Abstract
Sex- and age-associated deficits in brain structure and behavior are reported in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders. Although genetic and environmental factors are thought to contribute to the pathogenesis, there are only few examples in clinical or experimental systems that have identified specific causes. Here, we report that transforming growth factor-α (TGFα) may regulate sex- and age-dependent development of forebrain structures and associated neural functions after puberty. Waved-1 (Wa-1) mice inherit an autosomal recessive, spontaneous mutation that results in a postnatal reduction in TGFα gene expression. The assessment of forebrain structures using a three-dimensional magnetic resonance microscopy indicated ventricular enlargement and striatal reduction in both male and female Wa-1 adult mice, with Wa-1 males exhibiting a more severe phenotype. In contrast, the hippocampal volume was reduced only in adult Wa-1 males. Similarly, behavioral analyses showed impaired auditory and contextual fear learning in adult Wa-1 males only, whereas abnormal stress response was expressed by both male and female adult Wa-1 mice. Interestingly, all behavioral deficits were absent before full sexual maturation, despite some slight forebrain structural abnormalities. These results suggest that TGFα may regulate postpubertal, sex differentiation in ventricular and periventricular anatomy and associated behavior, affecting predominantly males. In particular, the adult male-specific reduction in hippocampal volume may reflect an age- and sex-specific regulation of stress homeostasis and fear learning. Furthermore, a lack of a behavioral phenotype, despite anatomical alterations in peripubertal Wa-1 mice, suggests that analysis of certain neuroanatomical features at puberty may predict neurobehavioral deficits in adulthood.
Introduction
Sex and age are factors that may contribute to the pathophysiology of specific neuropsychiatric disorders. For example, the average age of onset of schizophrenia is between adolescence and adulthood (for review, see Lewis and Lieberman, 2000). Males show an earlier onset of schizophrenic symptoms and exhibit more severe forebrain abnormalities and poorer clinical outcome than females (Nopoulos et al., 1997; Gur et al., 2004). Other neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism, are more prevalent in males than in females, with no X- or Y-linked patterns of inheritance (for review, see Rutter et al., 2003). The biological mechanisms responsible for these sex- and age-associated neural disruptions, however, are as yet unknown.
Interestingly, manipulations of transforming growth factor-α (TGFα) expression result in sex- and age-dependent behavioral and brain anatomical disturbances. TGFα is a member of the ErbB receptor tyrosine kinases, regulating developmental processes, including the onset of female puberty (Ma et al., 1992). Its expression is developmentally regulated, sensitive to gonadal steroids, and increases at approximately puberty in female rat and primate hypothalamus (Ma et al., 1994a,b). Waved-1 (Wa-1) is an autosomal recessive mouse carrying mutation(s) in TGFα gene (Crew, 1933; Fowler et al., 1993). Similar to other TGFα and erbB1 mutant mice, Wa-1 mice show abnormal hair follicles, resulting in wavy hair, and delayed prenatal eyelid fusion (Luetteke et al., 1993, 1994; Mann et al., 1993). In Wa-1 males, TGFα transcript and protein levels are reduced throughout postnatal development (Weickert and Blum, 1995; Berkowitz et al., 1996), which results in enlarged lateral ventricles and compromised contextual fear conditioning in adulthood but not during prepubescence (Burrows et al., 2000). These studies (except for Weickert and Blum, 1995) were conducted using the original strain of Wa-1 mice, which had a mixed strain background with additional mutations (Crew, 1933; Fowler et al., 1993). Other studies have shown that the overexpression of TGFα results in sex-dependent disruptions in stress behavior (Hilakivi-Clarke et al., 1992, 1993).
To clarify the function of TGFα in establishing the sex- and age-specific anatomical and behavioral properties during development, a more recent strain of Wa-1 on a C57BL/6J background carrying a sole mutation in TGFα gene was examined. Male and female adult and pubertal Wa-1 mice were studied. The hippocampus, amygdala, striatum, and lateral and third ventricle volumes and surface areas were determined using three-dimensional (3D) magnetic resonance microscopy (MRM). These forebrain structures have been implicated in sexual dimorphism (Filipek et al., 1994; Caviness et al., 1996; Giedd et al., 1997), neuropsychiatric disorders (Weinberger, 1995; Chetelat and Baron, 2003; MacQueen et al., 2003), and stress and learning behavior (Luine, 2002). We also assessed memory functions by using delay fear conditioning paradigms. Because memory can be negatively or positively affected by stress in a sex-specific manner (Luine, 2002; Bowman et al., 2003), we also assessed stress responses. The data indicate that TGFα regulates postpubertal development of selective forebrain structures predominantly in males, which is reflected in sex- and age-dependent memory and stress regulation impairment in Wa-1 mice.
Materials and Methods
Animals. Male and female control and Wa-1 C57BL/6J mice were used for all of the experiments. Heterozygous mating pairs were purchased from The Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME) to establish the colony. This strain of Wa-1 has been backcrossed over 12 generations to C57BL/6J strain by The Jackson Laboratory and our laboratory, creating a congenic background. During this process, other mutations present in the original strain of Wa-1 (APB/Le a/a Tyrplb/Tyrplb bt/bt p/p Bmp5SE/Bmp5SE) were eliminated (The Jackson Laboratory). All studies used animals obtained through timed matings. The control group included wild-type and heterozygous mice, because the latter did not show any difference from the wild type in any of the parameters measured. Although estrous phase was monitored daily by recording vaginal color and opening in all postpubertal females, none of the parameters were greatly affected by the estrous cycle (data not shown). The behavioral analyses, plasma corticosterone assay, and MRM were conducted in adult (3-4.5 months) and pubertal (1-1.5 months) mice. Only adult groups were used for plasma catecholamine analyses. Mice used for behavioral analyses were weighed at 1 or 3-4.5 months. Although there were age- and sex-dependent differences (adults heavier than pubertal mice; males heavier than females), there was no difference between control and Wa-1 body weight (data not shown). Mice used for the plasma catecholamine analyses and MRM analyses were not used previously in behavioral tests.
All mice were group housed and maintained in a central facility at University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine in a controlled environment with a 12 h light/dark cycle and access to food and water ad libitum. Experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines provided by University of Pittsburgh Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees and the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Genotyping using simple sequence-repeat PCR. Because Wa-1 is a spontaneous mutation and the exact site(s) of genomic mutation(s) is unknown, a simple sequence-repeat (SSR) PCR assay was developed to distinguish wild-type and mutant mice. SSRs are microsatellite sequences flanking a gene that vary in the number of repeats among different strains of mice (for review, see Weber, 1990; Weber et al., 1990). Because Wa-1 has been fully backcrossed to the C57BL/6J background, which is a different strain than the original strain of Wa-1, it was possible to genotype the mice by PCR amplification of the SSRs surrounding the TGFα gene. This method was used to map and genotype other spontaneous mutations in mice, tottering and Purkinje cell degeneration (Campbell and Hess, 1997; Campbell et al., 1999).
In brief, 3-5 mm tail was clipped and digested in non-ionic detergent buffer containing 50 mm KCl, 10 mm Tris-Cl, pH 8.3, 2 mm MgCl2, 0.1 mg/ml gelatin, 0.45% NP-40, 0.45% Tween 20, and 1.5 μl of 10 mg/ml proteinase K, at 52-54°C overnight. The digested tails were heat inactivated at 95°C for 10 min. The PCR reaction contained 0.68 μl of forward and 0.68 μl of reverse MapPair primers (Research Genetics, Huntsville, AL), 3 μl of 10× ThermoPol Reaction Buffer (New England Biolabs, Beverly, MA), 4.8 μl of 2.5 mm dNTP, 0.3 μl of Vent (exo-) DNA Polymerase (New England Biolabs), and 1-2 μl of tail digest in a total volume of 30 μl. The PCR program was as follows: 95°C for 5 min for 1 cycle, 95°C for 30 s, 62, 60, or 58°C for 1 min, and 72°C for 1 min for 5, 10, and 30 cycles (respectively, for the three annealing temperatures), followed by the final elongation at 72°C for 5 min. The PCR products were run on 3.5% agarose in 1× TAE (0.04 m Tris-acetate and 1 mm EDTA, pH 8.0) with ethidium bromide and were UV visualized. The MapPairs used were D6Mit8 [0.7-1.3 centimorgans (cM) upstream of TGFα/Wa-1; forward, TGCACAGCAGCTCATTCTCT; reverse, GGAAGGAAGGAGTGGGGTAG] and D6Mit228 (1.1-0.5 cM downstream of TGFα/Wa-1; forward, GTTCCCAACAAGGTGGCTAA; reverse, GAGCAGTATGCGGCAGATTT). The wild-type C57BL/6J band sizes were 164 bases for D6Mit8 and 108 bases for D6Mit228 MapPairs. The Wa-1 band sizes were estimated to be ∼190 bases for D6Mit8 and ∼130 bases for D6Mit228 MapPairs. Because Wa-1 has naturally wavy hair and whiskers from birth to adulthood, the genotype of Wa-1 homozygotes was visually confirmed by this phenotype.
MRM. Data from wild-type mice and detailed methods used for MRM have been published recently (Koshibu et al., 2004). This imaging technique was chosen over traditional histological methods to minimize distortion artifacts produced by the extreme treatment of the brain tissue during fixation, sectioning, and mounting and for the greater accuracy in determining structure volumes. Thus, MRM in intact tissues produces morphometric measurements with less variability compared with histological approaches, permitting a smaller statistical sampling size and a more rapid analysis.
Briefly, male and female control and Wa-1 C57BL/6J inbred mice at 1 month (puberty) and 3 months (adult) were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital and then transcardially perfused with 0.9% NaCl, followed by 4% formaldehyde/1% glutaraldehyde in PBS, pH 7.4. Brains were excised from the skull, immersed in fixative, and stored at 4°C. The digital image of each intact brain specimen was collected using an 11.7 tesla Bruker (Rheinstetten, Germany) AVANCE microimaging system. Two coregistered 3D data sets were acquired for each sample. One data set used a T2/spin-density weighted 3D Fourier transform spin-echo sequence, with a repetition time/echo time (TR/TE) = 900/45 ms. The other data set used a diffusion-weighted 3D Fourier transform pulsed-gradient spin-echo sequence (DW-3DFT), with TR/TE = 900/20 ms. For the DW-3DFT data sets, diffusion gradients were applied along the rostrocaudal direction with δ = 5 ms, Δ= 15 ms at a gradient strength of 30 g/cm. The DW-3DFT data set was particularly useful in segmenting the ventricles because signal from the fluid in these structures was readily dephased by the diffusion gradient pulses. Both data sets were acquired with a 256 × 128 × 128 matrix and then zero filled to 512 × 256 × 256 before Fourier transformation, yielding a final isotropic resolution of ∼50 μm. The total imaging time per data set was ∼18 h. Three mice were studied per group. Power analysis verified that this sample size is sufficient to determine statistical significance, given the low inter-animal variance on stereological measures (Koshibu et al., 2004).
Digital segmentation. From the MRM data, the lateral and third ventricles, hippocampus, amygdala, striatum, and total brain were digitally segmented in 3D in a semiautomated manner using the software AMIRA (TGS, San Diego, CA). Each structure was traced manually in a combination of coronal, horizontal, and sagittal orientations (see Fig. 1). Using AMIRA, the pixel-to-pixel borders were then smoothed in 3D to obtain the most accurate representation of each segmented structure. A 3D surface reconstruction of each structure was then created, and the volume and surface area were calculated as described by Koshibu et al. (2004).
The hippocampal volume included the fornix at the rostral boundary. The amygdala segment was bounded by the bifurcation of ventral corpus callosum anteriorly, the optic tract and ventral horn of the lateral ventricles medially, and extension of the ventral corpus callosum to the ventral cortex lateroventrally. The striatum was segmented in its entirety. An example of segmentation of the respective structures is shown in Figure 1. For the total brain, the entire brain was segmented except for olfactory bulbs and the brainstem posterior to the aqueduct.
Behavior. All behavioral experiments, except for stress-induced hyperthermia, were conducted in the afternoon between 2:00 and 6:30 P.M. using a TruScan Photobeam Activity System (Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA). The system consisted of a square arena (x, y, z dimension = 27, 27, 38 cm) with photobeam frame(s). The infrared light beams crisscrossed every 1.5 cm in x-y coordinate. This array of infrared beams enabled automated collection of all locomotor activity by a digital analyzer every 0.5 s.
Open-field activity test. To assess general activity and exploratory behavior, mice were placed in a TruScan Photobeam Activity System for 10 min. The total movement time and distance, marginal zone time, stereotypy, and number of rears were determined. The marginal zone was defined as 3.75 cm from the edges of arena. The stereotypy was defined as movements consisting of three consecutive photobeam crossings of the same photobeam, occurring within 2 s of each other. The stereotypic activity included repetitive behaviors, such as head bobbing, grooming, and paw licking. Fifteen control males, 12 Wa-1 males, 14 control females, and 15 Wa-1 females were used for adult groups. Nine control males, eight Wa-1 males, seven control females, and five Wa-1 females were used for pubertal groups. Two control adult females and one Wa-1 adult female were eliminated from the analysis as outliers, defined as values greater than 2 SDs away from the mean.
Light/dark choice test. The TruScan Photobeam Activity System was divided into light and dark sides by inserting a black box with a small entrance onto one side of the arena. Mice were placed on the brightly lit side of the arena and were left to explore both light and dark sides for 10 min. Total number of entries and time spent on the dark side were calculated to determine anxiety level. Eight control males, 10 Wa-1 males, 10 control females, and 9 Wa-1 females were used for adult groups. Six control males, six Wa-1 males, seven control females, and five Wa-1 females were used for pubertal groups. Two control adult males, two Wa-1 adult males, one control adult female, and one Wa-1 adult female were 2 SDs away from the mean and, thus, eliminated from the analysis.
Delay fear conditioning. A shock grid floor with 23 bars 6 mm apart was inserted in a TruScan Photobeam Activity System, and a speaker was mounted on top of the arena wall. The shock grid floor provided alternating current between the adjacent bars. On the training day, mice were exposed to this arena for 2 min, followed by 30 s continuous tone (80 dB, 2000 Hz). During the last 2 s of the tone, an ∼0.2 mA foot shock was given for 2 s. After 1 min, the tone-shock paired training was repeated. A total of three tone-shock paired trials were administered.
Approximately 24 h later, context learning was assessed. A mouse was placed in the training arena for 5 min, without tone or shock, and its activity was monitored. Associative conditioning to the tone was tested ∼1.5-3 h later. For assessment of tone conditioning, the testing arena was changed to eliminate the contextual learning as follows: (1) removing the shock grid floor, (2) reducing the size of the arena by half, (3) changing the size of the arena, (4) changing the smell of the arena by washing it with lemon-scented detergent, and (5) reducing the light intensity of the testing room. Mice were placed in this new context for 3 min (pre-tone), and then the 80 dB, 2000 Hz tone was sounded continuously for the next 3 min (tone).
Learning was assessed by determining the freezing response of mice. Lack of movement (i.e., freezing) lasting longer than 1 s was included in the percentage freezing response calculation (freeze time/total test time × 100%). The freezing response was analyzed for both training and testing trials. In addition, to assess shock sensitivity, speed of movement during the shock was measured during training (distance moved divided by 2 s). Nine control males, 8 Wa-1 males, 10 control females, and 8 Wa-1 females were used for adult groups. Six control males and five Wa-1 males were used for pubertal groups.
Stress-induced hyperthermia. Stress-induced hyperthermia was conducted as described by Zethof et al. (1994). Mice were individually caged in the late afternoon before the testing day. On the following morning (between 8:30 and 10:30 A.M.), a mouse was held in one hand, and its rectal temperature was measured for 20 s (T1) using a Thermalert TH-5 apparatus (PhysiTemp, Clifton, NJ). The mouse was then returned to the home cage for 10 min, after which, rectal temperature was measured again for 20 s (T2). The change in temperature (dT = T2 - T1) was calculated to determine changes in stress levels. Twenty control males, 8 Wa-1 males, 16 control females, and 16 Wa-1 females were used for adult groups. Six control males, five Wa-1 males, six control females, and six Wa-1 females were used for pubertal groups.
Corticosterone radioimmunoassay. Trunk blood samples (∼200-600 μl) were collected in microcentrifuge tubes with 20 μl of 1000 United States Pharmacopeial U/ml heparin sodium (American Pharmaceutical Partners, Los Angeles, CA) and placed on ice. Plasma was collected by spinning the samples at ∼1500 × g for 15 min at 4°C and stored at -80°C. The corticosterone levels were determined using Coat-A-Count Rat Corticosterone kit (Diagnostic Products, Los Angeles, CA) according to the protocol of the manufacturer. For the baseline level, blood was collected after an overnight individual housing with access to food and water ad libitum. For the stressed level, blood was collected immediately after the stress-induced hyperthermia test. The animals were killed by decapitation. All samples were collected in the morning between 8:30 and 10:30 A.M. Twenty control males, 8 Wa-1 males, 17 control females, and 15 Wa-1 females were used for adult stressed corticosterone measurements. Ten control males, five Wa-1 males, six control females, and five Wa-1 females were used for pubertal stressed levels. For the baseline analyses, 12 control males, 5 Wa-1 males, 15 control females, and 8 Wa-1 females for the adult groups and three animals each of the pubertal groups were used.
Plasma catecholamine measurements. Biogenic amines were determined by a specific HPLC assay using an Antec Decade (oxidation, 0.7) electrochemical detector and following the method by Lindsey et al. (1998). Briefly, plasma samples were purified and concentrated by adsorption of the amines [adrenaline (A); noradrenalin NA)] to solid alumina (Al2O3) to improve the accuracy of the measurements (Mefford, 1985). Dihydroxybenzylamine (internal standard), Tris buffer, pH 8.5, 100 mg Al2O3, and 100-200 μl of supernatant were mixed for 15 min, and then the buffer was aspirated. The Al2O3 was washed with distilled water, water was aspirated, and then the amines were desorbed from the Al2O3 using 125 μl of 0.1 N acetic acid. Twenty to 80 ml of these samples were then injected onto a Waters (Milford, MA) Nova-Pak C18 HPLC column using a Waters 717+ autosampler. Biogenic amines were eluted with a mobile phase consisting of 89.5% 0.1 m TCA, 0.01 m sodium acetate, 0.1 μm EDTA, and 10.5% methanol, pH 3.8. Solvent was delivered at 0.7 ml/min using a Waters 515 HPLC pump. A calibration curve was run along with the samples and was used to calibrate the instrument. HPLC control and data acquisition were managed by Millennium 32 software (Waters).
The biogenic amine measurements were normalized to the total protein concentrations, which were determined using BCA Protein Assay kit (Pierce, Rockford, IL) according to the protocol of the manufacturer.
Statistics. When applicable, a 2 × 2 × 2 (age × sex × genotype) multivariate generalized linear model (GLM) was used. The main effects of age, sex, and genotype and any interactions among various combinations of these factors were determined by type III sum of squares. In addition, a 2 × 2 (age × genotype) GLM was conducted separately for males and females. A univariate GLM with Tukey's post hoc test was used for HPLC data to detect the genotype effect in males and females separately. Any result indicating p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. All data are presented as estimated marginal means ± SEM.
MRM volumetric comparisons were conducted in three ways: (1) using raw data, (2) using data with total brain volume as a covariant, and (3) using data with total brain volume without the ventricular mass (i.e., solid brain volume) as a covariant. Similarly, surface areas were compared among the groups by using the volumetric measurement of respective structures as a covariant to determine a possible dissociation between volumetric and surface area changes. A lack of covariance between the volume and the surface area is consistent with a possible structural shape distortion. If the surface area is disproportionately larger than the volume, then it may indicate an increased convolution of the surface. Conversely, if the surface area is disproportionately reduced compared with the volume, a smoother surface may be present.
For fear conditioning results, the context, pre-tone, and tone freezing responses were assessed by using the initial freezing response during the first 2 min of training as a covariant to normalize for any general activity differences. For tone learning, pre-tone was used as a covariant to normalize for the differences in pre-tone freezing responses. Similarly, the stressed corticosterone level was normalized to the baseline corticosterone level by taking the baseline corticosterone as a covariant. Pearson's correlation test was conducted to test possible correlation between stress response and fear learning results.
Results
Forebrain anatomy analyses using MRM
The MRM methods used in this study provide very-high resolution images that allow clear identification of the subcortical structures (Fig. 1) (Koshibu et al., 2004). Representative single coronal slices taken from the DW-3DFT volumetric data sets are shown for the adult groups in Figure 2. A general enlargement of the lateral and third ventricles (hypotensive areas within the brain parenchyma) is clearly evident in adult females (Fig. 2c,d) compared with adult males (Fig. 2a,b) in the raw image data. Furthermore, both male and female adult Wa-1 (Fig. 2b,d, respectively) show enlarged ventricles compared with their respective controls (Fig. 2a,c). Because subtle changes in other structures cannot be determined at this level of analysis, 3D segmentation and reconstruction of all of the structures were conducted.
General developmental and sex-dependent changes in the forebrain structures in combined wild-type and Wa-1 population
As expected, the total brain volume is larger at 3 months than at 1 month in general (Fig. 3f). The developmental enlargement of structures, proportional to the total brain growth, is observed in both male and female hippocampus (Fig. 3c) and female lateral ventricles (Fig. 3a). In contrast, the striatum shows disproportionate reduction (p = 0.005), and the amygdala shows disproportionate enlargement (p < 0.001) in both sexes when normalized to the total brain growth. Reduced surface areas are detected in lateral (p = 0.023) and third (p = 0.036) ventricles and hippocampus (p = 0.016) at 3 months compared with 1 month, despite their increased respective volumes, suggesting a smoother surface of these structures in adult compared with pubertal mouse brains (compare Fig. 3a-c with supplemental Fig. 1a-c, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). In contrast, the increased surface area in amygdala and for total brain at 3 months compared with 1 month (supplemental Fig. 1e,f, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) is proportional to their respective volumetric changes (Fig. 3e,f), indicating no shape distortions in these structures.
The robust differences between males and females also are observed in specific forebrain structure volumes. The lateral (p = 0.002) and third (p = 0.018) ventricles are larger (Fig. 3a,b, respectively), whereas the amygdala is smaller (p = 0.002) (Fig. 3e) in females compared with males in general. These changes are not attributable to the differences in total brain volume between males and females. Larger lateral ventricles in females compared with the males are mainly attributable to the ventricular enlargement in adult females and not attributable to the sex differences in pubertal mice (Fig. 3a). The enlarged ventricles in females can be visually confirmed in the 3D reconstructions shown in Figure 4 [compare a, b (males) with c, d (females)]. The opposing volumetric changes in the lateral ventricle and amygdala indicate a possible encroaching of the lateral ventricles on the amygdala. No sex-dependent shape distortions were detected in any of the forebrain structures analyzed.
Sex-dependent developmental changes in forebrain structures of Wa-1
An enlargement of the lateral and third ventricular volumes is observed in adult Wa-1 (Fig. 4), whereas a slight reduction in these volumes is detected in pubertal Wa-1 compared with the control groups (Fig. 5a,b). These ventricular volumetric changes are more pronounced in males than in females. The pubertal Wa-1 males show 17 and 32% reductions in lateral and third ventricular volumes, respectively, and 76% enlargement in adult lateral ventricular volume compared with the control males. In contrast, the pubertal Wa-1 females show a 5% reduction in lateral ventricular volume and 47% enlargement in adult lateral ventricular volume. The changes in adult third ventricular volumes are approximately the same between male and female Wa-1. These differences persist even after normalized to the total brain volume changes.
The opposite pattern of changes is observed for hippocampal and striatal volumes. Both of these structures are smaller at 3 months but larger at 1 month in Wa-1 compared with the controls (Fig. 5c,d). Similar to ventricular changes, adult Wa-1 males show greater reduction in hippocampal (-9%) and striatal (-9%) volumes than adult Wa-1 females, which show no change in hippocampus and only 4% reduction in striatum compared with their respective controls. Pubertal changes are similar in both males and females. The reduction in Wa-1 striatal volume persists even after corrected for the total brain volume but not when normalized to the total solid brain volume (i.e., total brain volume without the ventricular mass). These results suggest a correlation between ventricular and striatum volumetric changes.
A simplification of the lateral ventricular shape in pubertal male and female Wa-1 mice compared with their respective controls is detected statistically (combined analyses of Fig. 3a and supplemental Fig. 1a, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). In contrast, at 3 months, Wa-1 males have a larger lateral ventricular surface area than male controls, disproportional to their respective volumetric differences (compare Fig. 3a with supplemental Fig. 1a, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) (see also Fig. 4), indicating an increased convolution of the structure in male Wa-1 adults compared with the male controls. A lack of changes in the Wa-1 hippocampal and striatal surface areas (supplemental Fig. 2c,d, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), despite the altered volumetric measurements (Fig. 5c,d), suggests shape differences in hippocampus and striatum between control and Wa-1 mice. Interestingly, only adult female Wa-1 shows comparable hippocampal volume and surface area with that of adult female control.
Behavior: general motor activity and anxiety assessments
Developmentally, pubertal mice are hypoactive compared with adult mice in general, which is apparent in the reduced total move time (p = 0.018; 420.9 ± 5.0 s in 1 month vs 436.2 ± 3.8 s in 3 months) (Fig. 6a). In addition, the pubertal mice spend more time in the margins of the arena (p < 0.001; 456.5 ± 8.7 s in 1 month vs 310.6 ± 6.5 s in 3 months) (Fig. 6c) and show greater episodes of stereotypy (p < 0.001; 47.5 ± 1.5 s in 1 month vs 38.9 ± 1.1 s in 3 months) (Fig. 6e) than their adult counterparts. These findings may reflect a heightened fear or anxiety response to a novel environment in the pubertal mice. In fact, the pubertal mice show an increased frequency of dark side entries compared with adult mice in general during light/dark choice test (Fig. 6f), which is another indication of increased anxiety.
When males and females are compared, there are no general differences between the sexes for the total move time, move distance, marginal zone time, and stereotypy (Fig. 6a-c,e). However, females enter the dark side at a higher frequency than males during light/dark choice test (p = 0.006; 17.3 ± 1.1 in males vs 21.6 ± 1.1 entries in females) (Fig. 6f). In adults, males show increased rearing (p = 0.01) (Fig. 6d) and stereotypy movements compared with females (p = 0.007) (Fig. 6e). These sex differences are not observed in pubertal mice. These observations indicate that some of the sex-dependent anxiety-related behaviors do not exist at approximately puberty but can be observed in adulthood.
In addition, when control and Wa-1 mice are compared, Wa-1 show hyperactivity compared with controls in both males and females, which is indicated by the increased total move time (p = 0.002; 418.4 ± 4.2 s in control vs 438.7 ± 4.7 s in Wa-1) (Fig. 6a) and total move distance (p = 0.047; 2029.8 ± 57.7 cm in control vs 2203.7 ± 63.7 cm in Wa-1) (Fig. 7b) in Wa-1. This difference is mainly attributable to the control and Wa-1 behavioral differences in the adult groups and not in the pubertal groups. The total margin time, rearing, stereotypy, and the total dark side entries during light/dark choice test are normal in Wa-1 groups. The hyperactivity in the absence of other anxiety-related behavioral measures in male and female Wa-1 may indicate increased exploratory behavior that may be attributable to a reduced fear response to the novel environment.
Delay fear conditioning: training response
There are no overall differences in the training responses (Fig. 7a-c) or shock sensitivity between control and Wa-1 mice in pubertal and female groups (data not shown). However, in adult males, Wa-1 mice show reduced freezing responses during the initial 2 min exploration period (p = 0.037; 4.9 ± 0.7% in controls vs 2.6 ± 0.8% in Wa-1) and the second 1 min exploration period [conditioned response 2 (CR2); p = 0.006] (Fig. 7b). The general hyperactivity in Wa-1 males has been discussed in the context of open-field activity. These observations indicate that the adult male Wa-1 show reduced fear response to a novel environment and to foot shocks, despite the normal shock sensitivity (data not shown). It is important to note, however, that, at the end of the training (CR3), adult male Wa-1 show an equivalent freezing response compared with the controls (Fig. 7b), indicating that the Wa-1 mice can reach a similar level of fear conditioning as their wild-type counterparts.
Contextual and auditory learning
A fear conditioning deficit is observed only in adult male Wa-1 mice when compared with adult male controls (Fig. 7e,h). The contextual (-54%), pre-tone (-54%), and tone (-44%) freezing responses are significantly reduced in adult male Wa-1 compared with the controls (p = 0.001, 0.007, and 0.009, respectively). When the freezing responses are normalized to the freezing differences during initial 2 min training period, only the context and tone freezing responses are significantly reduced in male Wa-1 (p = 0.016 and 0.002). This result indicates that the pre-tone freezing difference is attributable, in part, to the original differences in the general motor activity level. Furthermore, adult Wa-1 male mice show declined tone learning even after the tone freezing response is normalized to the pre-tone freezing differences (p = 0.014).
The fear learning impairment is not evident in the pubertal (Fig. 7d,g) or in female (Fig. 7f,i) Wa-1, indicating comparable learning abilities between controls and Wa-1 before reaching adulthood and that the learning impairment is not expressed in adult females. These data indicate that the observed learning deficit in Wa-1 is both sex- and age-dependent.
Stress homeostasis: stress-induced hyperthermia
Because the stress response is an integral part of fear learning, we measured potential stress regulation differences using a stress-induced hyperthermia behavioral paradigm. In general, the baseline stress temperature level (T1) is significantly higher (+1.35°C) at 1 month compared with 3 months (p < 0.001) (Fig. 8, compare a, b with c, d), which is in agreement with the general heightened anxiety/fearful state predicted from the open-field activity and light/dark choice tests in pubertal mice. In addition, adult females show a significantly higher T1 (+0.89°C) than adult males (p = 0.016) (Fig. 8, compare c, d). However, no sex difference in T1 is observed for the pubertal groups. This finding is also in agreement with the previous observations that certain sex-dependent anxiety level differences are apparent only after puberty.
Ten minutes after the acute restraint stress, the stressed temperature level (T2) is higher in pubertal mice than in adult mice (p < 0.001; +1.41°C) (Fig. 8, compare a, b with c, d), but no overall stress response difference (dT) is detected between these two general age groups (Fig. 8e). Similarly, when male and female populations are compared, T2 is elevated in females compared with males in general (p < 0.001; +0.33°C) (Fig. 8, compare a, c with b, d). However, there is no sex difference in the dT.
When Wa-1 and control mice are compared, adult male Wa-1 mice show a significantly lower T2 than adult male controls (Fig. 8c). Similarly, dT is reduced in Wa-1 compared with control in adult males and females (Fig. 8e). The adult male Wa-1 mice show a greater reduction in dT (-35%) than the adult female Wa-1 (-24%). Neither T2 nor dT shows genotype dependency at 1 month. These results indicate that the Wa-1 mice have a reduced stress response during adulthood but not during puberty. Furthermore, this blunted stress response is more pronounced in male Wa-1 than in female Wa-1. The degree of stress response differences may explain the sex-dependent fear conditioning deficit in the mutants. In fact, the contextual and tone freezing responses are significantly correlated with T2 (Pearson's correlation = 0.764, 0.789; p < 0.0001 for both) and dT (Pearson's correlation = 0.556 and 0.604; p = 0.02 and 0.010, respectively). These findings suggest that the reduced learning ability in Wa-1 adult males may be attributable, in part, to their reduced stress response. Similarly, lack of a learning deficit in pubertal Wa-1 male and Wa-1 females may be attributable to a general enhancement of the stress response in pubertal and female mice, which may have a compensatory effect during stress-dependent learning.
Plasma corticosterone
To delineate the possible biochemical mechanisms underlying the abnormal stress response in adult Wa-1 mice, plasma corticosterone levels were measured using radioimmunoassay. Corticosterone levels are significantly elevated for both baseline and stressed samples in females compared with males (p = 0.02; p < 0.001) and during puberty compared with adulthood (Fig. 8f,g). However, the fold increase in stressed over baseline corticosterone levels is the same between males and females, which parallels the behavioral findings in which no difference in stress response (dT) was observed between males and females. No differences are detected between control and Wa-1 animals.
Plasma catecholamines
To delineate possible causes for the abnormal behavioral stress response in Wa-1, plasma catecholamine levels in adult groups were also determined using HPLC. Similar to the plasma corticosterone result, the plasma NA and A levels are higher in control females than control males (p = 0.003 and 0.008, respectively) (Fig. 9). In female Wa-1, both NA and A levels are reduced compared with the control females (p = 0.006 and 0.018, respectively). However, the A level is elevated, whereas NA level is unchanged in male Wa-1 compared with male control mice (p = 0.002) (Fig. 9). Furthermore, the ratio between NA and A is the same between control and Wa-1 females, whereas the ratio is greatly reduced in Wa-1 compared with control males (p = 0.033).
Discussion
Stress responsiveness as a correlate of learning in Wa-1
The stress response and fear learning impairments in Wa-1 occur predominantly in males and after pubertal onset. In fact, we observed fear learning impairment only in adult Wa-1 males. This learning deficit may be attributable, in part, to the abnormalities during the acquisition of fear (a reduced freezing response during CR2). Because Wa-1 mice, in general, exhibited hyposensitivity to stress, neither the novel training environment nor the foot shocks during the training period may be as salient for Wa-1 mice as for the control mice. Thus, the observed sex difference in learning in Wa-1 mice may be partly attributable to a greater suppression of stress response in male Wa-1 than female Wa-1 compared with their respective controls, which suggests that a threshold stress response must be achieved for comparable fear learning. Interestingly, female rats, in general, appear less sensitive than males to the impairing effects of stress and show enhanced performance after the stress duration that induces impairments in males (Luine et al., 1994; Bowman et al., 2001; Luine, 2002). Therefore, the sex-dependent penetrance of the mutant behavioral phenotype reported here may be partly attributable to the general sex differences in stress compensatory mechanisms and stress-dependent learning behaviors. In fact, the TGFα transcript levels in stress-regulating structures, such as hypothalamus, pituitary, and adrenal, and memory-associated structures, such as hippocampus and amygdala, show sex and age specificity in both control and Wa-1 mice (Koshibu and Levitt, 2005). Such differences could contribute to the distinct sex-dependent compensatory mechanisms for the genetic mutation observed here.
Interestingly, our stress response findings in the male adult Wa-1 are inversely correlated with the previous findings in which TGFα overexpressing transgenic mice on a CD1 strain exhibited hypersensitivity to stress (Hilakivi-Clarke et al., 1992, 1993). A lack of impairment reported in other anxiety tests, such as elevated plus maze, voluntary alcohol intake (Hilakivi-Clarke et al., 1992), and light/dark choice test (this study), indicates the specificity of anxiety and stress systems regulated by TGFα, particularly in males. Consistent with this view, we found no differences in plasma corticosterone levels between the control and Wa-1 mice. However, there were differences in plasma A concentration in both male and female Wa-1 and plasma NA concentration in female Wa-1 compared with their respective controls. These results indicate that the behavioral abnormalities associated with Wa-1 mice may involve adrenal medulla and sympathetic systems.
The fact that the pubertal Wa-1 mice do not show a learning deficit is not surprising, because there are no differences in stress or fear responses in pubertal mutants compared with the controls. The late onset of learning and stress response changes in Wa-1 may be attributable to the relative hypersensitivity to stress in pubertal mice compared with adult mice as discussed below. If the behavioral deficit in adult Wa-1 is, in fact, attributable to the reduced fear and stress responses, then a general increase in the fear and anxiety levels in pubertal mice may mask the mutant behavioral phenotype during puberty.
Anatomical correlates of stress and learning impairments in Wa-1
The enlargement of lateral ventricles and the reduction of hippocampus and striatum parallel several behavioral findings in the Wa-1 mice. These changes are uniquely expressed in adults, when the behavioral abnormalities are observed, and preferentially in males. In particular, the reduced hippocampal volume was only observed in male and not in female adult Wa-1. Because the hippocampus is involved in both stress regulation and fear learning (Radulovic et al., 1999; Roozendaal et al., 1999), the adult male-specific hippocampal alteration may, in part, underlie the pronounced stress and fear learning deficits in male Wa-1 mice. Interestingly, modest differences in hippocampal volume and shape distortions also have been reported in subjects with schizophrenia (Csernansky et al., 1998; Narr et al., 2001).
In pubertal mice, although some minor forebrain structural changes were observed in Wa-1 mice compared with control mice, no behavioral abnormalities were detected. This unique finding indicates that brain anatomical changes may precede the development of behavioral deficits later in life.
To delineate the possible involvement of gonadal steroids in producing the observed Wa-1 phenotypes, gonadectomy was conducted at approximately the onset of puberty (postnatal days 26-30), before detection of a behavioral phenotype. However, the interpretation of this experiment was complicated because the sham surgery alone eliminated the differences in stress response and fear conditioning between control and Wa-1 in both male and female adult mice (K. Koshibu and P. Levitt, unpublished observations). Additional experiments to manipulate gonadal hormones at earlier ages would be interesting, because minor anatomical changes are already present at puberty. Our TGFα and epidermal growth factor receptor mRNA expression studies indicate that surgeries done at 1 or 2 postnatal weeks (when we saw maximum expression of these transcripts in some structures) would be reasonable time points for further investigating the involvement of gonadal steroid in regulating sex- and age-dependent behavioral and anatomical differences in control and Wa-1 mice.
Attribution of the Wa-1 phenotype to TGFα
The enlarged ventricles and contextual fear conditioning deficit observed in adult Wa-1 males are in agreement with our previous study (Burrows et al., 2000). However, there are a few discrepancies between Burrows et al. (2000) and our current study, which may be attributable to strain and gene mutation differences. There are numerous studies indicating differential effects of strains on behavior (Alexander et al., 1996; Homanics et al., 1999; Rogers et al., 1999; Avgustinovich et al., 2000; Bolivar et al., 2000) and strain-dependent knock-out phenotypes (for review, see Gingrich and Hen, 2000). Therefore, the difference between our previous and current studies may be partly attributable to their strain background differences. It also is important to emphasize that the original strain of Wa-1 possessed additional mutations (APB/Le a/a Tyrplb/Tyrplb bt/bt p/p Bmp5SE/Bmp5SE) and that, in this study, the brain anatomy and fear conditioning characterization was achieved using more current methods. These factors also could have contributed to the apparent differences in our studies.
Interestingly, TGFα knock-out mice on a congenic C57BL/6J background do not express the behavioral or anatomical phenotypes evident in the Wa-1 mice of either background strain (Burrows et al., 2000). We conclude that the failure of constitutive elimination of TGFα gene to produce a CNS phenotype is likely to be attributable to different compensatory mechanisms invoked in the hypomorphic Wa-1 strain compared with the full null strain.
General sex- and age-dependent features
Some of our general age- and sex-mediated behavioral and anatomical findings correlate well with human findings. For example, the observed hypersensitivity to stress in pubertal mice is in agreement with the previous findings in humans (Allen and Matthews, 1997) and in rodents (Walker et al., 1995; Hascoet et al., 1999). In addition, sex-dependent stress regulation was observed mainly in adult mice and not in pubertal mice, indicating that certain stress regulation systems mature during and/or after puberty in a sex-dependent manner (Ramaley, 1972; Gabriel et al., 1992; Spinedi et al., 1997).
Interestingly, despite these age- and sex-dependent differences in anxiety and stress responses, no fear learning differences were observed among the control mice. These findings may indicate that, under normal circumstances, the different sensitivity to stress in males and females, at different developmental stages, is compensated to the same level during fear learning. When a genetic mutation is introduced, however, the compensatory mechanisms are disturbed, resulting in sex- and age-dependent stress-mediated learning impairments in mutants. The implications of these findings are that susceptibility to specific gene mutations and the expression of affected phenotypes are sex dependent. The reported differences between sexes in the disruption of cognition and behavior in neuropsychiatric and developmental disorders are consistent with this hypothesis (Nopoulos et al., 1997; Nagy et al., 2001; Gur et al., 2004).
The anatomical analyses revealed that both pubertal and female mice show reduced amygdala volume compared with adult and male mice, respectively. These age- and sex-associated amygdala changes appear to be phylogenetically conserved, paralleling reports in humans (Filipek et al., 1994; Giedd et al., 1997; Goldstein et al., 2001). Because the amygdala is a well known structure associated with emotion, our findings may reflect developmental maturation and sexual differentiation of amygdala-related functions, such as stress and anxiety.
Conclusion
Structural and functional human neuroimaging studies are being combined with endophenotype and genotype analyses to directly address structure-function relationships in neuropsychiatric diseases (for review, see Hariri and Weinberger, 2003; Mattay and Goldberg, 2004). The present study in mice highlights the phylogenetic conservation of fundamental differences between brain structure-function relationships in the context of sex-dependent penetration of neurological disorders, reflecting selective and complex regulation of brain development during puberty.
Footnotes
This work was supported in part by National Science Foundation Integrative Graduate Education and Research Traineeship Predoctoral Grant DGE-9987588 and the Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition at the University of Pittsburgh (K.K.) and National Institutes of Health Grant MH45507 and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grant HD015052 (P.L.). E.T.A. is supported by National Institutes of Health Grants RO1-EB003453, P41-EB001977, and P50-AR049617. We thank Drs. Allan Sampson, Mark Gamalo, Jennifer Blackford, and Warren Lambert for expert statistical consulting and Drs. Edda Thiels, Laura Lillien, Paula Monaghan-Nichols, Karl Kandler, Gregg Stanwood, Daniel Campbell, and Elizabeth Powell for valuable scientific discussions. We also thank Raymond Johnson for the technical assistance in HPLC analyses.
Correspondence should be addressed to either of the following: Pat Levitt, Vanderbilt Kennedy Center for Research on Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Box 40 Peabody, 230 Appleton Place, Nashville, TN 37203, E-mail: pat.levitt{at}vanderbilt.edu; or Kyoko Koshibu, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology/University of Zurich, Brain Research Institute, Bau 55 H38, Winterthuerstrasse 190, CH-8057, Zurich, Switzerland, E-mail: koshibu{at}hifo.unizh.ch.
Copyright © 2005 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/05/253870-11$15.00/0