Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Cellular/Molecular

Developmental Switch in Synaptic Mechanisms of Hippocampal Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor-Dependent Long-Term Depression

Elena D. Nosyreva and Kimberly M. Huber
Journal of Neuroscience 16 March 2005, 25 (11) 2992-3001; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3652-04.2005
Elena D. Nosyreva
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kimberly M. Huber
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The presynaptic and postsynaptic properties of synapses change over the course of postnatal development. Therefore, synaptic plasticity mechanisms would be expected to adapt to these changes to facilitate alterations of synaptic strength throughout ontogeny. Here, we identified developmental changes in long-term depression (LTD) mediated by group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) and dendritic protein synthesis in hippocampal CA1 slices (mGluR-LTD). In slices prepared from adolescent rats [postnatal day 21 (P21) to P35], mGluR activation induces LTD and a long-term decrease in AMPA receptor (AMPAR) surface expression, both of which require protein synthesis. In neonatal animals (P8-P15), mGluR-LTD is independent of protein synthesis and is not associated with changes in the surface expression of AMPARs. Instead, mGluR-LTD at neonatal synapses results in large decreases in presynaptic function, measured by changes in paired-pulse facilitation and the rate of blockade by the use-dependent NMDA receptor blocker (+)-5-methyl-10,11-dihydro-5H-dibenzo[a,d]cyclohepten-5,10-imine maleate. Conversely, mGluR-LTD at mature synapses results in little or no change in presynaptic function, suggesting a postsynaptic mechanism of expression. The developmental switch in the synaptic mechanisms of LTD would differentially affect synapse dynamics and perhaps information processing over the course of postnatal development.

  • metabotropic glutamate receptor
  • long-term depression
  • protein synthesis
  • AMPA receptor endocytosis
  • hippocampus
  • CA1

Introduction

There is growing evidence that both the presynaptic and postsynaptic properties of synapses change over the course of postnatal development. Nascent synapses are characterized by high presynaptic release probability and few postsynaptic AMPA receptors (AMPARs). Synapse maturation is accompanied by the acquisition of AMPARs and decreases in neurotransmitter release probability (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1995; Pouzat and Hestrin, 1997; Liao et al., 1999; Petralia et al., 1999; Reyes and Sakmann, 1999; Pickard et al., 2000; Wasling et al., 2004) (but see Dumas and Foster, 1995; Hsia et al., 1998). For developing synapses to remain plastic over the course of synapse maturation, plasticity mechanisms must adapt with these changing synaptic properties. Indeed, there is evidence for changes in the mechanisms of longterm potentiation during synapse development (Yasuda et al., 2003). Here, we demonstrate that both the synaptic and molecular mechanisms of synaptic long-term depression (LTD) change over the course of synapse maturation in area CA1 of the rat hippocampus. Specifically, we examined the developmental changes of LTD, which rely on group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) and dendritic protein synthesis (mGluR-LTD) (Huber et al., 2000). This work was motivated by findings that group 1 mGluR function (measured as phosphoinositide turnover) and synaptic polyribosome number peak during the period of synapse formation and maturation [approximately postnatal day 7 (P7) to P15 in the hippocampus], suggesting that mGluR-LTD plays a significant role in the plasticity of developing synapses (Steward and Falk, 1985, 1991; Nicoletti et al., 1986; Dudek et al., 1989; Palmer et al., 1990; Casabona et al., 1997).

The developmental regulation and site of expression of mGluR-LTD in the hippocampus has been controversial. Some studies suggest that mGluR-LTD is only observed at early developmental time periods in area CA1 (P7-P11) (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Overstreet et al., 1997). However, others indicate that mGluR-LTD is restricted to adult animals (Kemp et al., 2000). There is general agreement that mGluR-LTD is induced postsynaptically (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Oliet et al., 1997; Watabe et al., 2002). However, many studies of mGluR-LTD of immature neurons (P4-P11) conclude that mGluR-LTD is expressed presynaptically using both electrophysiological and optical methods (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Oliet et al., 1997; Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Zakharenko et al., 2002; Feinmark et al., 2003; Rammes et al., 2003).

Evidence for a postsynaptic mechanism of mGluR-LTD expression comes from studies demonstrating that the selective group 1 mGluR agonist R,S-dihydroxyphenylglycine (DHPG), which induces LTD, results in endocytosis and a persistent decrease in postsynaptic AMPARs and NMDA receptors (NMDARs), which requires protein synthesis (Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001). DHPG-induced LTD is blocked by the postsynaptic injection of endocytosis and protein synthesis inhibitors (Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001), suggesting a postsynaptic mechanism of expression.

Here, we find a developmental switch in the protein synthesis dependence and synaptic locus of mGluR-LTD, which occurs between the second and fourth postnatal week. This switch may occur to accommodate the changing properties of synapses and may have consequences for information processing over the course of postnatal development.

Materials and Methods

Drugs. d,l-AP-5, (+)-5-methyl-10,11-dihydro-5H-dibenzo[a,d]cyclohepten5,10-imine maleate (MK801) (Tocris Cookson, Ellisville, MO), anisomycin, and picrotoxin (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) were prepared fresh in artificial CSF (ACSF). The cannabinoid 1 receptor (CB1R) antagonist 1-(2,4-dichlorophenyl)-5-(4-iodophenyl)-4-methyl-N-4-morpholinyl-1 H-pyrazole-3-carboxamide (AM281) and R-(+)-[2,3-dihydro-5-methyl-3-[(morpholinyl)methyl]pyrrolo[1,2,3-DE]-1,4-benzoxazin-6-yl]-(1-naphtmalenyl)methanone mesylate (WIN55,212-2) (Tocris Cookson) were prepared as a 1000× stocks in DMSO, used fresh or kept as stocks at -20°C, and diluted to final concentration in ACSF. Slices were preincubated in antagonists or inhibitors for 20-30 min before DHPG or paired-pulse low-frequency stimulation (PP-LFS). The effects of all of the pharmacological treatments on LTD were evaluated by comparing interleaved control and treated slices.

Electrophysiology. Long-Evans hooded rats were obtained from Charles River Laboratories (Wilmington, MA). Hippocampal slices (400 μm) were prepared from 8- to 35-d-old rats. Rats were anesthetized with the barbiturate pentobarbital (50 mg/kg) and decapitated soon after the disappearance of corneal reflexes. The brain was removed and dissected and then sliced using a vibratome (VT 1000S; Leica, Nussloch, Germany) in ice-cold dissection buffer containing the following (in mm): 2.6 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 0.5 CaCl2, 5 MgCl2, 212 sucrose, and 10 dextrose. Area CA3 was surgically removed from each slice immediately after sectioning. The slices were transferred into a reservoir chamber filled with ACSF containing the following (in mm): 124 NaCl, 5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, and 10 dextrose. Slices were allowed to recover for 2-5 h at 30°C. ACSF and dissection buffer were equilibrated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2.

For recording, slices were transferred to a submerged recording chamber, maintained at 30°C, and perfused continuously with ASCF at a rate of 2-3 ml/min. Field potentials (FPs) were recorded with extracellular recording electrodes (1 MΩ) filled with ACSF and placed in stratum radiatum of area CA1. FPs were evoked by monophasic stimulation (duration, 200 μs) of Schaffer collateral/commissural afferents with a concentric bipolar tungsten stimulating electrode (Frederick Haer Company, Bowdoinham, ME). NMDA receptor-mediated FPs were isolated in a modified ASCF containing the following (in mm): 3 CaCl2, 0.1 MgCl2, 2.5 KCl, 0.02 DNQX, 0.001 glycine, and 0.1 picrotoxin. Stable baseline responses were collected every 30 s using a stimulation intensity (10-30 μA), yielding 50-60% of the maximal response. FPs were filtered at 2 kHz, acquired, and digitized at 10 kHz on a personal computer using custom software (LabVIEW; National Instruments, Austin, TX). The initial slope of the FP was used to measure the stability of synaptic responses and quantify the magnitude of LTD. mGluR-LTD was induced by pairs of stimuli (interstimulus interval, 50 ms) delivered at 1 Hz for 15 min (1800 pulses; PP-LFS), by stimulation delivered at 5 Hz for 3 min (900 pulses; 5 Hz), or by application of 100 μm DHPG (Tocris Cookson) for 5 min. For paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) experiments, pairs of presynaptic stimulation (interpulse interval, 50 ms) were delivered every 30 s throughout the experiment.

The group data were analyzed as follows: (1) the initial slopes of the FP were expressed as percentages of the preconditioning or DHPG baseline average; (2) the time scale in each experiment was converted to time from the onset of conditioning or DHPG; and (3) the time-matched, normalized data were averaged across experiments and expressed as means ± SEM. The PPF ratio was obtained by dividing the initial field potential slope from the second pulse (FP2) by that of the first pulse (FP1) and then normalizing to the pre-DHPG baseline PPF value. For example, a 20% increase in PPF represents a change in the PPF or FP2/FP1 ratio from 1.6 to 1.92. Time constants (τ) of the rate of MK801 blockade of NMDAR FPs were determined by fitting the decay of the maximum amplitude of NMDAR FPs in MK801 with a double exponential using Origin analysis software (Microcal Software, Northampton, MA). Significant differences were determined by a Student's independent or paired t test (if indicated). p < 0.05 was considered to represent significant differences. For correlation analysis, correlation coefficients were determined, and z tests were performed to determine p values using StatView software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

Biochemical measurements of surface-expressed AMPA receptors. Biotinylation experiments were performed as described previously (Chung et al., 2000; Heynen et al., 2003). Hippocampal slices were prepared as described for electrophysiology experiments. After a 2-3 h recovery period in ACSF, slices were treated with DHPG (5 min), NMDA (3 min), or ACSF (control). DHPG experiments were performed in the presence of 100 μm d,l-AP-5. From each rat, two to three slices were pooled together for one condition. Slices were placed on ice to stop endocytosis and then were washed with ice-cold ACSF and incubated in ACSF containing 1 mg/ml sulfo-NHS-LC-biotin (Pierce, Rockford, IL) for 10 min on ice. To quench the biotin reaction, slices were washed three times with Trisbuffered saline and homogenized in a modified radioimmunoprecipitation assay (RIPA) buffer containing 50 mm Tris-HCl, pH 7.4, 1% Triton X-100, 0.1% SDS, 0.5% Na-deoxycholate, 150 mm NaCl, 2 mm EDTA, 50 mm NaH2PO4, 50 mm NaF, 10 mm Na4P2O7, 1 mm Na3VO4, and Protease Inhibitor Cocktail III (Calbiochem, La Jolla, CA). The homogenates were centrifuged at 14,000 × g for 10 min at 4°C. Protein concentrations were measured with a BCA Protein Assay (Pierce). Protein (15 μg) was removed for total protein measurements. Protein (150 μg) was then mixed with 150 μl of UltraLink immobilized NeutrAvidin beads (Pierce) by rotating for 2 h at 4°C. The beads were washed with 10 vol of RIPA buffer and then eluted with SDS-PAGE sample buffer supplemented with 50 mm dithiothreitol for 20 min at 90°C. Both total and biotinylated proteins were resolved by SDS-PAGE, transferred to nitrocellulose membranes, and probed with anti-GluR1 C-terminal antibody (1:5000; Upstate Biotechnology, Lake Placid, NY), anti-GluR2/3 N-terminal antibody (1:1000; Chemicon, Temecula, CA) or anti-α-actin antibody (1:1000; Chemicon), or anti-GluR6/7 (1:5000; Upstate Biotechnology). Immunoreactive bands were visualized by enhanced chemiluminescence, captured on autoradiography film (Eastman Kodak, Rochester, NY). Digital images were produced by densitometric scans of autoradiographs on a ScanJet 4300C (Hewlett Packard, Palo Alto, CA) and quantified using Scion (Frederick, MD) Image software. The surface/total protein ratio was calculated for each condition. When duplicate conditions were performed within one animal, the ratio values were averaged to obtain an animal average for that condition. Therefore, the n values for the biotinylation experiments (see Figs. 4, 5, 6) represent the number of rats, as opposed to slices. Significant differences between surface/total ratios of treated slices and within-animal control slices were determined using a paired t test (for ratio and p values, see Table 1). Although the raw ratio values were used for statistical comparisons, the group data are presented in Figures 4, 5, 6 as a percentage of condition control to compare across different treatment conditions.

  Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

DHPG treatment of acute hippocampal slices from adolescent rats results in a protein synthesis-dependent decrease in AMPAR surface expression. A-F, Sample Western blots of total (T) and surface (S) GluR2/3 or GluR1 subunits of the AMPA receptor. Quantitative data of the ratio of surface to total GluR2/3 or GluR1 in hippocampal slices taken either 15 or 60 min after DHPG application (100 μm; 5 min). The number of experiments per group is indicated on each bar. *p < 0.05. A, B, DHPG treatment of hippocampal slices prepared from adolescent rats results in a long-term decrease of GluR2/3 and GluR1 surface expression using receptor biotinylation. C, D, Preincubation in the mGluR antagonist LY341495 (100 μm) blocks DHPG-induced decreases in GluR2/3 and GluR1 surface expression. Quantitative data of the ratio of surface to total GluR2/3 or GluR1 in hippocampal slices taken 15 min after DHPG application (100 μm; 5 min) in the presence of LY341495. E, F, Preincubation of slices in the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (20 μm) specifically blocks the late (60 min) decrease in AMPAR surface expression induced by DHPG. Error bars represent SEM.

  Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

DHPG does not affect the surface expression of kainate receptors in adolescent rats. A, Sample Western blot of total (T) and surface (S) GluR6/7. Quantitative data of the ratio of surface to total GluR6/7 in hippocampal slices taken either 15 or 60 min after DHPG application (100 μm; 5 min). The number of experiments per group is indicated on each bar. B, Sample Western blot of total actin and actin “pulled down” (P) with avidin beads, demonstrating that intracellular proteins are not biotinylated in this assay. Error bars represent SEM.

  Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

NMDAR activation, but not mGluR activation, reduces AMPAR surface expression in neonatal rat slices. A, Sample Western blot of total (T) and surface (S) GluR2/3. Quantitative data of the ratio of surface to total GluR2/3 in hippocampal slices taken either 15 or 60 min after DHPG application (100 μm; 5 min). The number of experiments per group is indicated on each bar. *p < 0.05. B, Sample Western blot of GluR1 and quantitative data of the same samples as those used in A. C, Sample Western blot and quantitative data of the ratio of surface to total GluR2/3 in hippocampal slices taken either 10 or 60 min after NMDA application (20 μm; 3 min). D, Sample Western blot of GluR1 and quantitative data of the same samples as those used in C. Error bars represent SEM.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Raw ratio values from surface biotinylation experiments

Results

mGluR-LTD can be induced at immature synapses

We first determined whether mGluR-LTD can be induced pharmacologically and synaptically at immature synapses. Hippocampal slices were prepared from neonatal (P8-P15) or adolescent (P21-P35) rats. Extracellular field potential recordings were obtained in area CA1 and were elicited by Schaffer collateral stimulation. LTD was induced using either the group 1 mGluR agonist DHPG (100 μm; 5 min) or synaptic stimulation (900 pairs of stimulation pulses with a 50 ms interval delivered at 1 Hz; PP-LFS). Because the NMDAR-dependent form of LTD is robust at this developmental age (Dudek and Bear, 1993), all LTD experiments presented in this study were performed in the presence of the NMDAR antagonist d,l-AP-5 (100 μm). We find that LTD can be induced in slices of neonatal rats with DHPG or PP-LFS in the presence of AP-5 (Fig. 1). Although group 1 mGluR function peaks during the early developmental period, DHPG-induced LTD is not enhanced at immature synapses (83 ± 3% of pre-DHPG baseline; n = 10) compared with LTD in slices from adolescent rats (84 ± 4%; n = 7) (Fig. 1A). In contrast, LTD induced with PP-LFS is enhanced in slices from P8-P15 rats (63 ± 4%; n = 6) compared with P21-P35 rats (84 ± 3%; n = 7; p < 0.02) (Fig. 1B). Pretreatment of neonatal slices for 5 min in the broad mGluR antagonist (2S)-2-amino-2-[(1S,2S)-2-carboxycycloprop-1-yl]-3-(xanth-9-yl) propanoic acid (LY341495) (100 μm) (Fitzjohn et al., 1998) abolished DHPG-induced LTD (97 ± 2%; n = 6). In a subset of experiments, DHPG was applied after LY341495 washout and induced LTD (79 ± 4%; n = 3) (Fig. 1C). Preincubation (20-30 min) in LY341495 significantly reduced PP-LFS-induced LTD (92 ± 5%; n = 7) compared with interleaved control slices (68 ± 6%; n = 11; p = 0.004) (Fig. 1C). Although there was a slight residual depression observed with PP-LFS in LY341495, it was not significant compared with baseline values (p = 0.14). Our results indicate that an mGluR-dependent LTD can be induced in immature synapses by synaptic stimulation (PP-LFS) or DHPG.

  Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Chemically and synaptically induced mGluR-dependent LTD in neonatal hippocampus. All of the experiments shown in the figures were performed in the NMDA receptor antagonist d,l-AP-5 (100 μm). A-D, Plotted are FP slopes (mean ± SEM) as a function of time from onset of DHPG or conditioning stimulation. A, DHPG (100 μm; 5 min; arrow) induces LTD of FP slope values, which is similar in magnitude in neonatal (P8-P15) and adolescent (P21-P35) rats. B, LTD induced with PP-LFS is enhanced in slices from neonatal rats compared with adolescent rats. C, DHPG-LTD in neonatal rats is blocked by the broad mGluR antagonist LY341495 (100 μm). D, PP-LFS-induced LTD in neonatal rats is inhibited by LY341495 (100 μm). Representative FPs (average, 1 min) are shown for each experiment at the times indicated by the numbers on the graph. Calibration: 0.5 mV, 5 ms.

Developmental switch in the protein synthesis dependence of mGluR-LTD

mGluR-LTD in adolescent rats (P21-P35) is dependent on dendritic protein synthesis and is independent of transcription (Huber et al., 2000). Because numerous synaptic polyribosomes are observed at developing synapses (Steward and Falk, 1985, 1991), we hypothesized that mGluR-LTD at neonatal synapses would also be protein synthesis dependent. Surprisingly, we find that mGluR-LTD in neonatal rats is not sensitive to the protein synthesis inhibitors (Fig. 2). In neonatal rat slices, DHPG induced significant LTD in slices pretreated with anisomycin (88 ± 2%; n = 10) (Fig. 2A), which was not different from LTD in interleaved control slices (83 ± 3%; n = 10; p = 0.21). The control DHPG-LTD values are the same data as those presented in Figure 1A and were replotted in Figure 2A for comparison with anisomycin-treated slices. In contrast, as reported previously, DHPG-induced LTD in adolescent rats was completely inhibited by anisomycin (100 ± 5%, n = 6; interleaved control slices, 84 ± 3%, n = 7; p = 0.01) (Fig. 2C). Similar results were observed with another protein synthesis inhibitor, cycloheximide (60 μm). DHPG-LTD was completely blocked by cycloheximide in adolescent rats (103 ± 2%, n = 4; control slices, 77 ± 5%, n = 4; p = 0.01) (Fig. 2D) and was unaffected in neonatal rats (80 ± 3%, n = 6; control slices, 81 ± 3%, n = 5; p = 0.79) (Fig. 2C). Likewise, LTD induced with synaptic stimulation (PP-LFS) only requires protein synthesis at mature synapses (P8-P15: control, 63 ± 4%, n = 6; anisomycin, 60 ± 5%, n = 4; P21-P35: control, 77 ± 3%, n = 18; anisomycin, 99 ± 4%, n = 16; p = 0.0001) (Fig. 3A,B). PP-LFS-induced LTD in neonatal rats was also insensitive to cycloheximide (62 ± 6%, n = 8; control slices, 64 ± 2%, n = 7; p = 0.01) (Fig. 3C) in contrast to adolescent rats (Huber et al., 2000).

  Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

The protein synthesis dependence of chemically induced mGluR-LTD is developmentally regulated. A, C, DHPG-induced LTD in slices from neonatal rats is insensitive to the protein synthesis inhibitors anisomycin (20 μm; A) or cycloheximide (60 μm; C). B, D, In contrast, anisomycin (B) and cycloheximide (D) block DHPG-LTD in adolescent rats.

  Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

The protein synthesis dependence of synaptically induced mGluR-LTD is developmentally regulated. A, C, LTD induced with synaptic stimulation (PP-LFS) in neonatal rats is not affected by anisomycin (A) or cycloheximide (C). B, PP-LFS-induced LTD in slices from adolescent rats is blocked by anisomycin. D, The ability of anisomycin to block PP-LFS-induced LTD is significantly correlated with the postnatal age of the rats.

Previous studies of mGluR-LTD in neonatal hippocampus have used 5 Hz synaptic stimulation (3 min) (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Bolshakov et al., 2000; Zakharenko et al., 2002). Therefore, we also examined the protein synthesis dependence of mGluR-LTD using this protocol. As in LTD induced with DHPG and PP-LFS at this age, 5 Hz-induced LTD was also insensitive to anisomycin (P8-P15: interleaved controls, 81 ± 5%, n = 7; anisomycin, 78 ± 4%, n = 7; p = 0.89).

These results indicate that, as synapses mature, mGluR-LTD becomes increasingly dependent on protein synthesis. In support of this idea, the ability of anisomycin to block PP-LFS-induced LTD is significantly correlated with postnatal age (r = 0.53; p < 0.002) (Fig. 3D).

Developmental changes in mGluR-induced decreases in AMPAR surface expression

In dissociated cultured neurons, activation of group 1 mGluRs results in a long-term decrease in the surface expression of AMPARs and NMDARs, which requires protein synthesis (Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001). These results indicate that the new proteins required for mGluR-LTD most likely function to control AMPAR endocytosis or trafficking. Therefore, mGluR-LTD at immature synapses may not be mediated by AMPAR internalization or there may be sufficient levels of protein(s) to maintain internalization in the absence of new synthesis.

To determine whether mGluR-dependent AMPAR endocytosis was developmentally regulated, we measured DHPG-induced changes in surface expression of AMPAR subunits GluR1 and GluR2/3. Previous studies of DHPG-induced AMPAR endocytosis have been performed in dissociated cultured hippocampal neurons (Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001). To correlate the developmental changes in mGluR-LTD with DHPG-induced AMPAR endocytosis in the same preparation, we performed these experiments in acute hippocampal slices using receptor biotinylation. We first verified that we could observe decreases in AMPAR surface expression in acute slices from adolescent rats. Slices were incubated in control ACSF or treated with DHPG (100 μm; 5 min). Surface receptors were labeled with biotin either 15 or 60 min after application of DHPG. Biotinylated receptors were precipitated, and the ratio of surface to total GluRs was determined by quantitative Western blotting. The biochemical analysis confirmed that DHPG treatment of hippocampal slices (P21-P35) results in a long-term (60 min) decrease in biotinylated (surface)/total ratio of GluR2/3 (15 min after DHPG, 81 ± 6% of control slices, p = 0.03, n = 7; 60 min after DHPG, 74 ± 6% of control slices, n = 6, p = 0.004) (Fig. 4A) and GluR1 (15 min after DHPG, 81 ± 4%, n = 9, p = 0.001; 60 min after DHPG, 80 ± 5%, n = 6, p = 0.018) (Fig. 4B; for raw ratio values, see Table 1). DHPG did not affect the total levels of GluR2/3 (15 min, 99 ± 3% of control, p = 0.87; 60 min, 103 ± 12%, p = 0.96) or GluR1 (15 min, 106 ± 13%, p = 0.4; 60 min, 101 ± 22%, p = 0.78). As in mGluR-LTD, the DHPG-induced decreases in surface expression were blocked by the broad-range mGluR antagonist LY341495 (100 μm; GluR1, 15 min after DHPG, 113 ± 12%, n = 6, p = 0.39; GluR2/3, 15 min after DHPG, 113 ± 3%, n = 5, p = 0.02) (Fig. 4C,D).

As observed previously in neuronal culture, DHPG-induced decreases in AMPAR surface expression in hippocampal slices were sensitive to the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (20 μm). Slices preincubated in anisomycin displayed significantly reduced AMPAR surface expression at 15 min after DHPG treatment but not at 60 min after DHPG treatment (GluR2/3, 15 min after DHPG, 84 ± 6%, n = 8, p = 0.036; 60 min after DHPG, 102 ± 5%, n = 7, p = 0.97; GluR1, 15 min after DHPG, 84 ± 1%, n = 7, p = 0.0003; 60 min after DHPG, 94 ± 10%, p = 0.55, n = 9) (Fig. 4E,F).

To determine whether the effects of DHPG are specific to the AMPA subtype of receptors, we measured changes in surface expression of kainate receptor subunits GluR6 and GluR7. In area CA1, kainate receptors are expressed primarily presynaptically on both excitatory and inhibitory synapses and postsynaptically on interneurons (Huettner, 2003). DHPG did not affect the surface expression of GluR6/7 (15 min after DHPG, 100 ± 10%, p = 0.77, n = 6; 60 min after DHPG, 93 ± 5%, p = 0.58, n = 7) (Fig. 5A), indicating that DHPG causes a specific reduction in AMPAR surface expression. Additional control experiments confirmed that intracellular proteins, such as actin, were not biotinylated in this assay (n = 3) (Fig. 5B). Together, these experiments confirm that DHPG causes an mGluR- and protein synthesis-dependent long-term decrease in AMPAR surface expression in acute slices from adolescent rats.

In contrast to adolescent rats, DHPG treatment of slices from neonatal animals did not decrease the surface expression of GluR2/3 and GluR1 subunits (GluR2/3: 15 min after DHPG, 98 ± 5%, n = 10, p = 0.8; 60 min after DHPG, 104 ± 5%, n = 12, p = 0.69; GluR1: 15 min DHPG, 100 ± 4%, n = 9; 60 min after DHPG, 111 ± 4%, p = 0.003, n = 10) (Fig. 6A,B).

In addition to mGluRs, chemical activation of NMDARs induces LTD and internalization of AMPARs (Lee et al., 1998; Colledge et al., 2003). We performed additional experiments, treating neonatal slices with NMDA to determine whether there is a general deficit in activity-induced AMPAR internalization in neonatal slices or whether the deficit is specific for the mGluR pathway. We first confirmed that NMDA (20 μm; 3 min) induced LTD in CA1 of neonatal hippocampal slices (80 ± 3% of baseline at 55-60 min after NMDA application; n = 5). Unlike DHPG treatment of neonatal slices, we observed decreases in surface expression of GluR1 and GluR2/3 at 10 min (GluR1, 82 ± 2% of control slices; n = 6, p < 0.001; GluR2/3, 74 ± 6%, n = 5, p = 0.03) and 60 min (GluR1, 75 ± 5%, n = 5, p = 0.008; GluR2/3, 89 ± 2%, n = 4, p = 0.02) after NMDA (20 μm; 3 min) application (Fig. 6C,D; Table 1). These data demonstrate that mGluR-induced, but not NMDA-induced, decreases in AMPAR surface expression are developmentally regulated and do not occur at immature synapses. This finding also suggests that mGluR-LTD at this age is not mediated by a decrease in postsynaptic receptor number.

Presynaptic changes accompany mGluR-LTD at neonatal synapses

Previous studies of mGluR-LTD in slices from young (P4-P18) rats show strong evidence that LTD expression is mediated by a long-term decrease in presynaptic function (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Zakharenko et al., 2002; Rammes et al., 2003). Therefore, based on these studies and our findings that mGluR-dependent decreases in AMPAR surface expression are only observed in adolescent rats, we propose that there is a developmental change in the synaptic mechanisms of mGluR-LTD expression. To test this idea, we used two parameters to measure changes in presynaptic release probability during LTD, PPF, and the rate of blockade of NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic responses by MK801. These parameters were measured after DHPG-induced LTD in slices from both neonatal (P8-P15) and adolescent (P21-P35) rats. DHPG-induced LTD was examined, as opposed to synaptically induced LTD, because a similar magnitude of DHPG-induced LTD is observed across these developmental ages (Figs. 1A, 7B) and would allow comparison of the degree of PPF changes or rate of MK801 blockade across development.

  Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

mGluR-LTD in neonatal rats is associated with changes in presynaptic function. A, Representative FPs elicited by paired-pulse stimulation in P8-P15 and P21-P35 hippocampal slices during baseline (1) and after 50 min of DHPG onset (2). Calibration: 0.5 mV, 20 ms. DHPG trace (2) was scaled to baseline FP1 amplitude for comparison of PPF changes within a single experiment. Note that, in the P8 rat, the second response is facilitated but is unchanged at P29. B, Group data of PPF (FP2 slope/FP1 slope as a percentage of pre-DHPG baseline) change during LTD in neonatal and adolescent animals. The asterisk indicates that PPF changes are greater in neonatal than adolescent rats (p < 0.02). C, DHPG-induced LTD of FP1 was not different among age groups. D, Representative NMDAR FPs taken at the times indicated in E and F from a P13 or P33 rat. Calibration: 0.2 or 0.1 mV (as indicated), 10 ms. E, F, Group average of decay of NMDAR FP amplitude in the presence of MK801 (10 μm) in control or after DHPG (100 μm; 5 min) treatment of neonatal (E) or adolescent (F) rat slices. Fast component (τ1) is slower in DHPG-treated neonatal slices.

Manipulations that alter presynaptic release probability, such as decreases in the Ca2+/Mg2+ ratio or adenosine, increase paired-pulse facilitation. Generally, the magnitude of PPF is inversely related to presynaptic release probability (Creager et al., 1980; Manabe et al., 1993; Debanne et al., 1996). However, preferential postsynaptic “silencing” of high- or low-release probability synapses also result in PPF changes (Poncer and Malinow, 2001). Pairs of stimulation (interstimulus interval, 50 ms) were delivered during baseline stimulation, during DHPG application, and for 1 h after DHPG. In slices from neonatal rats, PPF ratios increased during DHPG application and persisted for at least 60 min after DHPG (116 ± 2% of pre-DHPG baseline PPF values; n = 14; p < 0.0001), as reported previously (Fitzjohn et al., 2001). PPF values also increased during LTD in slices from adolescent rats (109 ± 2% of pre-DHPG baseline; n = 24; p = 0.008). However, the PPF increases in neonatal animals were significantly greater than those observed in adolescent rats (p < 0.02) (Fig. 7A). There was no difference in the magnitude of DHPG-induced LTD (P8-P15, 72 ± 2%, n = 14; P21-P35, 69 ± 2%, n = 24; p = 0.4) (Fig. 7C) or the absolute levels of PPF [P8-P15, 1.63 ± 0.05 (FP2/FP1 slope); P21-P35, 1.59 ± 0.05; p = 0.63] between these developmental ages. To further support a developmental change in the presynaptic contribution to mGluR-LTD, there is an inverse correlation of PPF changes during LTD and postnatal age (r = 0.4; p < 0.01).

As another means to measure developmental changes in presynaptic function associated with LTD, we used the irreversible, use-dependent NMDAR antagonist MK801. During repetitive presynaptic stimulation, the rate of blockade of NMDAR responses by MK801 has been used to determine presynaptic release probability (Pr) and detect changes in Pr during long-term plasticity, such as long-term potentiation and LTD (Hessler et al., 1993; Rosenmund et al., 1993; Weisskopf and Nicoll, 1995; Kullmann et al., 1996; Xiao et al., 1997; Kaneko and Takahashi, 2004). If Pr is relatively high, there is a greater number of NMDAR channels opened and a more rapid blockade of the response by MK801. We compared the rate of MK801 blockade of NMDAR-mediated FPs in control slices and those in which LTD had been induced using DHPG from both neonatal and adolescent rats. DHPG was used to induce LTD in normal ACSF. After LTD (of the AMPAR response) was established (30 min after DHPG application), a modified ACSF (see Materials and Methods) was applied to the slice to isolate the NMDAR-mediated FP, and this response was allowed to stabilize (∼20-25 min). Synaptic stimulation was stopped for 10 min, and MK801 (10 μm) was applied to the slice. Synaptic stimulation (200 pulses) was resumed at 0.5 Hz in the presence of MK801. The rate of decay of the NMDAR FP amplitude by MK801 was fit by a double exponential, and time constants for the fast (τ1) and slow (τ2) components were obtained (Fig. 7E,F) (Rosenmund et al., 1993). These two components are thought to represent two populations of synaptic terminals with a high and low Pr (Rosenmund et al., 1993). In neonatal slices, DHPG caused an increase in τ1 (DHPG, τ1, 49 ± 8 s; control, τ1, 26 ± 3 s; n = 6; p = 0.02), consistent with a lower Pr after DHPG treatment. This change was evident by the slower decay curves in DHPG-treated slices (Fig. 7E). In contrast, DHPG treatment of adolescent slices did not significantly affect τ1, and the decay curves overlapped (DHPG, τ1, 49 ± 8 s; control, τ1, 33 ± 8 s; n = 6; p = 0.19) (Fig. 7F). There was no change in τ2 at either age (neonatal: DHPG, τ2, 222 ± 22 s; control, τ2, 178 ± 21 s; n = 6; p = 0.15; adolescent: DHPG, τ2, 188 ± 24 s; control, τ2, 254 ± 35 s; n = 6; p = 0.11). There were no differences in τ1 or τ2 between the neonatal and adolescent groups (p = 0.45 and 0.1, respectively). These results, together with the observed PPF changes during LTD, confirm that mGluR-LTD at neonatal synapses is accompanied by decreases in presynaptic release probability (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Zakharenko et al., 2002). Our results also indicate that the magnitude of these presynaptic changes diminish as synapses mature and suggest that other, perhaps postsynaptic, mechanisms contribute to mGluR-LTD in mature synapses.

CB1 receptor activation is not required for mGluR-LTD in neonatal rats

Previous work has demonstrated that the induction of mGluR-LTD at neonatal synapses requires postsynaptic Ca2+ influx and depolarization (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Oliet et al., 1997). Because of the presynaptic changes associated with LTD, the existence of a retrograde messenger has been postulated. Here, we tested the involvement of potential retrograde messengers in mGluR-LTD at neonatal synapses: endocannabinoids.

Endocannabinoids are released from CA1 pyramidal neurons in response to group 1 mGluR activation and depress both excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission via a presynaptic CB1R (Misner and Sullivan, 1999; Varma et al., 2001; Ohno-Shosaku et al., 2002). Endocannabinoids have also been implicated in LTD at inhibitory synapses in area CA1 and at excitatory synapses in the basal ganglia and neocortex (Gerdeman et al., 2002; Robbe et al., 2002; Chevaleyre and Castillo, 2003; Sjostrom et al., 2003). A previous study failed to find a role for CB1R in DHPG-induced LTD in area CA1 of adolescent mice (Rouach and Nicoll, 2003). We obtained similar results in neonatal rat hippocampal slices and found no role for CB1R in mGluR-LTD (Fig. 8B,C). The effects of the CB1R antagonist AM281 (1 μm) were tested on mGluR-LTD in neonatal slices, which blocks mGluR and depolarization-induced endocannabinoid release (Gifford et al., 1997; Maejima et al., 2001; Ohno-Shosaku et al., 2002; Melis et al., 2004). AM281 (1 μm) had no effect on either DHPG-induced LTD (AM281, 84 ± 6%, n = 7; control, 84 ± 4%, n = 7; p = 0.94) or LTD induced with PP-LFS (AM281, 73 ± 5%, n = 8; control, 67 ± 3%, n = 10; p = 0.26). In contrast, preapplication of AM281 greatly reduced the synaptic depression induced by the CB1R agonist WIN55,212-2 (2 μm; 10 min) (D'Ambra et al., 1992; Al-Hayani and Davies, 2000), indicating that the antagonist is effective in our slice preparation (WIN55,212-2, 47 ± 9% of baseline, n = 4; WIN55,212-2 plus AM281, 87 ± 2%, n = 4; p = 0.03) (Fig. 8A). These results confirm that CB1R activation depresses excitatory synaptic transmission in neonatal hippocampal area CA1 (Al-Hayani and Davies, 2000) and demonstrate that CB1Rs are not required for mGluR-induced LTD at this age.

  Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

CB1R activation is not required for mGluR-LTD in neonatal slices. A, CB1R antagonist AM281 (1 μm) is effective in the hippocampal slice preparation and inhibits synaptic depression induced by a brief application of the CB1R agonist WIN55,212-2 (WIN 55; 2 μm; 10 min). Solid bars indicate the time of drug application. Representative FPs are taken at the times indicated by the numbers on the graph. Calibration: 0.5 mV, 5 ms. B, C, Preincubation in AM281 does not affect LTD induced with DHPG (B) or PP-LFS (C) in neonatal rat slices.

Discussion

Here, we show, using both pharmacological and synaptic stimulation of group 1 mGluRs, that the synaptic mechanisms and protein synthesis dependence of mGluR-LTD change with developmental age. In neonatal synapses, mGluR-LTD does not rely on protein synthesis and is associated with large changes in PPF and the rate of MK801 blockade, suggesting a presynaptic site of LTD expression. Consistent with this idea, mGluR activation of neonatal slices does not cause a decrease in AMPAR surface expression. As synapses mature, mGluR-LTD and the associated decrease in AMPAR surface expression require new protein synthesis and result in a small or no change in presynaptic function.

Previous studies on the synaptic mechanisms of mGluR-LTD have yielded conflicting results. Many of the studies, which concluded there is a presynaptic site of mGluR-LTD expression, were performed in hippocampal slices from neonatal animals (P4-P18) (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Zakharenko et al., 2002; Rammes et al., 2003) (but see Watabe et al., 2002). However, the studies demonstrating a role for postsynaptic protein synthesis were performed at more mature synapses (14-21 d in vitro cultures or slices from P10-P30 rats) (Huber et al., 2000; Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001). Our results offer an explanation for these disparate results and indicate that the developmental state of the synapse determines its response to postsynaptic mGluR activation.

To quantitate and compare the developmental changes in LTD, we grouped our data into two age groups: neonatal (P8-P15) and adolescent (P21-P35). However, the significant correlations of postnatal age with the effects of anisomycin on LTD (Fig. 3D) and the magnitude of PPF changes associated with LTD suggest that synaptic changes in mGluR-LTD do not occur abruptly but gradually as the synapses mature. Furthermore, the fact that there are small changes in PPF (Fig. 7B) and a nonsignificant reduction in τ1 of MK801 blockade, which occur during LTD in the adolescent group, suggest that a presynaptic expression mechanism, albeit reduced, may persist at mature synapses. However, because mGluR-LTD in adolescents is blocked by protein synthesis inhibitors, this suggests that any presynaptic contribution must also require protein synthesis. Alternatively, or in addition, PPF changes observed in the adolescent group may have a contribution from AMPAR removal at high release probability synapses (Poncer and Malinow, 2001).

There are alternative explanations for the greater PPF changes we observe during LTD at neonatal synapses. A recent study demonstrated that the baseline PPF magnitude is inversely correlated with the PPF changes observed during LTD (Santschi and Stanton, 2003). Therefore, a higher Pr at neonatal synapses would be reflected by a lower PPF and could explain why there are greater increases in PPF during LTD. In our study, we find no correlation with baseline PPF and the change in PPF with LTD (r = 0.05; p = 0.7). The difference in our findings may be attributable to a difference in the route of LTD induction (Santschi and Stanton, 2003). Furthermore, we do not find a correlation with developmental age and the baseline PPF (r = 0.13; p = 0.4) or differences in the average baseline PPF values or the τ1 of MK801 blockade between neonatal and adolescent groups (see Results). A recent study described developmental decreases in Pr at hippocampal synapses, as measured by PPF and the rate of MK801 blockade, but these changes occurred during an earlier developmental window (from P6 to P12) than our developmental switch (Wasling et al., 2004). Other studies of Pr later in hippocampal development (P15-P35) report an increase in Pr or no change (Dumas and Foster, 1995; Hsia et al., 1998). We think that the greater DHPG-induced changes in PPF and rate of MK801 blockade at neonatal synapses represent a greater presynaptic contribution to the LTD compared with more mature synapses.

Although we find that mGluR-LTD is independent of protein synthesis at developing synapses, our results do not address the question of whether mGluRs activate synaptic protein synthesis at this age. Instead, our data indicate that there are developmental changes in mGluR regulation of AMPAR trafficking. New proteins are required for the persistent decrease in surface AMPAR expression at mature synapses (Fig. 4E,F) (Snyder et al., 2001). Therefore, it is likely that LTD is protein synthesis independent in the neonatal slices because mGluR activation does not alter AMPAR surface expression (Fig. 6). Because NMDAR-dependent decreases in AMPAR surface expression are intact in neonatal synapses, we conclude that components of the mGluR-mediated AMPAR endocytosis process are developmentally regulated, as opposed to general AMPAR endocytosis machinery.

We measured changes in GluR surface expression and not endocytosis per se. Therefore, mGluR activation of neonatal synapses may induce endocytosis of AMPARs but also increase insertion rates so there is no net change in surface expression. In addition, with receptor biotinylation, we cannot determine whether we are measuring surface expression of synaptic or extrasynaptic receptors or both. Therefore, the developmental differences that we observe in DHPG-induced decreases in AMPAR surface expression could be attributable to a difference in the ability to detect surface changes in synaptic receptors with biotinylation. Using immunocytochemistry in dissociated neuronal culture, DHPG has been shown to reduce the number of synaptic AMPARs associated with presynaptic markers, and we would predict that similar changes are occurring in the slice (Snyder et al., 2001). However, it remains to be determined whether there is a developmental change in the effects of DHPG specifically on synaptic AMPARs. Recent work demonstrated that postsynaptic Ca2+ increases alone, independent of NMDARs or mGluRs, are sufficient to induce “silent” synapses (presumably by removing postsynaptic AMPARs) at immature (P3-P12), but not mature (P29-P32), synapses (Xiao et al., 2004). This previous work and our current results suggest that the mechanisms that induce AMPAR removal, such as those that regulate insertion, change with synapse development (Esteban et al., 2003; Yasuda et al., 2003).

The CB1R agonist WIN55,212-2 induced a small depression in the presence of 1 μm AM281 (Fig. 8A), suggesting an incomplete blockade of CB1Rs. This leaves open the possibility that a minority of CB1R activation can lead to full mGluR-LTD. However, we find this unlikely and think that our results are consistent with those of Rouach and Nicoll (2003) (from mature rats), who found no role for endocannabinoids in mGluR-LTD of excitatory synaptic transmission. Because endocannabinoids have been implicated in mGluR-dependent LTD of inhibitory synaptic transmission (Chevaleyre and Castillo, 2003), this suggests that mGluRs use distinct mechanisms to induce LTD of inhibitory and excitatory synaptic transmission. Another potential retrograde messenger for mGluR-LTD at immature synapses is the arachidonic acid metabolite 12-(S)hydroperoxyeicosatetraenoic acid, which is required for mGluR-LTD induced with 5 Hz stimulation (Feinmark et al., 2003). Additional experiments are required to confirm the role of arachidonic acid metabolism in DHPG and PP-LFS-induced LTD.

The developmental switch in the synaptic mechanisms of LTD may occur to accommodate the changing properties of synapses over the course of maturation. Because there are fewer surface AMPARs at immature synapses (Liao et al., 1999; Petralia et al., 1999; Pickard et al., 2000), it may be more efficacious to depress synaptic transmission at developing synapses by reducing presynaptic release probability, as opposed to endocytosis of AMPARs. The consequences of the developmental switch of LTD on hippocampal function are unclear. Because mGluR-LTD affects the short-term dynamics of nascent synapses (Fig. 7), it may differentially impact information processing and plasticity of developing and mature CA1 synapses (Fuhrmann et al., 2002).

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R01NS045711, the McKnight Foundation, and the FRAXA Research Foundation. K.M.H. is a Southwestern Medical Foundation Endowed Scholar in Biomedical Research. We thank Christine Daly and Lenora Volk for technical assistance and Ege Kavalali and Jay Gibson for helpful discussions.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Kimberly Huber, Center for Basic Neuroscience, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, 5323 Harry Hines Boulevard, Dallas, TX 75390-9111. E-mail: kimberly.huber{at}utsouthwestern.edu.

  • Copyright © 2005 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/05/252992-10$15.00/0

References

  1. ↵
    Al-Hayani A, Davies SN (2000) Cannabinoid receptor mediated inhibition of excitatory synaptic transmission in the rat hippocampal slice is developmentally regulated. Br J Pharmacol 131: 663-665.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Bolshakov VY, Siegelbaum SA (1994) Postsynaptic induction and presynaptic expression of hippocampal long-term depression. Science 264: 1148-1152.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Bolshakov VY, Siegelbaum SA (1995) Regulation of hippocampal transmitter release during development and long-term potentiation. Science 269: 1730-1734.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    Bolshakov VY, Carboni L, Cobb MH, Siegelbaum SA, Belardetti F (2000) Dual MAP kinase pathways mediate opposing forms of long-term plasticity at CA3-CA1 synapses. Nat Neurosci 3: 1107-1112.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Casabona G, Knopfel T, Kuhn R, Gasparini F, Baumann P, Sortino MA, Copani A, Nicoletti F (1997) Expression and coupling to polyphosphoinositide hydrolysis of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors in early postnatal and adult rat brain. Eur J Neurosci 9: 12-17.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Chevaleyre V, Castillo PE (2003) Heterosynaptic LTD of hippocampal GABAergic synapses: a novel role of endocannabinoids in regulating excitability. Neuron 38: 461-472.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Chung HJ, Xia J, Scannevin RH, Zhang X, Huganir RL (2000) Phosphorylation of the AMPA receptor subunit GluR2 differentially regulates its interaction with PDZ domain-containing proteins. J Neurosci 20: 7258-7267.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    Colledge M, Snyder EM, Crozier RA, Soderling JA, Jin Y, Langeberg LK, Lu H, Bear MF, Scott JD (2003) Ubiquitination regulates PSD-95 degradation and AMPA receptor surface expression. Neuron 40: 595-607.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Creager R, Dunwiddie T, Lynch G (1980) Paired-pulse and frequency facilitation in the CA1 region of the in vitro rat hippocampus. J Physiol (Lond) 299: 409-424.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    D'Ambra TE, Estep KG, Bell MR, Eissenstat MA, Josef KA, Ward SJ, Haycock DA, Baizman ER, Casiano FM, Beglin NC (1992) Conformationally restrained analogues of pravadoline: nanomolar potent, enantioselective, (aminoalkyl)indole agonists of the cannabinoid receptor. J Med Chem 35: 124-135.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Debanne D, Guerineau NC, Gahwiler BH, Thompson SM (1996) Paired-pulse facilitation and depression at unitary synapses in rat hippocampus: quantal fluctuation affects subsequent release. J Physiol (Lond) 491: 163-176.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    Dudek SM, Bear MF (1993) Bidirectional long-term modification of synaptic effectiveness in the adult and immature hippocampus. J Neurosci 13: 2910-2918.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  13. ↵
    Dudek SM, Bowen WD, Bear MF (1989) Postnatal changes in glutamate stimulated phosphoinositide turnover in rat neocortical synaptoneurosomes. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 47: 123-128.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    Dumas TC, Foster TC (1995) Developmental increase in CA3-CA1 presynaptic function in the hippocampal slice. J Neurophysiol 73: 1821-1828.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    Esteban JA, Shi SH, Wilson C, Nuriya M, Huganir RL, Malinow R (2003) PKA phosphorylation of AMPA receptor subunits controls synaptic trafficking underlying plasticity. Nat Neurosci 6: 136-143.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Feinmark SJ, Begum R, Tsvetkov E, Goussakov I, Funk CD, Siegelbaum SA, Bolshakov VY (2003) 12-Lipoxygenase metabolites of arachidonic acid mediate metabotropic glutamate receptor-dependent long-term depression at hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses. J Neurosci 23: 11427-11435.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    Fitzjohn SM, Bortolotto ZA, Palmer MJ, Doherty AJ, Ornstein PL, Schoepp DD, Kingston AE, Lodge D, Collingridge GL (1998) The potent mGlu receptor antagonist LY341495 identifies roles for both cloned and novel mGlu receptors in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Neuropharmacology 37: 1445-1458.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Fitzjohn SM, Palmer MJ, May JE, Neeson A, Morris SA, Collingridge GL (2001) A characterisation of long-term depression induced by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation in the rat hippocampus in vitro. J Physiol (Lond) 537: 421-430.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Fuhrmann G, Segev I, Markram H, Tsodyks M (2002) Coding of temporal information by activity-dependent synapses. J Neurophysiol 87: 140-148.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    Gerdeman GL, Ronesi J, Lovinger DM (2002) Postsynaptic endocannabinoid release is critical to long-term depression in the striatum. Nat Neurosci 5: 446-451.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Gifford AN, Tang Y, Gatley SJ, Volkow ND, Lan R, Makriyannis A (1997) Effect of the cannabinoid receptor SPECT agent, AM 281, on hippocampal acetylcholine release from rat brain slices. Neurosci Lett 238: 84-86.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Hessler NA, Shirke AM, Malinow R (1993) The probability of transmitter release at a mammalian central synapse. Nature 366: 569-572.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Heynen AJ, Yoon BJ, Liu CH, Chung HJ, Huganir RL, Bear MF (2003) Molecular mechanism for loss of visual cortical responsiveness following brief monocular deprivation. Nat Neurosci 6: 854-862.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Hsia AY, Malenka RC, Nicoll RA (1998) Development of excitatory circuitry in the hippocampus. J Neurophysiol 79: 2013-2024.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Huber KM, Kayser MS, Bear MF (2000) Role for rapid dendritic protein synthesis in hippocampal mGluR-dependent LTD. Science 288: 1254-1257.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Huettner JE (2003) Kainate receptors and synaptic transmission. Prog Neurobiol 70: 387-407.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Kaneko M, Takahashi T (2004) Presynaptic mechanism underlying cAMP-dependent synaptic potentiation. J Neurosci 24: 5202-5208.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Kemp N, McQueen J, Faulkes S, Bashir ZI (2000) Different forms of LTD in the CA1 region of the hippocampus: role of age and stimulus protocol. Eur J Neurosci 12: 360-366.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Kullmann DM, Erdemli G, Asztely F (1996) LTP of AMPA and NMDA receptor-mediated signals: evidence for presynaptic expression and extrasynaptic glutamate spill-over. Neuron 17: 461-474.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Lee HK, Kameyama K, Huganir RL, Bear MF (1998) NMDA induces longterm synaptic depression and dephosphorylation of the GluR1 subunit of AMPA receptors in hippocampus. Neuron 21: 1151-1162.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Liao D, Zhang X, O'Brien R, Ehlers MD, Huganir RL (1999) Regulation of morphological postsynaptic silent synapses in developing hippocampal neurons. Nat Neurosci 2: 37-43.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Maejima T, Ohno-Shosaku T, Kano M (2001) Endogenous cannabinoid as a retrograde messenger from depolarized postsynaptic neurons to presynaptic terminals. Neurosci Res 40: 205-210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Manabe T, Wyllie DJ, Perkel DJ, Nicoll RA (1993) Modulation of synaptic transmission and long-term potentiation: effects on paired pulse facilitation and EPSC variance in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. J Neurophysiol 70: 1451-1459.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Melis M, Pistis M, Perra S, Muntoni AL, Pillolla G, Gessa GL (2004) Endocannabinoids mediate presynaptic inhibition of glutamatergic transmission in rat ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons through activation of CB1 receptors. J Neurosci 24: 53-62.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Misner DL, Sullivan JM (1999) Mechanism of cannabinoid effects on longterm potentiation and depression in hippocampal CA1 neurons. J Neurosci 19: 6795-6805.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Nicoletti F, Iadarola MJ, Wroblewski JT, Costa E (1986) Excitatory amino acid recognition sites coupled with inositol phospholipid metabolism: developmental changes and interaction with alpha 1-adrenoceptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 83: 1931-1935.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Ohno-Shosaku T, Tsubokawa H, Mizushima I, Yoneda N, Zimmer A, Kano M (2002) Presynaptic cannabinoid sensitivity is a major determinant of depolarization-induced retrograde suppression at hippocampal synapses. J Neurosci 22: 3864-3872.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    Oliet SH, Malenka RC, Nicoll RA (1997) Two distinct forms of long-term depression coexist in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells. Neuron 18: 969-982.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Overstreet LS, Pasternak JF, Colley PA, Slater NT, Trommer BL (1997) Metabotropic glutamate receptor mediated long-term depression in developing hippocampus. Neuropharmacology 36: 831-844.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Palmer E, Nangel-Taylor K, Krause JD, Roxas A, Cotman CW (1990) Changes in excitatory amino acid modulation of phosphoinositide metabolism during development. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 51: 132-134.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Petralia RS, Esteban JA, Wang YX, Partridge JG, Zhao HM, Wenthold RJ, Malinow R (1999) Selective acquisition of AMPA receptors over postnatal development suggests a molecular basis for silent synapses. Nat Neurosci 2: 31-36.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Pickard L, Noel J, Henley JM, Collingridge GL, Molnar E (2000) Developmental changes in synaptic AMPA and NMDA receptor distribution and AMPA receptor subunit composition in living hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 20: 7922-7931.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Poncer JC, Malinow R (2001) Postsynaptic conversion of silent synapses during LTP affects synaptic gain and transmission dynamics. Nat Neurosci 4: 989-996.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Pouzat C, Hestrin S (1997) Developmental regulation of basket/stellate cell→ Purkinje cell synapses in the cerebellum. J Neurosci 17: 9104-9112.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    Rammes G, Palmer M, Eder M, Dodt HU, Zieglgansberger W, Collingridge GL (2003) Activation of mGlu receptors induces LTD without affecting postsynaptic sensitivity of CA1 neurons in rat hippocampal slices. J Physiol (Lond) 546: 455-460.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    Reyes A, Sakmann B (1999) Developmental switch in the short-term modification of unitary EPSPs evoked in layer 2/3 and layer 5 pyramidal neurons of rat neocortex. J Neurosci 19: 3827-3835.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    Robbe D, Kopf M, Remaury A, Bockaert J, Manzoni OJ (2002) Endogenous cannabinoids mediate long-term synaptic depression in the nucleus accumbens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99: 8384-8388.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    Rosenmund C, Clements JD, Westbrook GL (1993) Nonuniform probability of glutamate release at a hippocampal synapse. Science 262: 754-757.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    Rouach N, Nicoll RA (2003) Endocannabinoids contribute to short-term but not long-term mGluR-induced depression in the hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 18: 1017-1020.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Santschi LA, Stanton PK (2003) A paired-pulse facilitation analysis of longterm synaptic depression at excitatory synapses in rat hippocampal CA1 and CA3 regions. Brain Res 962: 78-91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Sjostrom PJ, Turrigiano GG, Nelson SB (2003) Neocortical LTD via coincident activation of presynaptic NMDA and cannabinoid receptors. Neuron 39: 641-654.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Snyder EM, Philpot BD, Huber KM, Dong X, Fallon JR, Bear MF (2001) Internalization of ionotropic glutamate receptors in response to mGluR activation. Nat Neurosci 4: 1079-1085.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Steward O, Falk PM (1985) Polyribosomes under developing spine synapses: growth specializations of dendrites at sites of synaptogenesis. J Neurosci Res 13: 75-88.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Steward O, Falk PM (1991) Selective localization of polyribosomes beneath developing synapses: a quantitative analysis of the relationships between polyribosomes and developing synapses in the hippocampus and dentate gyrus. J Comp Neurol 314: 545-557.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Varma N, Carlson GC, Ledent C, Alger BE (2001) Metabotropic glutamate receptors drive the endocannabinoid system in hippocampus. J Neurosci 21: RC188(1-5).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    Wasling P, Hanse E, Gustafsson B (2004) Developmental changes in release properties of the CA3-CA1 glutamate synapse in rat hippocampus. J Neurophysiol 92: 2714-2724.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    Watabe AM, Carlisle HJ, O'Dell TJ (2002) Postsynaptic induction and presynaptic expression of group 1 mGluR-dependent LTD in the hippocampal CA1 region. J Neurophysiol 87: 1395-1403.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    Weisskopf MG, Nicoll RA (1995) Presynaptic changes during mossy fibre LTP revealed by NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic responses. Nature 376: 256-259.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Xiao MY, Niu YP, Wigstrom H (1997) Comparing long-term depression with pharmacologically induced synaptic attenuations in young rat hippocampi. Synapse 26: 329-340.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    Xiao MY, Zhou Q, Nicoll RA (2001) Metabotropic glutamate receptor activation causes a rapid redistribution of AMPA receptors. Neuropharmacology 41: 664-671.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    Xiao MY, Wasling P, Hanse E, Gustafsson B (2004) Creation of AMPA-silent synapses in the neonatal hippocampus. Nat Neurosci 7: 236-243.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Yasuda H, Barth AL, Stellwagen D, Malenka RC (2003) A developmental switch in the signaling cascades for LTP induction. Nat Neurosci 6: 15-16.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Zakharenko SS, Zablow L, Siegelbaum SA (2002) Altered presynaptic vesicle release and cycling during mGluR-dependent LTD. Neuron 35: 1099-1110.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
View Abstract
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 25 (11)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 25, Issue 11
16 Mar 2005
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Developmental Switch in Synaptic Mechanisms of Hippocampal Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor-Dependent Long-Term Depression
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Developmental Switch in Synaptic Mechanisms of Hippocampal Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor-Dependent Long-Term Depression
Elena D. Nosyreva, Kimberly M. Huber
Journal of Neuroscience 16 March 2005, 25 (11) 2992-3001; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3652-04.2005

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Developmental Switch in Synaptic Mechanisms of Hippocampal Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor-Dependent Long-Term Depression
Elena D. Nosyreva, Kimberly M. Huber
Journal of Neuroscience 16 March 2005, 25 (11) 2992-3001; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3652-04.2005
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Identification of the Acid-Sensitive Site Critical for Chloral Hydrate (CH) Activation of the Proton-Activated Chloride Channel
  • mTORC2 Loss in Oligodendrocyte Progenitor Cells Results in Regional Hypomyelination in the Central Nervous System
  • An Atypical, Staged Cell Death Pathway Induced by Depletion of SNARE-Proteins MUNC18-1 or Syntaxin-1
Show more Cellular/Molecular
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.