleotide changes surrounding the P-element insertion site in the untranslated region. The remaining jump-out lines were screened by PCR, and 13 of them contained deletions larger than 1 kb. Six of them were characterized in more detail. Sequencing of the complete region revealed that they had lost part or all of the S6KII gene (Fig. 3). Deletion line Df(1)ignΔ24-3 had a loss of 1322 bp of genomic sequence in the 5′ region of the S6KII gene, removing part of the first exon. In a similar mutant, Df(1)ignΔ30-2, 2197 bp were deleted. In excision lines Df(1)ignΔ67-1 and Df(1)ignΔ58-1, the coding region of S6KII was completely removed. Lines Df(1)ignΔ24-3, Df(1)ignΔ30-2, and Df(1)ignΔ58-1 still had retained part of the P-element, ranging from 16 bp to 5.3 kb. The largest of the deletions, Df(1)ignΔ67-1, removed the entire S6KII gene, the P-element, and a neighboring gene encoding a serine-threonine phosphatase (Fig. 3) (CG17598). All deletion lines were homozygous viable. The name ignorant (ign) was retained (as a synonym of S6KII) for all mutants derived from ignP1.

  Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Molecular map of the S6KII gene (partitions, 0.5 kb). The following are shown: EcoRV restriction sites; insertion site of P[lacW] of the ignP1 mutant; the exon/intron structure of S6KII and predicted neighboring genes; structure of sequenced cDNAs (SD0522, GH21818, and GH08264); extension of deletions; the rescue construct; and the regions amplified by RT-PCR (a, with primers GCGGCAAAATCGTGTCCCTTTC and CTGGATTTTCTTCGTGGCGGTG; b, with primers ACCTCGGGAGCCACAAAATTGG and AGCATCCACATCCACTTCTGCC; c, with primers ATATAGATGCCCCGCACAG and GCAGCAGAATCACATCTCC). N-K, N-terminal kinase domain; C-K, C-terminal kinase domain.

Expression of S6KII

To investigate whether ignP1 and the deficiencies were null mutants, we searched for transcripts by reverse transcription (RT)PCR. As expected, lines Df(1)ignΔ67-1, with a deletion of 11,366 bp, and Df(1)ignΔ58-1 (4762 bp), both removing all or nearly all of the S6KII gene, were null mutants by this criterion. Deletion lines Df(1)ignΔ24-3 and Df(1)ignΔ30-2 still had S6KII transcript in the regions where genomic DNA was intact. In the original P-element line, the S6KII gene seemed to be fully transcribed because signal was obtained in all investigated genomic regions (see a-c in Fig. 3) (RT-PCR data not shown). To analyze the expression of S6KII protein in wild-type and mutant flies, an antiserum directed against a short peptide from the N-terminal region of the protein was generated and used in Western blots (Fig. 4A). As a control, UAS:S6KII transgenic flies (Rintelen et al., 2001) were crossed to flies carrying a Gal4 transgene under control of the heat shock promoter. After heat shock, high levels of a 90 kDa protein corresponding in size to the predicted molecular weight of S6KII were expressed. A protein of the same molecular weight was detected in wild-type flies but was missing in the deletion line Df(1)ignΔ58-1. Unexpectedly, in the mutant ignP1, S6KII was reduced in both sexes to a similar degree, despite the sexually dimorphic behavior of ignP1 in operant place learning. As expected, in Df(1)ignΔ24-3, no S6KII protein could be detected, because the serum recognized a peptide from the region deleted in the mutant.

  Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Western blots of WT-CS and mutants. S6KII has a molecular weight of 90 kDa as witnessed by overexpression in hsGAL4/UAS-ign flies (left). A, This band has the same intensity in males and females of wild type and is equally reduced in both genders in the P-element line ignP1. The band is missing in the full deletion Df(1)ignΔ58-1, and the partial deletion Df(1)ignΔ24-3 is missing the genomic sequence used for antigen production. B, S6KII expression is restored in the male progeny of transgenic lines T1-T3 crossed to mutant Df(1)ignΔ58-1 females (e.g., 58-1 male + T1). (Note the faint band in the fourth lane.) The bottom bands are unrelated proteins cross-reacting with the anti-S6KII-serum.

Performance of jump-out lines in heat-box conditioning

Similar to the ignP1 mutant, some of the excision lines above were backcrossed extensively to WT-CS after recombination of the w+ gene onto the X chromosome. These lines were tested in the heat box (Fig. 5a,b). Focusing on the last training minute, both males and females of the precise excision line ignΔ1P1 performed well (U test; males: Z = 1.08, p = NS; females: Z = -0.77, p = NS). The same was found for ignΔ2P1 (U test; males: Z = 0.64, p = NS; females: Z = -1.91, p = NS). Thus, excision of the P-element reverts the phenotype of ignP1 flies to wild type.

  Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Operant learning of deletion lines versus WT-CS in the heat box standard experiment. Performance indices of the last 1 min training (gray bars) and first 1 min memory (empty bars) periods are shown for females (a) and males (b). The numbers above the bars indicate the number of flies tested. The error bars are SEMs. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Flies carrying a deletion of the 5′ part of the S6KII gene including the N-terminal kinase domain (Df(1)ignΔ24-3; Df(1)ignΔ30-2) showed reduced heat-box learning in both genders (U test; Df(1)ignΔ24-3, homozygous females: Z = 4.19, p < 0.001; hemizygous males: Z = 3.51, p < 0.001; data for Df(1)ignΔ30-2 are shown in supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org). Crossing Df(1)ignΔ24-3 to the larger-deficiency Df(1)R29 (see Flybase) deleting the whole region of the S6KII gene gave similar results: Df(1)R29/Df(1)ignΔ24-3 flies had a reduced learning score (F. Schulz, B. Poeck, and M. Heisenberg, unpublished results). Also, trans-heterozygous Df(1)ignΔ24-3/Df(1)ignΔ58-1 flies showed reduced learning (U test; Z = 3.69, p < 0.001) (Fig. 6a) indistinguishable from that of the homozygous Df(1)ignΔ24-3 (Fig. 6a). Surprisingly, the defect was fully dominant because heterozygous females (Df(1)ignΔ24-3/+) performed as poorly as homozygous mutant females and hemizygous mutant males (heterozygous females: U test: Z = 2.73, p < 0.01) (Fig. 6a).

  Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

a, Deficit in heat-box learning of partial deletion mutant Df(1)ignΔ24-3 is dominant. Shown are female (f) and male (m) performance in the last 1 min training period of homozygous and heterozygous mutant flies, as well as double heretozygous Df(1)ignΔ24-3/Df(1)ignΔ58-1 flies versus WT-CS in the standard experiment. b, Homozygous transgenic lines expressing two copies of S6KII show reduced operant learning in the heat box. (Compare expression levels of T1-T3 in Fig. 4.) c, Rescue of operant learning if expression of transgene (T1) is compensated by the deletion of the endogenous S6KII gene. Note that the PI of T1/T1 is higher than in b because of a different protocol. The numbers above the bars indicate the number of flies tested. The error bars are SEMs. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

The next surprise came with Df(1)ignΔ58-1. Removal of the complete coding region did not affect operant conditioning in the heat box (U test; males: Z = 1.24, p = NS; females: Z = 0.34, p = NS). Even the removal of 11.3 kb extending to both sides of the P-element insertion site and including S6KII and a neighboring putative phosphatase gene (CG17598) in Df(1)ignΔ67-1 did not lead to a defect in performance (U test; males: Z = 0.90, p = NS; females: Z = -0.73, p = NS), implying that the wild-type function of the S6KII gene is not essential for heat-box learning. This further implies that the small deletions Df(1)ignΔ24-3 and Df(1)ignΔ30-2, as well as ignP1, are gain-of-function mutants.

To exclude the possibility that thermo-reception was impaired in any of the lines with reduced performance, flies were tested in a thermo-sensitivity assay. Neither in males (repeated-measures ANOVA; F = 0.93, p = NS) nor in females (repeated-measures ANOVA; F = 0.77, p = NS) was a defect in thermo-reception found for any of the investigated lines, supporting the idea that the observed low performance in operant conditioning reveals a learning deficit (see supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org).

Transgenic expression of S6KII interferes with place learning

We cloned a 6.5 kb genomic fragment encompassing only the S6KII transcription unit, into a transformation vector (Fig. 3), and established six independent transgenic lines with single insertions (T1-T6). All were homozygous viable. Males from three transgenic lines were crossed to Df(1)ignΔ58-1 females, and the male progeny was analyzed on Western blots for expression of S6KII. In all cases, the transgene restored expression of S6KII. In two lines (T1, T3), the amount was similar to that in wild type, and in one (T2), it was distinctly less (Fig. 4B).

All six lines were repeatedly back-crossed to WT-CS and tested in heat-box conditioning. In all lines, females were impaired. In four of them, performance indices during training and test were close to zero (see supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org). In males, impairment was slightly less, with mutant phenotypes in only four of the six stocks (three shown in Fig. 6b) (U test; T1 males: Z = 6.14, p < 0.001; T2 males: Z = 1.64, p = NS; T3 males: Z = 5.15, p < 0.001; full data in supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org). Expression levels in the three lines tested in Western blots approximately paralleled the behavioral defects (compare Figs. 4B, 6b). Moreover, the effect was dose dependent: A single copy of the transgene (T1) was still effective but suppressed the performance less than two copies (U test; T1/+ males: Z = 2.30, p < 0.05) (Fig. 6c).

T1 was crossed to various S6KII mutants. Df(1)ignΔ58-1/Y; T1/+ males carrying a single dose of S6KII as a transgene showed normal training performance in the range of WT-CS and Df(1)ignΔ58-1 (U test, Z = -0.63, p = NS) (Fig. 6c). This indicates that loss of the endogenous S6KII gene can compensate for the dominant-negative effect of the transgene, suggesting that the latter is attributable to dosage rather than tissue specificity (i.e., a modified expression pattern of the transgene).

Pavlovian olfactory conditioning

Not only place learning in the heat box, but also olfactory discrimination learning (Tully and Quinn, 1985), was affected by mutations in the S6KII gene. Df(1)ignΔ58-1 females and males showed a reduction of ∼40% in 3 min, 30 min, and 3 hr memory compared with WT-CS flies (not separated for gender in Fig. 7, because no sexual differences could be found in any of the data; controls are presented as supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org). Independent of sex, the performance of mutant ignP1 flies in olfactory conditioning was not distinguishable from wild type during the first 3 hr, although memory scores tended to be lower in all experiments. The small-deficiency Df(1)ignΔ24-3 caused 40% reduction in 3 min memory in both genders (Fig. 8 for males) (data not shown for females). Heterozygous Df(1)ignΔ24-3/+ flies performed distinctly better than homozygous mutant flies and were not significantly different from wild type (PI24-3/+ = 61.3 +/- 6.1; n = 6; p < 0.001 for comparison with homozygous Df(1)ignΔ24-3; p = NS for comparison with WT-CS). Hence, the three kinds of mutants had different effects in the two learning paradigms. The full deletion caused a defect only in olfactory but not in place learning. Inversely, ignP1 males were approximately normal in olfactory but impaired in place learning, and, finally, the partial deletion was a recessive mutation in olfactory and a dominant gain-of-function mutation in place learning.

  Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Pavlovian olfactory discrimination learning is impaired in the S6KII null mutant but not significantly in ignP1. The 3 min, 30 min, and 3 hr memory scores are shown. Data for males and females are pooled because no statistically significant difference for gender was found. In the line ignΔ1P1, the P-element is precisely eliminated by jump-out.

  Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Memory defects of the null mutant [Df(1)ignΔ58-1] and the partial deletion [Df(1)ignΔ24-3] in pavlovian olfactory conditioning are shown. The defect in Df(1)ignΔ58-1 is rescued by one copy of the transgene (T1). For technical reasons, only data of males are shown. The error bars indicate SEMs. ***p < 0.001.

Genomic rescue in olfactory conditioning

Homozygous flies of the transgenic line T1 with two extra copies of the gene originally showed significantly better 3 min memory in olfactory conditioning than WT-CS flies (T1/T1: PI = 0.71, n = 6; WT-CS: PI = 0.62, n = 14; p < 0.05). When this experiment was repeated ∼10 generations later, the difference was marginal and not significant (T1/T1: PI = 0.77, n = 6; WT-CS: PI = 0.75, n = 7; p > 0.05) (data pooled in Fig. 8), leaving the possibility that selection on the genetic background (de-Belle and Heisenberg, 1994) meanwhile had abolished the effect. Whether or not this was the case, the result again contrasted the deleterious effect of the transgene on place learning (Fig. 6b,c). Moreover, the olfactory learning defect of the deficiency could be rescued by the transgene. Df(1)ignΔ58-1/Y; T1/+ males learned significantly better than Df(1)ignΔ58-1 flies without the transgene and were indistinguishable from the wild-type controls (Fig. 8). The same was found for the partial deletion Df(1)ignΔ24-3 (supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org).

Discussion

The Drosophila S6KII is, perhaps surprisingly, encoded by a non-essential gene. Null mutants are viable and normal by morphological criteria and casual behavioral observation. Also, brain structure seems not to be severely affected. As the only apparent defect, Df(1)ignΔ58-1 shows a 40% reduced memory score in pavlovian olfactory learning. Regarding this memory component, the properties of the S6KII mutants are in line with what one might expect from S6KII in vertebrates (Bjorbaek et al., 1995; Nebreda and Gavin, 1999). If it is the kinase domain in the N-terminal part of the enzyme that is phosphorylating target proteins, deletion of that domain should abolish phosphorylation. Indeed, in the mutant Df(1)ignΔ24-3, lacking part of the N-terminal kinase domain olfactory memory is reduced as much as in the null mutant [Df(1)ignΔ58-1]. The small and the large deletions are recessive (data for Df(1)ignΔ58-1/+ are shown as supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org), indicating that 50% S6KII enzymatic activity in the heterozygote is sufficient to provide the full function. In the mutant ignP1, again the reduced overall amount of protein is enough to allow for near to normal learning/memory. Also, the transgene in the genetic background of the null mutant or the partial deletion Df(1)ignΔ24-3 (data not shown) provides enough enzyme to fully support the behavioral task. Finally, overexpression of S6KII as a transgene has no deleterious effect. If at all, it enhances memory.

The involvement of the S6KII gene in operant place learning could hardly be more different. In wild-type Drosophila, S6KII is not essential for place learning, because removing the whole gene has no apparent phenotype. An influence of the S6KII gene on heat-box learning is seen only in mutants that leave the gene or part of it intact. The P insertion, the N-terminally truncated gene, and the S6KII transgenes all reduce learning performance. In the P-insertion mutant, the learning defect seems not to be related to the overall amount of S6KII because females learn well and show the same reduced amount of S6KII-like immunoreactivity as males. The heat-box phenotypes need to be studied in detail to find out whether the three kinds of mutations (P insertion, partial deletions, overexpression) interfere with heat-box learning in the same manner. Whether S6KII exerts its various effects in adulthood or during development can now be addressed using conditional expression systems (McGuire et al., 2003; Mao et al., 2004).

The S6KII gene is nested in a large intron of a putative gene (CG17602) of unknown function. It should be noted that the mutant phenotypes cannot be assigned to this nearby gene because the genomic transgenes do not contain CG17602 and are inserted far away from the original genomic location. Their phenotypes correspond to and even interact with those of the deficiency mutants.

Assuming that it is at the protein level in which S6KII exerts its deleterious effect on heat-box learning in the mutants, a smaller protein must be synthesized from the C-terminal part of the S6KII gene in the small-deletion mutants. If this is true for the small deletions, it may well be true also for the wild type. This hypothetical small isoform might be mostly suppressed by the large isoform in wild type. Kinases with a large and a small isoform are not uncommon, e.g., in the protein kinase C family (Sacktor et al., 1993; Drier et al., 2002; Hernandez et al., 2003). It is too early to speculate about mechanisms before a small isoform is found.

Both olfactory learning and place learning require cAMP signaling (Tully and Quinn, 1985; Davis et al., 1995; Wustmann et al., 1996) but have their cAMP-dependent memory traces in different neurons (Zars et al., 2000a,b). Because it is now well established that the memory trace for olfactory learning is localized in sets of intrinsic neurons (Kenyon cells) of the mushroom bodies, it will be of interest to determine, by the same reconstitution approach, whether the molecular functions of S6KII documented here colocalize in the same cells. For operant place learning, the localization of the memory trace is not as clear cut as in olfactory learning because several groups of neurons are still candidate locations for the memory trace. However, using the reconstitution strategy, it can be determined whether the same GAL4 lines that rescue in the case of rutabaga also do so with S6KII-cDNA in Df(1)ignΔ58-1.

Current schemes for vertebrates place cAMP upstream of S6KII in the same signaling pathway (Impey et al., 1999). This may hold true in Drosophila for pavlovian olfactory conditioning, whereas for operant place learning, S6KII may not lie in the direct pathway (because it is dispensable) but rather in a side branch. With the phenotypes of the mutants and overexpression lines, one is inclined to assume that a close interaction partner or a protein similar to S6KII should be directly involved in place learning. For instance, the signaling pathways for the two learning tasks might diverge at the level of the MAPK that could be blocked by an excess of S6KII and, in particular, by the hypothetical small C-terminal isoform of S6KII. Recent studies in Xenopus oocytes already showed that an N-terminally truncated RSK mutant can constitutively interact with ERK (Gavin et al., 1999; Biondi and Nebreda, 2003).

We do not know whether S6KII in Drosophila is, as in vertebrates, part of the Raf-Ras pathway. As mentioned in Introduction, mutant studies on the Drosophila gene leonardo encoding a 14-3-3 protein (Skoulakis and Davis, 1996) suggest a role for Raf/Ras signaling in olfactory learning and memory. Yet, if S6KII would, as shown in vertebrates, exert its function by phosphorylating CREB or some other transcription factors (Impey et al., 1999), it could not be involved directly in synaptic plasticity. The defect in olfactory learning in the S6KII mutants is apparent already within 3 min, much too early to reflect transcriptional control induced by the learning process. This does not prove an involvement in the modulation of synaptic efficacy, though. A role of the Raf-Ras pathway in the formation and decay of synapses has been demonstrated for the Drosophila neuromuscular junction (Koh et al., 2002). This may be the way in which the leonardo gene affects olfactory learning and memory. This gene has been shown to be required during adulthood (Philip et al., 2001). Yet, the rescue experiments using a heat shock GAL4 driver left a time window of several hours between heat induction and memory test, which would be enough to restore, for instance, the abundance of synapses in a mutant with reduced synaptic density.

Our assessment of S6KII function in Drosophila has focused on learning and memory. This does not exclude that the gene might be involved in other important processes as well. Also, future work will have to show whether the differential effects of the mutants on operant and classical conditioning can be generalized to other operant and classical learning tasks. The study, therefore, cannot claim to have shown that operant and classical associative processes require different biochemical mechanisms. Yet, the strikingly different mutant phenotypes of Drosophila S6KII in operant place learning and pavlovian olfactory conditioning may help to address this question.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (SFB 554; Graduiertenkolleg “Arthropodenverhalten”), the Human Frontiers Science Program (RG 0143), and Fonds der Chemischen Industrie. We thank Dr. U. Schaefer for providing the P-element lines, Dr. E. Hafen for the UAS:S6KII transgenic lines, Dr. S. Kramer for contribution to the behavioral screen, R. Wolf for assistance throughout this study, K. Oechsner and H. Kaderschabek for maintenance of the heat box, and in particular E. Dierichs-Schmitt for skillful technical help.

  • The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 USC section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Martin Heisenberg, Lehrstuhl für Genetik und Neurobiologie, Biozentrum, Am Hubland, D97074 Wuerzburg, Germany. E-mail: heisenberg{at}biozentrum.uni-wuerzburg.de.

  • G. Putz's present address: Max-Planck Institute of Molecular Cell Biology and Genetics, Pfotenhauerstrasse 108, D-01307 Dresden, Germany.

  • T. Zars's present address: Division of Biological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211.

  • Copyright © 2004 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/04/249745-07$15.00/0

References

  1. ↵
    Ballif BA, Blenis J (2001) Molecular mechanisms mediating mammalian mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) kinase (MEK)-MAPK cell survival signals. Cell Growth Differ 12: 397-408.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    Biondi RM, Nebreda AR (2003) Signalling specificity of Ser/Thr protein kinases through docking-site-mediated interactions. Biochem J 372: 1-13.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Bjorbaek C, Zhao Y, Moller DE (1995) Divergent functional roles for p90rsk kinase domains. J Biol Chem 270: 18848-18852.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    Davis RL, Cherry J, Dauwalder B, Han PL, Skoulakis E (1995) The cyclic AMP system and Drosophila learning. Mol Cell Biochem 149-150: 271-278.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    de-Belle JS, Heisenberg M (1994) Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. Science 263: 692-695.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    De Cesare D, Jacquot S, Hanauer A, Sassone-Corsi P (1998) Rsk-2 activity is necessary for epidermal growth factor-induced phosphorylation of CREB protein and transcription of c-fos gene. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 12202-12207.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Drier EA, Tello MK, Cowan M, Wu P, Blace N, Sacktor TC, Yin JC (2002) Memory enhancement and formation by atypical PKM activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat Neurosci 5: 316-324.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Dufresne SD, Bjorbaek C, El-Haschimi K, Zhao Y, Aschenbach WG, Moller DE, Goodyear LJ (2001) Altered extracellular signal-regulated kinase signaling and glycogen metabolism in skeletal muscle from p90 ribosomal S6 kinase 2 knockout mice. Mol Cell Biol 21: 81-87.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Engels WR, Johnson-Schlitz DM, Eggleston WB, Sved J (1990) High-frequency P element loss in Drosophila is homolog dependent. Cell 62: 515-525.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    English JD, Sweatt JD (1997) A requirement for the mitogen-activated protein kinase cascade in hippocampal long term potentiation. J Biol Chem 272: 19103-19106.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    Frodin M, Gammeltoft S (1999) Role and regulation of 90 kDa ribosomal S6 kinase (RSK) in signal transduction. Mol Cell Endocrinol 151: 65-77.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Gavin AC, Ni Ainle A, Chierici E, Jones M, Nebreda AR (1999) A p90(rsk) mutant constitutively interacting with MAP kinase uncouples MAP kinase from p34(cdc2)/cyclin B activation in Xenopus oocytes. Mol Biol Cell 10: 2971-2986.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    Guo A, Li L, Xia SZ, Feng CH, Wolf R, Heisenberg M (1996) Conditioned visual flight orientation in Drosophila: dependence on age, practice, and diet. Learn Mem 3: 49-59.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    Hernandez AI, Blace N, Crary JF, Serrano PA, Leitges M, Libien JM, Weinstein G, Tcherapanov A, Sacktor TC (2003) Protein kinase M zeta synthesis from a brain mRNA encoding an independent protein kinase C zeta catalytic domain. Implications for the molecular mechanism of memory. J Biol Chem 278: 40305-40316.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    Hoskins RA, Nelson CR, Berman BP, Laverty TR, George RA, Ciesiolka L, Naeemuddin M, Arenson AD, Durbin J, David RG, Tabor PE, Bailey MR, DeShazo DR, Catanese J, Mammoser A, Osoegawa K, de Jong PJ, Celniker SE, Gibbs RA, Rubin GM, Scherer SE (2000) A BAC-based physical map of the major autosomes of Drosophila melanogaster. Science 287: 2271-2274.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    Impey S, Obrietan K, Wong ST, Poser S, Yano S, Wayman G, Deloulme JC, Chan G, Storm DR (1998) Cross talk between ERK and PKA is required for Ca2+ stimulation of CREB-dependent transcription and ERK nuclear translocation. Neuron 21: 869-883.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Impey S, Obrietan K, Storm DR (1999) Making new connections: role of ERK/MAP kinase signaling in neuronal plasticity. Neuron 23: 11-14.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Klemenz R, Weber U, Gehring WJ (1987) The white gene as a marker in a new P-element vector for gene transfer in Drosophila. Nucleic Acids Res 15: 3947-3959.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Koh YH, Ruiz-Canada C, Gorczyca M, Budnik V (2002) The Ras1-mitogen-activated protein kinase signal transduction pathway regulates synaptic plasticity through fasciclin II-mediated cell adhesion. J Neurosci 22: 2496-2504.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    Lowry B, Miller JR, Fraser FC (1971) A new dominant gene mental retardation syndrome. Association with small stature, tapering fingers, characteristic facies, and possible hydrocephalus. Am J Dis Child 121: 496-500.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Mao Z, Roman G, Zong L, Davis RL (2004) Pharmacogenetic rescue in time and space of the rutabaga memory impairment by using Gene-Switch. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 198-203.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    Martin KC, Michael D, Rose JC, Barad M, Casadio A, Zhu H, Kandel ER (1997) MAP kinase translocates into the nucleus of the presynaptic cell and is required for long-term facilitation in Aplysia. Neuron 18: 899-912.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    McGuire SE, Le PT, Osborn AJ, Matsumoto K, Davis RL (2003) Spatiotemporal rescue of memory dysfunction in Drosophila. Science 302: 1765-1768.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    Nebreda AR, Gavin AC (1999) Perspectives: signal transduction. Cell survival demands some Rsk. Science 286: 1309-1310.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Orban PC, Chapman PF, Brambilla R (1999) Is the Ras-MAPK signalling pathway necessary for long-term memory formation? Trends Neurosci 22: 38-44.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Philip N, Acevedo SF, Skoulakis EM (2001) Conditional rescue of olfactory learning and memory defects in mutants of the 14-3-3zeta gene leonardo. J Neurosci 21: 8417-8425.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    Putz G, Heisenberg M (2002) Memories in Drosophila heat-box learning. Learn Mem 9: 349-359.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Rintelen F, Stocker H, Thomas G, Hafen E (2001) PDK1 regulates growth through Akt and S6K in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 15020-15025.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    Robertson HM, Preston CR, Phillis RW, Johnson-Schlitz DM, Benz WK, Engels WR (1988) A stable genomic source of P element transposase in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 118: 461-470.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    Sacktor TC, Osten P, Valsamis H, Jiang X, Naik MU, Sublette E (1993) Persistent activation of the zeta isoform of protein kinase C in the maintenance of long-term potentiation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90: 8342-8346.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    Sgambato V, Pages C, Rogard M, Besson MJ, Caboche J (1998) Extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) controls immediate early gene induction on corticostriatal stimulation. J Neurosci 18: 8814-8825.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    Skoulakis EM, Davis RL (1996) Olfactory learning deficits in mutants for leonardo, a Drosophila gene encoding a 14-3-3 protein. Neuron 17: 931-944.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Stapleton M, Carlson J, Brokstein P, Yu C, Champe M, George R, Guarin H, Kronmiller B, Pacleb J, Park S, Wan K, Rubin GM, Celniker SE (2002) A Drosophila full-length cDNA resource. Genome Biol 3: RESEARCH0080.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  34. ↵
    Trivier E, De Cesare D, Jacquot S, Pannetier S, Zackai E, Young I, Mandel JL, Sassone-Corsi P, Hanauer A (1996) Mutations in the kinase Rsk-2 associated with Coffin-Lowry syndrome. Nature 384: 567-570.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Tully T, Quinn WG (1985) Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol [A] 157: 263-277.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Wassarman DA, Solomon NM, Rubin GM (1994) The Drosophila melanogaster ribosomal S6 kinase II-encoding sequence. Gene 144: 309-310.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Wustmann G, Rein K, Wolf R, Heisenberg M (1996) A jew paradigm for operant conditioning of Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol [A] 179: 429-436.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. ↵
    Xing J, Ginty DD, Greenberg ME (1996) Coupling of the RAS-MAPK pathway to gene activation by RSK2, a growth factor-regulated CREB kinase. Science 273: 959-963.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    Young ID (1988) The Coffin-Lowry syndrome. J Med Genet 25: 344-348.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    Zars T (2001) Two thermosensors in Drosophila have different behavioral functions. J Comp Physiol [A] 187: 235-242.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Zars T, Fischer M, Schulz R, Heisenberg M (2000a) Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. Science 288: 672-675.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    Zars T, Wolf R, Davis R, Heisenberg M (2000b) Tissue-specific expression of a type I adenylyl cyclase rescues the rutabaga mutant memory defect: in search of the engram [In Process Citation]. Learn Mem 7: 18-31.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 24 (44)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 24, Issue 44
3 Nov 2004
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The S6KII (rsk) Gene of Drosophila melanogaster Differentially Affects an Operant and a Classical Learning Task
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
The S6KII (rsk) Gene of Drosophila melanogaster Differentially Affects an Operant and a Classical Learning Task
Gabriele Putz, Franco Bertolucci, Thomas Raabe, Troy Zars, Martin Heisenberg
Journal of Neuroscience 3 November 2004, 24 (44) 9745-9751; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3211-04.2004

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
The S6KII (rsk) Gene of Drosophila melanogaster Differentially Affects an Operant and a Classical Learning Task
Gabriele Putz, Franco Bertolucci, Thomas Raabe, Troy Zars, Martin Heisenberg
Journal of Neuroscience 3 November 2004, 24 (44) 9745-9751; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3211-04.2004
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
  • Generation of Intensity Selectivity by Differential Synaptic Tuning: Fast-Saturating Excitation But Slow-Saturating Inhibition
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.