Abstract
Synchronization of neuronal activity, often associated with network oscillations, is thought to provide a means for integrating anatomically distributed processing in the brain. Neuronal processing, however, involves simultaneous oscillations in various frequency bands. The mechanisms involved in the integration of such spectrally distributed processing have remained enigmatic. We demonstrate, using magnetoencephalography, that robust cross-frequency phase synchrony is present in the human cortex among oscillations with frequencies from 3 to 80 Hz. Continuous mental arithmetic tasks demanding the retention and summation of items in the working memory enhanced the cross-frequency phase synchrony among α (∼10 Hz), β (∼20 Hz), and γ (∼30-40 Hz) oscillations. These tasks also enhanced the “classical” within-frequency synchrony in these frequency bands, but the spatial patterns of α, β, and γ synchronies were distinct and, furthermore, separate from the patterns of cross-frequency phase synchrony. Interestingly, an increase in task load resulted in an enhancement of phase synchrony that was most prominent between γ- and α-band oscillations. These data indicate that cross-frequency phase synchrony is a salient characteristic of ongoing activity in the human cortex and that it is modulated by cognitive task demands. The enhancement of cross-frequency phase synchrony among functionally and spatially distinct networks during mental arithmetic tasks posits it as a candidate mechanism for the integration of spectrally distributed processing.
Introduction
A major challenge in neuroscience has been to elucidate the mechanisms that integrate spatially distributed processing and signal the relatedness of neurons coding different features of the same object (Singer, 1999). This “binding problem” is thought to be solved by synchronization of neurons into transient oscillatory assemblies (Singer, 1999; Varela et al., 2001). Indeed, synchronous oscillations in β-frequency (14-25 Hz) and γ-frequency (25-70 Hz) bands have been implicated in feature binding (Gray et al., 1989; Singer, 1999; Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999; Varela et al., 2001), and they have also been detected during working memory maintenance when sustained activation of neural representations is required (Tallon-Baudry et al., 2001; Pesaran et al., 2002). Therefore, it has been concluded that these oscillations may represent the contents of working memory. In humans, θ-band (4-7 Hz) (Raghavachari et al., 2001; Halgren et al., 2002) and α-band (7-14 Hz) (Halgren et al., 2002; Jensen et al., 2002) oscillations are also linked to working memory. However, they do not appear to underlie feature integration per se (Singer, 1999; Howard et al., 2003) but are instead involved in top-down modulation (von Stein et al., 2000; Fanselow et al., 2001; Mima et al., 2001). Hence, in contrast to the content-mediating β-/γ-band oscillations, the θ-/α-band oscillations serve top-down modulating, “attentional,” and other context-defining (Llinás et al., 1998) functions. Thus, there is the problem of how the various assemblies at different levels of functional hierarchy (cf. Hochstein and Ahissar, 2002) act in concert to support unified cognitive functions. Evidently, this demands dynamic interactions between activities in distinct frequency bands (Engel and Singer, 2001; VanRullen and Koch, 2003), but the nature of these interactions has remained empirically unresolved.
Bispectral measures such as bicoherence (Dumermuth et al., 1971; Jeffrey and Chamoun, 1994) have been used to detect nonlinear cross-frequency (CF) phase coupling in the cat cortex (Schanze and Eckhorn, 1997; von Stein et al., 2000) and in human electroencephalograms (EEGs) (Jeffrey and Chamoun, 1994; Shils et al., 1996; Schack et al., 2002). However, like conventional coherence (cf. Tass et al., 1998), bicoherence mixes phase and amplitude information. Hence, it leaves the specific form and, consequently, also the putative neuronal mechanisms of the phase coupling unclear. To date, two principal forms of CF phase interactions have been recognized: n:m phase synchrony, which indicates amplitude-independent phase locking of n cycles of one oscillation to m cycles of another oscillation (Tass et al., 1998), and nested oscillations, which reflect the locking of the amplitude fluctuations of faster oscillations to the phase of a slower oscillation (Vanhatalo et al., 2004). Although there is evidence for nested oscillations in the human brain (Schack et al., 2002; Bruns and Eckhorn, 2004; Vanhatalo et al., 2004) and in the rat hippocampus (Chrobak and Buzsaki, 1998), n:m phase synchrony has not been observed among cortical oscillations (cf. Tass et al., 1998, 2003). n:m phase synchrony is, nevertheless, an attractive means for achieving CF integration, because, unlike nested oscillations, it operates with the temporal precision of the faster oscillation and hence conserves the temporal codes therein.
In the present work, we used neuromagnetic recordings to investigate whether the human cortex exhibits CF (n:m) phase synchrony among ongoing oscillations. We then used continuous mental arithmetic tasks to ask whether cognitive operations are associated with changes in CF phase synchrony and to examine the notion that the contents-to-context binding involves phase synchrony between the constituent β-/γ-band and θ-/α-band oscillations.
Materials and Methods
Recordings and tasks. We recorded data from 17 healthy subjects using a whole-scalp magnetoencephalography (MEG) system with 204 gradiometer and 102 magnetometer channels (Elekta Neuromag, Helsinki, Finland). In all sessions, the subjects sat relaxed with their eyes closed in a dimly lit and acoustically insulated room and performed one of three uninterrupted mental tasks in a pseudorandom order, each lasting 20 min. The subject's head position in the MEG system was identical during all tasks with an accuracy of ∼3 mm, measured using three coils attached to the head. The recorded data were high- and low-pass filtered on-line at 0.03 and 100 Hz, respectively, sampled at 600 Hz, and decimated off-line to a sampling rate of 300 Hz.
In one session, the subjects were instructed to actively clear their imagined visual and auditory fields [active rest (R)]. During the other two tasks [task 1 (T1) and T2], they were instructed to visualize two or three numbers, respectively, that were given by the experimenter at the onset of task execution, calculate their sum, discard the first of the original numbers, and place the sum as the last number. This was iterated until the sum exceeded 100, after which the last two or three digits of the final sum were used as a new set of starting numbers (see Fig. 2 A). The task R thus minimized the number of simultaneously active neural representations as well as the multitude of cognitive processes requiring integration. T1 and T2, however, required the working memory system to operate near or at its 3 ± 1 object capacity limit (Luck and Vogel 1997) and also demanded the coordination of several cognitive operations including, for example, the simultaneous maintenance of several neural number representations and their arithmetical summation. Together, the comparison of R with T1/T2 reflects the gross difference between active rest and active cognitive processing, whereas the comparison between T1 and T2 reveals processes associated with increasing working memory load. The subjects were interviewed after the recordings, and they rated the task difficulty and subjective success in task performance. All tasks were reported to be difficult, and all demanded a highly attentive and aroused state.
Continuous working memory tasks (e.g., of n-back type) have been used previously in EEG (Gevins et al., 1997) and imaging (Carlson et al., 1998) studies. Unlike the n-back tasks, ours did not use external stimuli to avoid the complications of extensive stimulus-locked oscillations (Lachaux et al., 1999; Halgren et al., 2002). An obvious drawback of continuous tasks is that they do not allow the dissection of encoding, retention, and retrieval processes of working memory or, in our case, the dissection of working memory-specific processes from those underlying the mental arithmetic. Nevertheless, in continuous tasks, the lack of the temporal dimension greatly simplifies both the data analysis and visualization.
Wavelet filtering. The amplitude and phase of ongoing oscillations were obtained as a function of time using Morlet wavelets. Convolution X(t,f0) = x(t) × w(t,f0) of the signal x(t) with the Morlet wavelet w(t,f0) = Aexp(-t2/2σ2t)exp(2iπf0t), where σt = m′/2πf0 and i is the imaginary unit, gives a complex vector X(t,f0) of which the real part corresponds to the filtered signal x, modulus corresponds to the amplitude envelope, and angle corresponds to the phase of the signal x in a frequency band with a center frequency of f0 and a frequency-domain SD of σf = f0/m′. The constant m′ defines the compromise between time and frequency precision. The results shown in this study were essentially identical with values of m′ ranging from 6.0 to 8.5.
Narrow time-window filtering. Because of their Gaussian shape in both time and frequency domains, Morlet wavelets are limited to m′ values >5, which sets the minimum time-domain SD (σt) to ∼0.8 s for unit frequency. To obtain filter families with much less time-domain spread, we used combinations of high- and low-pass finite-impulse response (FIR) filters that were characterized by high-pass stop bands of 0.3 and low-pass stop bands of 3.0 for a center frequency of 1.0 (giving 3 dB attenuation at 0.8 and 1.65, respectively). The time-domain SD for these filters is ∼0.4 s for unit frequency. When using FIR filters, the imaginary part for the complex vector X(t,f) was obtained with the Hilbert transform. These filters were used in Figures 1 A-D and 3, A and C.
Phase synchrony. Oscillations at frequencies fx and fy, of which the ratio is given by the integers n and m so that nfx = mfy, may be considered n:m phase synchronized when their phase difference (ωn,m, ωn,m = nθx - mθy, where θx and θy are the phases of oscillations x and y) is not uniformly distributed (Tass et al., 1998). We evaluated the presence of non-random phase ordering using the phase-locking factor (PLF) for frequency ratios with n = 1 and m = 1-6. For N complex numbers zi, i = 1... N, with unit modulus, the PLF is given by .
Statistical evaluation of phase locking. The PLF of uncorrelated and uniformly distributed phases obeys the Rayleigh distribution and approaches zero in a log-log linear manner with a slope of -0.5 when N approaches infinity (data not shown). For analyses with narrow windows (see Fig. 1 A-D), the temporal redundancy (correlations between nearby samples) caused by the filters is considerable, and the confidence limits were obtained by estimating the surrogate distribution of PLFs with 200 randomly selected epochs of data with an identical window size. For analyses over whole recording sessions (see Figs. 1 E, F, 2, 3 A, C, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8), the value of PLFshuffled at Nmax was estimated for each gradiometer and frequency band by linear regression from averaged values of PLFshuffled at Nmax/10, Nmax/32, Nmax/100, and Nmax/320. Because the Rayleigh distribution is a function of its mean only, the extrapolated estimate of the PLFshuffled(Nmax) gives the confidence limits directly with the ratios PLFreal/PLFshuffled of 1.95 and 2.42 corresponding to p values of 0.05 and 0.01, respectively. All PLF values in this study are shown using this normalization. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used for the statistical evaluation of task effects. In addition, where thus stated, we used Bonferroni's correction (p→pB) for multiple statistical comparisons and/or exploited binomial statistics to estimate the probability (pbin) that the observed statistically significant effects would have occurred by chance. The binomial probability (pkout of n) of finding by chance k tests significant out of n possible is given by the following equation:
where p denotes the significance level. pbin used here is formulated as the probability of finding ko or more tests significant and hence is given by the following equation:
where ko denotes the observed number of tests exceeding the significance threshold (p < 0.01).
Quantification of CF amplitude correlations. The amplitude envelope values (obtained with Morlet wavelets) of two frequency bands were paired and sorted into 10 bins to obtain histograms of the mean amplitudes of X in the 10 percentiles of amplitude Y, and vice versa for mean amplitudes of Y as a function of the 10 percentiles of amplitude X. These histograms were normalized with the respective maximum variations of X and Y quantified with histograms of mean amplitudes of X in the 10 percentiles of amplitude X, and likewise for Y. After normalization, the sum over histogram X(Y), ranging from 0 to 1, reflects the dependency of X on Y and conversely for Y(X). The larger of these sums for each gradiometer and fx,m pair was selected and averaged over gradiometers and subjects (see Fig. 3B). The advantage of this approach (over, for example, Spearman's correlation coefficient) is that it allows for the evaluation of the form of interaction and also the discovery and quantification of non-monotonic (e.g., quadratic) interactions.
Topographical visualization of MEG data. Each MEG recording site contains two orthogonal gradiometers. For the topographical visualization of PLFs for within-gradiometer synchrony (see Figs. 1 F, 2 B), we used the gradiometer with the larger PLF. Correspondingly, for the visualization of between-gradiometers synchrony (see Figs. 4, 5), we used the gradiometer pair (of the four possible combinations) with the largest PLF. Hence, the matrices of phase synchrony between gradiometers (see Figs. 4, 6) are reduced to a size of 102 × 102 from the original 204 × 204. To highlight the topography of medium- and long-range connections in the visualization of interareal phase synchrony during T0 (see Fig. 4) despite the exponential decrease in synchrony with increasing pair separation (data not shown), we plotted 50 gradiometer pairs for each of the 10 incremental distance scales. The topographical maps of mean interareal phase synchrony (i.e., the color maps underneath the lines linking gradiometers in Fig. 4) were obtained by averaging all PLFs for the phase synchrony of a given gradiometer with all other gradiometers. This gives an approximate measure of the inter-regional differences in large-scale phase synchrony (see Fig. 4) and the task effects thereof (see Fig. 5). Note that highly significant task effects were observed between T2 and T1, although they were not shown by this measure (compare Figs. 5C,D, 6E-H, 8).
Results
Cross-frequency phase synchrony among cortical oscillations
Superimposed α-frequency (8-14 Hz) and β-/γ-frequency (14-50 Hz) band oscillations were readily observed in filtered gradiometer data (Fig. 1A) recorded during R (see Materials and Methods). We first addressed the presence of CF phase synchrony, which thus far has remained undiscovered in cortical activity. We obtained the continuous phase (Fig. 1B, red and blue traces) of the α and β/γ oscillations using the Hilbert transform, evaluated their phase difference (Fig. 1B, purple trace), and computed 1:m phase-locking factors in narrow sliding time windows (Fig. 1C,D) (PLF1:m; see Materials and Methods). We found that intermittent periods of 1:3 and 1:4 γ-α (Fig. 1A-D) as well as 1:2 β-α (data not shown) phase synchronies were strikingly prominent in the spontaneous activity of posterior brain areas.
To obtain the continuous phases of 3-80 Hz activities over whole recording sessions and within all MEG gradiometers, we then mapped the occurrence of CF phase synchrony by using Morlet wavelets and estimated the corresponding PLF1:m with m = 2-6. Figure 1E shows the percentage of gradiometers detecting statistically significant (p < 0.01) synchrony between oscillations at frequencies fx and fy (color scale) averaged over subjects (N = 17). The frequency of the faster oscillation (fx) is plotted on the y-axis, and the m factor that gives the frequency of the slower oscillation (fy) (fy = fx/m) is on the x-axis. Because the gradiometers are the most sensitive to neuronal sources directly underneath them (Hämäläinen et al., 1993), such within-channel CF phase synchrony reflects mainly local interactions in cortical networks. During rest, we found significant 1:2 or 1:3 phase synchronies between all analyzed frequency bands (Fig. 1E). Phase synchronies (1:2) were dominated by the locking of β-(∼20 Hz) and α-band (∼10 Hz) oscillations widely over the cortex but predominantly over occipitoparietal and central/somatomotor regions (Fig. 1F, left). However, 1:3 phase synchrony was found mainly between γ-band (∼30 Hz) and α-band oscillations and mostly over the occipitoparietal and somatomotor regions (Fig. 1F, right).
Mental arithmetic tasks enhance local CF phase synchrony
Next, we asked whether these patterns of CF phase synchrony would be affected by mental arithmetic tasks T1 and T2 (Fig. 2A) (see Materials and Methods). Both tasks strongly enhanced local 1:2 γ-β, 1:2 β-α, and 1:3 γ-α synchronies (Fig. 2B) compared with R. These effects were most pronounced over right-hemispheric parietal and temporal/prefrontal areas but were also highly significant over corresponding left-hemispheric areas (Fig. 2B). The differences in local CF phase synchrony between T1 and T2 were much more subtle and exhibited greater intersubject variability. Interestingly, however, T2 was associated with significantly stronger 1:3 γ-α synchrony than T1 over right-hemispheric parietal areas (average over nine parietal gradiometer locations: right hemisphere, 17%, p < 0.01; left hemisphere, 1.6%, p > 0.8; also see below, the T2 vs T1 data on interareal phase synchrony).
The contribution of artifactual harmonics to CF phase synchrony
The frequency content of oscillations with nonsinusoidal (spiky, for instance) waveforms is not limited to the band corresponding to the fundamental frequency of the oscillation. Such nonsinusoidal signals produce harmonic peaks in power spectra and CF phase synchrony that is artifactual in the sense that it does not reflect an interaction between two oscillations at distinct frequencies. Nonsinusoidal waveforms, in contrast, also arise when two interacting oscillations at different frequencies are actually present in the data. We addressed whether the present observations of CF synchrony can be accounted for by artifactual harmonics.
First, visual inspection of the data revealed that, during periods of prominent CF phase synchrony, the faster oscillation was also detectable in broadband-filtered data, indicating that it did not arise as a filter artifact from the slower oscillation (Fig. 1A). Second, we estimated CF phase synchrony over recording sessions using the Hilbert transform and temporally very narrow, broad-pass-band filters (Fig. 3A,B) (see Materials and Methods). The results were comparable with those obtained with Morlet wavelets, whereas CF phase synchrony caused by filter artifacts should have been dramatically weaker (compare Figs. 1E, 3A and Figs. 2B, 3B). Third, we estimated the strength and form of CF amplitude correlations within gradiometers with cross-amplitude histograms (Fig. 3C) and Spearman's rank-order coefficients (data not shown). The CF amplitude correlations were, in general, extremely weak, albeit positive (Fig. 3C). If the detection of faster oscillations was strongly biased by nonsinusoidal properties of the slower oscillations, the amplitudes of faster oscillations should have been tightly linked to the amplitude of the slower oscillations. Moreover, in contrast to the band-specific enhancements of CF phase synchrony (Fig. 2B), T1 and T2 robustly weakened the amplitude correlations between all frequency pairs below the γ band compared with R (Fig. 3D). This indicates that the enhancement of CF phase synchronies during T1 and T2 was not attributable to changes in α waveform.
Together, several lines of evidence indicate that CF phase synchronies reported in this study do indeed reflect the interactions of networks oscillating in distinct frequency bands.
Interareal phase synchrony
The data above (Figs. 1, 2, 3) showed that CF phase synchrony is present among cortical oscillations and that it is modulated by differences in cognitive operations. To obtain a more complete picture of its characteristics, we then addressed phase synchrony between anatomically distinct areas. We quantified 1:2 and 1:3 phase synchronies of 10, 20, and 30 Hz oscillations for all pairs of the MEG gradiometers as well as the “classical” 1:1 synchronization within these frequency bands. In rest, 1:1 phase synchrony was extremely robust for all frequency bands analyzed and highly significant for essentially every gradiometer pair (Fig. 4A). In contrast, 1:2 and especially 1:3 phase synchronies were weaker and more local (Fig. 4B). Furthermore, although 1:1 phase synchronies were rather homogeneously distributed over the cortex, 1:2 and 1:3 phase synchronies were concentrated over posterior cortical areas (Fig. 4, matrices). Such qualitative differences in the distributions of 1:1 and 1:2/1:3 phase synchronies suggest that the mechanisms and functions of CF and 1:1 phase synchronies are indeed distinct.
It should be noted that the magnetic field gradients produced by a focal cortical source are detected by several neighboring gradiometers that are located side by side above the source (Hämäläinen et al., 1993). Therefore, such signal spreading may contribute to observations of cross-channel synchrony between adjacent or very nearby gradiometers.
Task effects on interareal phase synchrony
The execution of T1 and T2, compared with R, enhanced interareal 1:1 phase synchronies in all frequency bands (Fig. 5A), although the strengthening of 10 Hz phase synchrony was clearly most pronounced and widespread (Fig. 6A,B). The 10 Hz phase synchrony was enhanced predominantly among posterior and anterior regions (Fig. 6A,B) and showed the strongest task effects at long distances [i.e., far from the matrix diagonal (Fig. 6A,B)]. We also plotted, as a function of gradiometer separation, the mean phase synchrony within (solid colors) and between (two-colored lines) the gradiometers in four quadrants of the MEG helmet (Fig. 7). These graphs corroborate the findings above and further highlight the central role of right-hemispheric posterior regions in large-scale 10 Hz synchrony (Fig. 7A, red lines in the 10-10 Hz row). A conspicuous feature of T1/T2-related 10 Hz phase synchrony was the marginality of task effects on synchrony within and between left-hemispheric posterior and anterior regions, despite the fact that these regions were massively synchronized with the right hemisphere (Figs. 6A, lower-left quadrant of the 10 Hz matrix, 7A, yellow and green lines). Phase synchronies (20 and 30 Hz), on the other hand, were predominantly found within posterior and between posterior and anterior brain regions (Fig. 5A) but much less so within anterior regions (Figs. 6A,B, matrix corners, 7A, blue and green lines) and with much less prominent right-hemispheric dominance than that observed for the 10 Hz phase synchrony (Fig. 6A).
Interareal 1:2 β-α and 1:3 γ-α synchronies were also considerably stronger during T1 and T2 than during R (Fig. 5B). Surprisingly, the interareal CF phase synchronies, 1:2 β-α in particular, were enhanced even more than the 1:1 phase synchronies (Fig. 6, compare A, B with C, D; note the different range for β-α synchrony). Moreover, in stark contrast to the 1:1 phase synchronies, the CF phase synchronies were most strongly enhanced on short distances (Fig. 7B) and over the right hemispheric posterior regions (Fig. 6C,D), providing additional evidence for the fundamentally distinct characters of 1:1 and CF phase synchronies. The strongest long-range CF phase synchronies were found between right posterior and anterior regions as well as between right and left posterior regions (Fig. 7B, blue-red and red-yellow lines, respectively).
The differences in interareal 1:1 (Fig. 5C) and CF (Fig. 5D) synchronies between T1 and T2 were much weaker than between T1/T2 and R (Fig. 5, compare A, B). However, although the differences were relatively small, 1:3 γ-α phase synchrony was clearly stronger during T2 than during T1 (Fig. 6G,H), mainly over right parietal regions (Figs. 6G,H, 8B, red line). In contrast, the massive 1:2 β-α synchrony showed only a small difference between T1 and T2, suggesting that the functional roles of these 1:2 and 1:3 phase synchronies are distinct. In addition, 1:1 phase synchronies were significantly stronger in T2 than in T1 (Fig. 6E,F) but were enhanced less than the 1:3 γ-α synchrony (Fig. 8, compare A, B). Interestingly, 10 Hz 1:1 phase synchrony was enhanced within the left hemisphere and bilaterally within the anterior regions, apparently linking and recruiting specifically those areas that were the least involved in the task execution in T1. Because T1 and T2 differed essentially only in the number of simultaneously memorized object representations, these data strongly suggest that 1:3 γ-α phase synchrony has a role in the neuronal mechanisms of working memory maintenance.
Task effects on oscillation amplitudes
To relate the data obtained with the present tasks to previous studies using working memory paradigms, we evaluated the oscillation amplitudes with Morlet wavelets for all frequency bands in Figures 1, 2, 3. The results showed that amplitudes of α (Bastiaansen et al., 2002; Halgren et al., 2002; Jensen et al., 2002) and β/γ (Tallon-Baudry et al., 1998; Pesaran et al., 2002; Howard et al., 2003) bands were slightly but significantly increased in T1 and T2 compared with R (Fig. 9). The amplitude enhancements were mainly observed over occipitoparietal and temporal/prefrontal areas. Interestingly, the amplitudes of θ-band (3-8 Hz) oscillations were enhanced over prefrontal regions (Gevins et al., 1997; Howard et al., 2003) but were suppressed widely over the posterior regions (cf. Bastiaansen et al., 2002).
Discussion
Although the well established “temporal correlation hypothesis” (Singer, 1999) underscoring within-frequency synchrony has been successful in explaining neuronal integration at the level of feature binding, the demand for CF interactions in higher-level neuronal processing has also been recognized (Lisman and Idiart, 1995; Engel and Singer, 2001; Varela et al., 2001; VanRullen and Koch, 2003). Relatively few studies have thus far investigated nonlinear (CF) phase coupling (Jeffrey and Chamoun, 1994; Shils et al., 1996; Schanze and Eckhorn, 1997; von Stein et al., 2000; Schack et al., 2002) or nested oscillations (Chrobak and Buzsaki, 1998; Schack et al., 2002; Bruns and Eckhorn, 2004; Vanhatalo et al., 2004). We have shown here that, among cortical oscillations, CF phase interactions are also found in the form of 1:m-type CF phase synchrony. CF phase synchrony was a salient characteristic of ongoing activity in the human cortex, and it was strongly influenced by differences in cognitive task demands, supporting its role in higher-level neuronal processing. Although future studies will need to pinpoint its specific functional roles, several lines of evidence support an idea that CF phase synchrony is a plausible mechanism for the integration of spectrally distributed processing. Unlike CF amplitude correlations and nested oscillations, it provides a way to integrate spectrally distributed processing with the temporal precision of the faster oscillation.
The enhancement of CF phase synchrony by cognitive processes
CF phase synchrony was robust and present during each task among all frequency bands analyzed. However, mental calculation and working memory demands of T1 and T2 enhanced specifically the 1:2-1:4 phase synchronies among α, β, and γ oscillations. The enhancements of the 1:1 phase synchrony, the oscillation amplitudes, and the distinct spatial patterns of 1:1 synchrony of these oscillations, imply, in good agreement with other evidence (Singer, 1999; Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999; von Stein et al., 2000; Varela et al., 2001; Howard et al., 2003), that α, β, and γ oscillations are all involved in the performance of T1 and T2 but with distinct functional roles. Thus, CF phase synchrony among these oscillations is well posed to mediate the integration of this spectrally distributed processing into the cognitive operations required by mental calculations and working memory.
The role of 1:3 γ-α phase synchrony in working memory
The mental arithmetic tasks in this study affected the amplitude, 1:1 phase synchrony, and hemispheric lateralization of ongoing neuronal activity in a manner very similar to that observed previously in working memory tasks, which is well in line with the notion that the principal task load during easy mental calculation is on the working memory system (Houdé and Tzourio-Mazoyer, 2003). Nevertheless, the comparison of task T1/T2 and rest does not allow the dissection of working memory-related activities from those involved in, for example, the mental calculation, but reveals the overall differences between minimal and near-maximal cognitive load. However, the comparison of tasks T1 and T2 reveals specifically the components sensitive to working memory load. It is therefore intriguing to find that 1:3 γ-α phase synchrony was moderately but highly significantly stronger during T2 than during T1. This result suggests that 1:3 synchronized γ and α oscillations may play a role in the neuronal mechanisms of working memory. Taking the proposed “representational” (Singer, 1999; Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999) and attentional (von Stein et al., 2000; VanRullen and Koch, 2003) roles of β/γ and α oscillations, the results suggest that such 1:3 synchronization of these oscillations may mediate the contents-to-context binding of neuronal object representations into working memory and/or the multiplexing of these object representations, which is required when multiple representations must be kept active simultaneously.
Parietal focus of CF phase synchrony
β-α and γ-α phase synchronies were strongest over the right hemispheric parietal regions, as were their task-related enhancements. At the same time, the corresponding 1:1 synchronies were widely distributed over the cortex. In addition to the fact that these results support the differential origin of CF phase and within-frequency synchronies, they also support the notion that CF phase synchronies are related to attentional and working memory functions. The right-hemispheric parietal regions have been shown to play a critical role in attention and working memory by imaging studies (Corbetta et al., 1998; Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Awh and Jonides, 2001; Linden et al., 2003) and by the diversity of attentional deficits caused by parietal damage (Friedman-Hill et al., 1995; Driver and Vuilleumier, 2001). In addition, the brain areas specifically responsible for multiplexing functions (Friedman-Hill et al., 1995) are located, at least partly, in the parietal regions, as suggested again both by imaging studies localizing cortical activity correlated with working memory capacity (Linden et al., 2003) and by findings that damage in parietal regions can result in an inability to perceive multiple objects simultaneously (simultagnosia) (Friedman-Hill et al., 1995).
Putative cellular-level mechanisms involved in CF phase synchrony
The concept of “dynamic binding” posits that the postsynaptic impact of “bound” presynaptic inputs is greater than the impact of “unbound” ones (Singer, 1999). Although excitatory inputs synchronized in a 1:1 manner have better chances of evoking action potentials in downstream target neurons than asynchronous inputs (Konig et al., 1996; Singer, 1999), little is known about the functional significance of 1:m (m > 1) synchronization in neuronal signaling.
Interestingly, a type of neocortical layer 5 (L5) pyramidal cell with dendrites spanning all cortical layers has characteristics that are relevant in this context. The main excitatory connections to proximal and basal dendrites of these cells originate from specific thalamic nuclei (Jones, 2002), from local layer 4 spiny stellate cells, and, overall, from cortical areas at the same level or lower in hierarchy (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991). In contrast, inputs to distal apical dendrites primarily arise from diverse nonspecific thalamic regions (Jones, 2002) and from cortical areas higher in hierarchy (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991; Cauller et al., 1998). L5 pyramidal cells thus receive bottom-up and top-down signals in distinct integrative compartments, and both proximal (bottom-up) and distal (top-down) inputs alone, if sufficiently strong, can evoke action potentials or burst firing (Helmchen et al., 1999; Larkum et al., 1999; Williams and Stuart, 1999; Larkum and Zhu, 2002). However, burst firing is facilitated if the proximal and distal inputs are temporally correlated with a precision of ∼5 ms (Larkum et al., 1999). Because bursts have a much stronger impact on postsynaptic neurons than single action potentials (Lisman, 1997; Williams and Stuart, 1999), L5 pyramidal cells can effectively signal the coincidence of bottom-up and top-down signals and thus are able to mediate cross-hierarchical integration.
Although the feedforward/bottom-up inputs to L5 pyramidal cells often involve γ-band rhythmicity (Steriade et al., 1996; Castelo-Branco et al., 1998; Singer, 1999; von Stein et al., 2000), the feedback/top-down signals are more likely to have periodicities in the α band (von Stein et al., 2000; Mima et al., 2001; Halgren et al., 2002; Yamagishi et al., 2003). L5 pyramidal cells can also entrain each other (Silva et al., 1991) as well as their subcortical targets in the brainstem (Nicolelis et al., 1995) and superior colliculus (Brecht et al., 1998) into ∼10 Hz oscillations. Moreover, the regenerative Ca2+ action potentials underlying distal dendritic excitation (Helmchen et al., 1999; Larkum et al., 1999; Larkum and Zhu, 2002) cannot occur at frequencies more than ∼10 Hz (Larkum and Zhu, 2002). The narrowness of the correlation window (Larkum et al., 1999) indicates that oscillatory proximal and distal inputs in 20-40 and <10 Hz bands, respectively, can readily induce burst firing only when they are phase synchronized in a 1:m manner. This implies that the L5 pyramidal neurons can function as “read-out devices” for 1:m phase synchrony. Moreover, this kind of mechanism would also enable an unambiguous and rapid attentional selection of target representations from among other intermittent assemblies. Therefore, it is of interest to note that many of the subcortical targets of L5 pyramidal cells, such as the pulvinar and superior colliculus, have a critical role in attention and visual orienting (Karnath et al., 2002).
Another type of neuron that deserves attention in this context is fast rhythmic-bursting (FRB) cells. In a manner akin to L5 pyramidal neurons, they can switch from single-spiking to burst-firing mode, in which they show very fast (300-600 Hz) bursts at γ (30-40 Hz) frequencies (Steriade et al., 1998, 2004). Notably, FRB cells, located in all cortical layers from 2 to 6, are key elements in the reciprocal thalamocortical loops that are involved in the generation of β-/γ-frequency band oscillations (Steriade et al., 1996, 1998).
Conclusion
We propose that cross-frequency phase synchrony mediates cross-hierarchical and cross-functional integration essential for unified cognitive operations by enabling the integration of spectrally distributed neuronal processing.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Academy of Finland, the Sigrid Juselius Foundation, the Foundation for the Advancement of Technology, the Oskar Öflund Foundation, and the Ella and Georg Ehrnrooth Foundation. We are grateful to Gábor Tamás, Christopher Bailey, and Sampsa Vanhatalo for helpful comments on previous versions of this paper.
Correspondence should be addressed to J. Matias Palva, Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, P.O. Box 65 (Viikinkaari 1), University of Helsinki, FIN-00014 Helsinki, Finland. E-mail: matias.palva{at}helsinki.fi.
Copyright © 2005 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/05/253962-11$15.00/0