Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Competitive Stimulus Interactions within Single Response Fields of Superior Colliculus Neurons

Xiaobing Li and Michele A. Basso
Journal of Neuroscience 7 December 2005, 25 (49) 11357-11373; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3825-05.2005
Xiaobing Li
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michele A. Basso
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

In addition to its role in saccade generation, the superior colliculus (SC) is involved in target selection, saccade selection, and shifting the focus of spatial attention. Here, we investigated the influence of saccade selection on sensory interactions within single response fields (RFs) of SC neurons. One or two differently shaped stimuli were presented within single RFs of SC neurons, and the shape of a centrally located cue indicated whether and where to make a saccade (Go-Go) or whether to make or withhold a saccade (Go/No-Go). We found that, when two stimuli appeared at different locations within a single RF, SC neuronal activity was reduced compared with when a single stimulus appeared in isolation within the center of the RF in both the Go-Go and Go/No-Go tasks. In both tasks, a subsequent cue indicating one stimulus as a saccade target reduced the influence of the second stimulus located within the RF. We found that the time course of the suppression resulting from the two stimuli was ∼130 ms, a time close to that seen in cortex. Finally, we found that the influence of two stimuli within single RFs of SC neurons changed over time in both the Go-Go and the Go/No-Go tasks. Initially, the neurons averaged the influence of two stimuli. As the trial progressed, the SC neurons signaled only the saccade vector that was produced. We conclude that cues to shift gaze, like attention, modulate the influence of sensory interactions, providing additional support for the linkage between attention and saccade selection.

  • saccade
  • spatial attention
  • target selection
  • population coding
  • decision
  • electrophysiology

Introduction

The superior colliculus (SC) plays a critical role in eye movement generation and the preceding events such as visual target selection for saccades (Basso and Wurtz, 1998; Port and Wurtz, 2003; McPeek and Keller, 2004), for smooth pursuit (Krauzlis and Dill, 2002; Carello and Krauzlis, 2004), saccade selection (Glimcher and Sparks, 1992), saccade preparation (Dorris et al., 1997), shifting the focus of spatial attention (Goldberg and Wurtz, 1971, 1972b; Cavanaugh and Wurtz, 2004; Ignashchenkova et al., 2004; Muller et al., 2005), and perceptual decision making (Horwitz and Newsome, 1999, 2001). Psychophysical experiments demonstrate a strong coupling between saccades and shifts of spatial attention (Kowler, 1990; Kowler and Blaser, 1995; Kowler et al., 1995; Deubel and Schneider, 1996; Schneider and Deubel, 2002), suggesting that physiological mechanisms underlying selection for attention and selection for action are shared. How this is implemented within the SC is unknown and is the focus of the current report.

A conceptual model of attention developed from studies in visual areas V4 and inferotemporal cortex IT is called the biased competition model (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Reynolds et al., 1999). This model has two major predictions. First, when two stimuli appear in a single response field (RF), they will compete. If a single stimulus produces a strong response in a neuron, the addition of a second, weaker stimulus will reduce the response. Second, when attention is directed to one of the two stimuli, the neuronal activity will be dominated by the response of the neuron to that stimulus when it is presented in isolation. Thus, attention appears to modulate sensory responses in cortex (Luck et al., 1997; Reynolds et al., 1999, 2000). Recent experiments in SC and frontal eye field (FEF) using multiple stimuli with large spatial separations demonstrate that neuronal responses are reduced compared with when a single stimulus appears in isolation (Schall, 1991, 1995; Schall et al., 1995; Basso and Wurtz, 1998; McPeek and Keller, 2002), similar to observations made in extrastriate cortex (Motter, 1993, 1994b; Treue and Maunsell, 1996; Luck et al., 1997; Recanzone et al., 1997). In SC, when a peripheral cue indicates which stimulus will be a saccade target, neuronal activity increases to levels measured in single stimulus trials (Basso and Wurtz, 1998), suggesting that a cue to shift gaze can influence sensory interactions arising from the surrounding stimuli.

Here, we test predictions of the biased competition model (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Reynolds et al., 1999) in individual SC neurons. For SC, the relevant stimulus dimension is location, so we adapted feature attention tasks (Reynolds et al., 1999) to visually guided saccade tasks to determine whether the biased competition model could be generalized to spatial location within the RF of SC neurons. The results demonstrate stimulus interactions influencing individual SC neurons. The interactions were modified by cues to shift gaze. Our observations are consistent with the hypothesis that cues to shift gaze, like attention, modulate the influence of sensory interactions, providing additional support for the linkage between attention and saccade selection.

Materials and Methods

Physiological procedures

For electrophysiological recording of single neurons and monitoring eye movements, cylinders and eye coils were implanted in four rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) using standard procedures as described previously (Basso and Wurtz, 2002). Anesthesia was induced initially with an intramuscular injection of ketamine (5.0-15.0 mg/kg). Atropine (0.5 mg/kg) was provided to minimize salivation. Monkeys were intubated and maintained at a general anesthetic level with isofluorane. A subconjunctival eye coil was implanted (Judge et al., 1980). A plastic head holder for restraint and a cylinder for subsequent microelectrode recording were mounted on the top of the exposed skull and secured with titanium screws and dental acrylic. Plastic hardware allowed subsequent magnetic resonance images to be obtained with minimal artifact. For access to the SC, the recording cylinder was placed stereotaxically on the midline and angled 38° back so that the electrode penetrations were directed caudorostrally, toward the SC. An antibiotic (Cefadroxil, 25 mg/kg) was given 1 d before the operation and every day for a minimum of 4 d after the operation. Analgesia was provided by the administration of buprenorphine (0.01-0.03 mg/kg) and Flunixin (1-2 mg/kg) for 48 h postsurgically as needed. Monkeys recovered for 1-2 weeks before behavioral and physiological recording commenced. All experimental protocols were approved by the University of Wisconsin-Madison Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and complied with or exceeded standards set by the Public Health Service policy on the humane care and use of laboratory animals.

General behavioral procedures

We used a real-time experimental data acquisition and visual stimulus generation system (Tempo and VideoSync; Reflective Computing, St. Louis, MO) to create the behavioral paradigms and acquire eye position and single neuron data. Monkeys were trained to sit in a customdesigned primate chair with head fixed during the experimental session (typically 3-5 h). Visual stimuli were rear projected on a tangent screen at 51 cm distance using a digital light projector (DLP) (LP335; Infocus, Wilsonville, OR) with a native resolution of 1024 × 768 and operating at 60 Hz. The background luminance was 0.28 cd/m2. The visual stimulus presentation was controlled by VideoSync software (Reflective Computing) running on a dedicated personal computer (PC) with a 1024 × 768 VGA video controller (Computer Boards). The PC was a slave device to the PC used for experimental control and data acquisition. Because there is an inherent time limitation in DLPs (both the vertical refresh rate and the vertical synch pulse), a photocell was placed on the tangent screen, which sent a signal [a transistor-transistor logic (TTL) pulse] to the experimental PC providing an accurate measure of stimulus onset.

Behavioral tasks

Control task.Because we used different shapes (triangle and square) in our tasks, we initially measured neuronal activity during performance of this task to ensure that SC neurons were not shape selective. The same number of white pixels made up each isoluminant stimulus (2.17 cd/m2). The size of the stimuli was scaled with the distance from the fovea, either 0.6° for distances <15° or 0.9° for distances >15°. Initially, monkeys looked at a centrally located fixation spot for 800-1500 ms. Then, either a triangle or a square was displayed on the screen. The target was placed in the optimal location of the RF of the neuron under study (see below for RF mapping). At the end of this second delay (800-1000 ms), the fixation point was removed, and this cued the monkeys to make a saccade to the target. Monkeys were required to maintain eye position at the target location for 400-600 ms. If they maintained accuracy within a 2° square electronic window (if the target was farther than 10°, the window was increased to 3-4° square), they received a liquid reward. Square and triangle trials were randomized. In this task, before training on the other behavioral tasks commenced, we recorded seven buildup and eight visual-tonic neurons. A visual (0-200 ms from target onset), a delay (300-800 ms after target onset), and a saccade (100 ms before to 100 ms after the onset of a saccade) interval were measured to compare the neuronal activity in the two conditions. No SC neuron showed any significant differences in these two conditions in any of the intervals measured (t test, p > 0.05; data not shown). Thus, not surprisingly, we concluded that our stimuli were neutral to SC neurons before any training in the tasks.

  Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Behavioral tasks used to explore competitive stimulus interactions. a, Go-Go task. Along the top, the bars labeled “fixation on,” “stimuli on,” and “cue” depict the temporal sequence of the tasks. A schematic of the eye position is shown below and is labeled “eye.” The different trial types are shown next. 1T, One triangle presented with the triangle cued; 1S, one square presented with the square cued; 2T, triangle and square presented with the triangle cued; 2S, triangle and square presented with the square cued. This task is a visually guided, delayed-saccade task. The large squares depict the screen the monkeys' viewed, and the small dashed squares indicate the electronic window that determined fixation accuracy. Saccade accuracy windows are not shown to minimize clutter. b, Go/No-Go task. The arrangement of the figure is the same as in a. Note in this version of the task that the monkey remained fixating when the square cue appeared at the center of the screen.

Go-Go task. After the onset of a centrally located fixation spot for a random time of 1000-1500 ms, either one or two shapes were presented within a single RF of an SC neuron (see below, Response field mapping). The fixation point remained illuminated along with the peripheral stimuli for a random delay of 800-1000 ms, and then the fixation point changed shape to either a triangle or a square (Fig. 1a). After another 800-1000 ms delay, the fixation point turned back to its original spot, and this was the cue for the monkey to make a saccade to the cued stimulus. If the monkey acquired the target and its eye position remained at that location for 400-600 ms, a liquid reward was provided. The accuracy criterion was 2° square around the target (if the target was farther than 10°, the criterion was increased to 3-4° square around the target). The smallest distance between two stimuli was ∼2°, whereas the largest was ∼25°. Importantly the separation between the two stimuli exceeded the size of the windows to discourage averaging saccades and encourage precise saccades.

Each experimental session consisted of four trial types presented in two conditions. The trial types were presented in blocks in one condition (“blocked”) and randomly in a second condition (“interleaved”). One trial type was a triangle presented in the RF with a triangle cue presented at the fovea (1T). A second type was a square presented in the RF with a square cue presented at the fovea (1S). A third type presented both the triangle and square stimuli in the RF with a triangle cue at the fovea (2T). In the fourth type, both the triangle and square were presented in the RF, and a square cue was at the fovea (2S). We collected, on average, 20-50 trials for each type.

Go/No-Go task. This task was identical to the Go-Go task, except here, the triangle served as a cue for the Go condition, whereas a square indicated the No-Go condition (Fig. 1b). The square cue indicated that the monkey had to remain fixating at the central spot for 400-600 ms to obtain a fluid reward. In the Go trials, saccades had to be accurate, and we ensured this by making the acceptance criterion defined by the electronic windows nonoverlapping. Thus, averaging saccades were discouraged. We collected, on average, 20-50 trials for each trial type in this condition.

Response field mapping

A stimulus was moved around to assess qualitatively the boundary and the center of the visual RF of SC neurons. We then placed the triangle stimulus in the approximate center of the RF and a second stimulus (square) at any location around the center but, importantly, within the boundaries of the RF. The placement of the second stimulus was determined empirically by listening and watching the neuronal discharge online. Because we were interested in accessing stimulus interactions, we placed the second stimulus within a region of the RF that was likely to produce suppression, if it existed. However, during data analysis, we discovered that, in some cases, the two stimuli resulted in enhanced responses in the averaged discharge (these neurons can be identified on the population plots in Results). The separation between the two stimuli was scaled by the diameter of the visual RF. For example, if the diameter of the field was ∼5°, the two stimuli would be located ∼2° apart; if the diameter was ∼10°, then the two stimuli would be located ∼5° apart. We also placed the two stimuli approximately equidistant from the fixation point to keep the amplitude of the vector constant. We did not use neurons with RFs that included the fovea. For some neurons with large RFs, we were unable to determine exactly the distal boundaries of the RF.

Neuronal classification

We classified neurons as either buildup or visual-tonic in the single target, visually guided saccade task using the following statistical criteria. We computed a baseline measure of activity (200 ms before the onset of the visual target), a delay period activity (400 ms before a cue), and a saccade period activity (100 ms before saccade onset). Using only saccade correct trials, we defined buildup neurons as those neurons with a significantly greater activity in the delay period compared with the baseline (t test, p < 0.05) and a significantly greater activity in the saccade period compared with the delay period (t test, p < 0.05). If a neuron had a visual response and had a significantly greater level of activity in the delay period than the baseline (t test, p < 0.05) but had no significant difference between the saccade period and the delay period, we classified the neuron as a visual-tonic neuron. Examples of each type are shown in Figure 2.

Data acquisition and analysis

Using the magnetic search coil technique (Fuchs and Robinson, 1966) (Riverbend Instruments, Birmingham, AL), voltage signals proportional to horizontal and vertical components of eye position were filtered (eight-pole Bessel, -3 dB, 180 Hz), digitized at 16-bit resolution, and sampled at 1 kHz (CIO-DAS1602/16; Measurement Computing, Middleboro, MA). The data were saved for off-line analysis using an interactive computer program designed to display and measure eye position and calculate eye velocity. We used an automated procedure to define saccadic eye movements by applying velocity and acceleration criteria of 50°/s and 5000°/s2, respectively. The adequacy of the algorithm was verified on a trial-by-trial basis by the experimenter. Single neurons were recorded with tungsten microelectrodes (Frederick Haer Company, Bowdoinham, ME) with impedances between 0.3 and 1.0 MΩ measured at 1 kHz. Electrodes were aimed at the SC through stainless steel guide tubes held in place by a plastic grid secured to the cylinder (Crist et al., 1988). Action potential waveforms were identified with a window discriminator (Bak Electronics, Germantown, MD) that returned a TTL pulse for each waveform that met voltage and time criteria. The TTL pulses were sent to a digital counter (PC-TIO-10; National Instruments, Austin, TX) and were stored with a 1 ms resolution.

  Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Examples of SC neurons. a, An SC buildup neuron during a visually guided saccade. Along the top, eye position traces are plotted against time. The dashed, horizontal line indicates primary position. An upward deflection in the position traces is right, and a downward deflection is left. Traces on the left are aligned on target onset, and traces on the right are aligned on saccade onset. The alignment is indicated by the dashed, vertical line and arrowhead. Below the eye position traces are the raster plots and the superimposed spike density functions (σ = 15 ms). Each tick in the raster represents an action potential, and each row of ticks represents an individual trial in which the monkey made a delayed, visually guided saccade. b, Arrangement is the same as in a, for an SC visual-tonic neuron.

Statistical analyses and curve fits were performed using Matlab (Math-Works, Natick, MA). Standard parametric descriptive and inferential (ANOVA, t tests with modified Bonferroni corrections) statistics were used (Keppel, 1991). If the data failed to pass normality tests, nonparametric statistics were used. Wilcoxon's signed-rank test was used for sample data analyses. To compare the time course of neuronal activity changes in the different stimulus conditions, we computed receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves based on signal detection theory (Green and Swets, 1966), similar to that performed by others (Cohn et al., 1975; Bradley et al., 1987; Britten et al., 1992; Thompson et al., 1996). For each trial, we divided individual spike trains recorded in the two conditions into 5 ms bins and averaged the discharge rate within the bin. We then computed the probability that the discharge rate exceeded a criterion. The criterion was incremented from 0 to the maximum discharge rate for each 5 ms bin, and a probability value was computed for each criterion. Therefore, a single point on the ROC curve is produced for each increment in the criterion, and the entire ROC curve is generated from all of the criteria. The area under the ROC curve is a measure of the separation between the two distributions, and the area value provides a measure of the probability that the two curves could be distinguished. However, whether this value is statistically significant is ambiguous. Therefore, we performed a permutation test (Efron and Tibshirani, 1998) to determine whether the areas we measured were statistically reliable. For each 5 ms bin and each neuron, we randomly sampled the discharge rate 1000 times and generated an ROC curve for each permutation. This resulted in a distribution of ROC areas for every 5 ms time point. The original ROC area for individual neurons was compared with this distribution of areas to determine whether it fell within or outside of the 95th percentile. If the original value fell outside of the 95th percentile, we determined that the difference between the two curves at that time point was statistically reliable. We then computed a mean separation time from all of the significant times across the sample of neurons.

Results

We recorded from 151 SC neurons in four monkeys and classified (see Materials and Methods) each as buildup (106) or visual-tonic (45). Although variable, we tended to find visual-tonic neurons dorsally in the SC (mean ± SD, 1.1 ± 0.7 mm from the surface) and buildup neurons below visual-tonic neurons (mean ± SD, 1.6 ± 0.6 mm from the surface). Subsets of these neurons were recorded in different tasks (control, Go-Go and Go/No-Go). In two monkeys, we recorded neurons only during performance of the Go/No-Go task, and, in two monkeys, we recorded during both tasks. Of the two that learned both tasks, one learned the Go/No-Go task first and then learned the Go-Go task, and the second learned the Go-Go task first and then learned the Go/No-Go task. Table 1 shows the breakdown of how many neurons were recorded in each condition and in each task. It also indicates the number of neurons recorded in both tasks. Note that the monkeys did not always complete all of the trial types in all tasks, so particularly in the interleaved conditions, there are often fewer neurons. Also note that the neurons recorded in the control task (n = 15) are excluded from the table.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

The breakdown of neurons recorded in each condition and in each task

Go-Go task: behavior of buildup neurons

In most models of competition, neurons with similar properties are mutually excitatory, whereas neurons with dissimilar properties are mutually inhibitory (Desimone and Duncan, 1995). In our task, the presentation of two stimuli within a single SC RF led to the activation of two overlapping populations of neurons (McIlwain, 1975, 1986) reflecting close (generally within 5°) but different locations. If stimulus competition exists, we predicted a reduced level of activity in SC neurons in the two stimulus conditions compared with when a single stimulus was presented. We performed this task in blocked and interleaved trial conditions.

To display the results, we constructed mean spike density functions for all of the neurons except those showing statistically significant effects that were opposite our predictions in at least one interval (for description of intervals, see below or Materials and Methods). To assess the variability, the results for the individual neurons across the entire sample are shown in plots, generally in the figures after the spike density functions. We recorded 24 buildup neurons in interleaved conditions of the Go-Go task, and two had opposite effects. Therefore, 22 neurons were used to construct the mean spike density function shown in Figure 3a. Across this sample of buildup neurons, when a single stimulus was presented in the center of the RF, we observed a robust discharge of action potentials (Fig. 3a, left panel, red traces 1T). During the delay period, action potentials were maintained at a lower level (Fig. 3a, middle panel, red traces 1T). When the fixation point changed shape, indicating the target for an upcoming saccade, the activity of buildup neurons increased further (Fig. 3a, middle panel, red traces 1T). Finally, when the fixation point changed back to its original shape, providing a cue to initiate a saccade to the target, we observed a robust discharge of action potentials (Fig. 3a, right panel, red traces 1T). Because the stimulus located at the edge of the RF was also excitatory for the neurons, a qualitatively similar profile of activity was observed (Fig. 3a, blue traces 1S), although at a lower level. When these two stimuli were presented simultaneously within the RF, the activity of buildup neurons was reduced (Fig. 3a, left panel, green traces 2T, yellow traces 2S). When the saccade target was identified, the neuronal activity seen in buildup neurons showed two possible profiles. If the stimulus located in the center of the field was indicated, the activity increased (Fig. 3a, middle panel, green traces 2T). If the edge stimulus was indicated as the target of the saccade, the activity increased slightly or remained essentially unchanged (Fig. 3a, middle panel, yellow traces 2S). Note, however, that the pair activity was still higher when the cue indicated the center stimulus compared with when the cue indicated the edge stimulus. By the time of saccade initiation, the neuronal activity measured in these two conditions was clearly separate (Fig. 3a, right panel, green traces 2T, yellow traces 2S). When the saccade was made to the center of the field, the activity was statistically indistinguishable for the one or two stimulus conditions across the sample of neurons (Fig. 3a, right panel, red traces 1T, green traces 2T). A similar pattern was also seen when the saccade was made to the edge stimulus, although the saccade-related discharge was less robust (Fig. 3a, blue traces 1S, yellow traces 2S).

Compared with the interleaved target trial conditions, the neuronal modulations were more robust when the trial conditions were blocked. The results obtained in the blocked target trial condition for a single buildup neuron are shown in Figure 3b. We recorded from 39 neurons in the blocked condition (24 of these were also recorded in the interleaved condition) (Table 1). Seven neurons showed opposite effects; therefore, the mean spike density function for 32 buildup neurons is shown in Figure 3c. In the single neuron as well as the sample data, the separation between the 2T and 2S conditions was seen even in the initial visual response (Fig. 3b,c, green and yellow traces). This was evident because the monkeys knew in advance which saccade would be required (Basso and Wurtz, 1997). Because of the clarity of the effects, we will use examples from the blocked conditions to illustrate most of the rest of the results.

  Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Buildup neurons have reduced activity when two stimuli are in the RF and a central cue biases neuronal activity. a, Spike density functions (σ = 15 ms) averaged over 22 buildup neurons are shown for each of the four trial types in the interleaved condition. Data are from correctly performed trials. Red traces are from 1T trials, blue traces are from 1S trials, green traces are from 2T trials, and yellow traces are from 2S trials. The labeled bars at the top of the figure indicate the temporal arrangement of the task, and the eye position trace labeled “eye” is a schematic. The insets are schematics of the stimulus configurations, and the arrows indicate trial conditions in which saccades were required. The left panel is aligned on the onset of the visual stimuli indicated by the dashed vertical line and arrowhead. The middle panel is aligned on the onset of the cue indicated by the dashed vertical line and arrowhead, and the right panel is aligned on the onset of the saccade, also indicated by a dashed vertical line and arrowhead. The rectangles at the bottom of each panel indicate the measurement intervals used for statistical comparisons, and filled rectangles indicate statistically significant differences across all conditions (ANOVA, p < 0.05) (blue, visual interval; green, delay 1 interval; red, delay 2 interval; black, saccade interval). b, Spike density function for a single neuron recorded in blocked trials of the Go-Go task. This example is an average of at least 20 trials. c,Spike density functions (σ = 15 ms) averaged over 32 buildup neurons are shown for each of the four trial types in the blocked condition.

Buildup neuron activity across the sample: two stimulus interactions

Two excitatory stimuli located within a single RF of SC buildup neurons resulted in a suppression of neuronal activity compared with when a single, excitatory stimulus was presented in isolation. To quantify the activity across all of the neurons in our sample during the Go-Go task, we divided the task into four intervals and measured the mean neuronal discharge rate in these intervals. The first 200 ms interval began at the time the stimuli appeared (visual interval). The second interval (delay 1) was defined as the 400 ms of activity before the onset of the cue. The third interval began at the time the cue indicated the target and continued for 600 ms (delay 2). The saccade interval was defined as 100 ms before to 100 ms after saccade onset (for saccade detecting algorithm, see Materials and Methods).

In the blocked condition, 29 of 39 (74%) buildup neurons showed significantly lower responses in the 2T condition compared with the 1T condition during the visual interval (Fig. 4a, filled blue circles, points below the unity line), whereas 13 of 24 (54%) showed this in the interleaved condition (Fig. 4a, filled blue triangles) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Seventeen of 39 (44%) buildup neurons in blocked trials showed significantly reduced activity for the 2T condition compared with the 1T condition during delay 1 (Fig. 4b, filled green circles), whereas 4 of 24 (17%) showed this pattern in interleaved trials (Fig. 4b, filled green triangles) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). During delay 2, 17 of 39 (44%) showed significantly reduced activity in blocked trials (Fig. 4c, filled red circles), and 6 of 24 (25%) buildup neurons had significantly reduced activity (Fig. 4b, filled red triangles) in interleaved trials (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Despite the fact that the same saccade was made in the 2T and 1T conditions, 8 of 39 (21%) neurons had significantly lower activity during the saccade interval for the 2T condition in blocked trials (Fig. 4d, filled black circle), and 4 of 24 (17%) showed this in interleaved trials (Fig. 4d, filled black triangle) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Across all neurons in the sample, the differences were significantly different for all intervals except the saccade interval (Wilcoxon's signed-rank test; p < 0.001; p = 0.002; p = 0.002; p = 0.538). Thus, the majority of buildup neurons showed similar behavior in the Go-Go task, and this pattern was more evident when the trial conditions were presented in blocks.

Buildup neuron activity across the sample: a central cue affects stimulus interactions in the Go-Go task

When the cue located at the center of the screen appeared, monkeys were required to select one of the two stimuli as the saccade target. Consistent with a biased competition, when the target in the center location was selected (2T), we expected the level of neuronal activity to closely match the triangle-alone condition (1T). Similarly, when the square was indicated as the saccade target (2S), we expected the neuronal activity to closely match that measured during the square-alone condition (1S). This pattern was seen in many buildup neurons (Fig. 3, middle and right panels). To determine whether this was consistent across our sample of neurons, we plotted the neuronal activity from the 2T trials against the neuronal activity in the 2S trials for the visual, delay 1 and delay 2 measurement intervals (Fig. 4e-g). In the blocked target trials, we found that 14 of 39 (36%) buildup neurons had significantly (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05) higher visual responses for the 2T condition compared with the 2S condition (Fig. 4e, filled blue circles, points above the unity line) in the visual interval. Fourteen of 39 (36%) showed significantly higher activity measured during delay 1 (Fig. 4f, filled green circles), and 24 of 39 (62%) buildup neurons had significantly greater activity for 2T compared with 2S during delay 2 (Fig. 4g, filled red circles) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Because the monkeys could not know which stimulus would be identified as a saccade target in the interleaved conditions, we found few neurons with these differences before the saccade cue was provided. One of 24 and 2 of 24 showed significant differences during the visual and delay 1 intervals (Fig. 4e, filled blue triangles, f, filled green triangles), whereas 7 of 24 (29%) showed significant differences during the delay 2 interval (Fig. 4g, filled red triangles) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Consistent with a biased competition, when a cue indicated a saccade to a strong stimulus, suppressive stimulus interactions decreased. When a cue indicated a saccade to a weak stimulus (at the edge of the RF), the reduction in stimulus interactions was less obvious. Across all neurons in the sample, the differences were significant for the three intervals (Wilcoxon's signed-rank test; p = 0.046; p = 0.012; p < 0.001).

  Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Buildup neurons have reduced activity when two stimuli are in the RF. The neuronal activity from individual buildup neurons (39 in blocked condition and 24 in the interleaved condition, 24 overlapping) in the 2T condition is plotted against the neuronal activity in the 1T condition. Points above the unity line indicate that the discharge rate in the 1T condition was greater than the discharge rate in the 2T condition. In a-d, the measurement interval plotted is indicated by the color, and the shape indicates trial condition. Circles are from blocked target trials, and triangles are from interleaved target trials. Blue circles and triangles are visual (0 -200 ms after stimulus onset), green circles and triangles are delay 1 (400 ms before the cue onset), red circles and triangles are delay 2 (0-600 ms after cue onset), and black circles and triangles indicate the saccade interval (100 ms before to 100 ms after saccade onset). The filled symbols indicate that the differences between the conditions were statistically significant (p<0.05). Open symbols indicate differences were not statistically significant. e-g, Neuronal activity measured in the 2T condition was plotted against the neuronal activity measured in the 2S for the three intervals and the same neurons as in a. The color and symbol schemes are the same as in a. Each symbol is taken from a single neuron and is an average of 20-50 trials. Wilcoxon's signed-rank test was performed across the sample of neurons and was significant for each interval except the saccade interval indicated by the p values.

  Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Visual-tonic neurons show reduced activity with two stimuli in the RF, and a central cue biases neuronal activity. The neuronal activity from 12 visual-tonic neurons is plotted as averaged, spike density functions (σ = 15 ms) for each of the four trial types. The arrangement and color scheme is identical to that shown in Figure 3. The data are taken from correctly performed trials when the conditions were blocked. Alignment times are indicated by the vertical dashed lines and the arrowheads at the bottom of each panel. Filled rectangles along the bottom of the panels indicate significant differences across the four conditions (ANOVA, p < 0.05).

Go-Go task: behavior of visual-tonic neurons

We recorded from 17 visual-tonic neurons in the blocked trials, and nine of these were also recorded in the interleaved trials. We found that visual-tonic neurons behaved similarly to buildup neurons. Figure 5 shows the average neuronal activity profile recorded from 12 visual-tonic neurons (recall that we plotted spike density functions for all neurons excluding those showing opposite effects in at least one interval of the task). The initial visual response of these neurons was larger with a single stimulus in the RF compared with the condition in which a single stimulus was located at the edge of the RF (Fig. 5, red and blue traces). The activity was also greater for a single stimulus compared with when two stimuli were present in the RF (Fig. 5, red, green, and yellow traces).

  Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Visual-tonic neurons show reduced activity with two stimuli in RF, and a central cue biases neuronal activity. In a-c, the neuronal activity from individual visual-tonic neurons (17 blocked and 9 interleaved) in the 1T condition is plotted against the activity measured in the 2T condition. Points above the unity line indicate that the discharge rate in the 1T condition was greater than the 2T condition. Intervals are indicated by color, and blocked or interleaved trial types are indicated by shape. Filled symbols indicate that differences were statistically significant. The arrangement is the same as in Figure 4, except the saccade interval is not shown. In e-g, the neuronal activity in the 2T condition was plotted against the neuronal activity measured in the 2S condition. Each symbol is taken from a single neuron and is the mean of at least 20-50 trials. Wilcoxon's signed-rank test was performed across all neurons and was significant for each interval except the delay 2 interval in the 1T versus 2T comparison as indicated by the p values.

Visual-tonic activity across the sample: two stimulus interactions

In blocked trials, 6 of 17, 6 of 17, and 7 of 17 of visual-tonic neurons had significantly (t test, p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) lower activity in 2T compared with 1T target trials in all three measurement intervals (Fig. 6a-c, filled blue, green, and red circles). In interleaved trials, three of nine (33%) neurons had a significantly lower level of activity in the 2T condition compared with the 1T condition in the visual interval (Fig. 6a, filled blue triangles) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Five of nine (55%) had less activity in the 2T condition compared with the 1T condition during delay 1 (Fig. 7b, filled green triangles), and one of nine (11%) had less activity during the delay 2 interval (Fig. 6c, filled red triangles) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Across all neurons in the sample, the differences were significant for the visual and delay 1 intervals but not the delay 2 interval (Wilcoxon's signed-rank test; p < 0.001; p = 0.005; p = 0.096).

Visual-tonic activity across the sample: a central cue affects stimulus interactions in the Go-Go task

As we had done for buildup neurons, we plotted visual-tonic neuronal activity in the 2T condition against that measured in the 2S condition for each measurement interval (Fig. 6d-f). We found that, in blocked trials, 7 of 17 (41%) visual-tonic neurons showed significantly higher visual activity when monkeys knew they should select a saccade target located in the center of the RF compared with when they knew they should select a saccade target located at the edge of the RF (Fig. 6d, filled blue circles, blocked; filled blue triangles, interleaved) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). This was also evident for delay 1 intervals (Fig. 6e, filled green circles, blocked; filled green triangles, interleaved) and delay 2 intervals (Fig. 6f, filled red circles, blocked; filled red triangles, interleaved) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Across all neurons in the sample, the differences were significant for all three intervals (Wilcoxon's signed-rank test; p = 0.046; p = 0.010; p < 0.001).

Go/No-Go task: behavior of buildup neurons

In extrastriate cortex, sensory interactions and the ability of attention to bias these interactions were tested by comparing attended conditions with unattended conditions (Reynolds et al., 1999). We sought to dissociate stimulus interactions from processes related to events preceding eye movement initiation. Therefore, we recorded from buildup and visual-tonic neurons while monkeys performed the Go/No-Go task. In our Go/No-Go task, when the fixation point changed shape to a triangle, this indicated that a saccade to the triangle would be required. When the fixation point changed shape to a square, this indicated that the monkey should hold its gaze at the fixation point. In the latter condition, we could examine stimulus interactions while the monkeys remained fixating and presumably did not engage in processes leading up to a saccade. We assumed this was analogous to the unattended condition in previous experiments. We measured neuronal activity in two variations of this scheme. In one, the triangle was located in the center of the RF and was always associated with the production of a saccade. This stimulus was “strong” because it drove the neuron maximally. The square located at the edge of the RF drove the neuron less well and therefore was similar to the introduction of a weak stimulus. Therefore, using this visual stimulus configuration, we could assess the influence of cueing a gaze shift to a strong stimulus on the stimulus interactions. In the second variation, the triangle was located at the edge of the RF. This stimulus was still excitatory for the neuron, but it drove the neuron less well. The square was now located at the center of the RF and drove the neuron maximally. In this arrangement, we could assess the influence of a cue to shift gaze to a weak (edge) stimulus on the stimulus interactions.

First, we describe the results from buildup neurons in the experiments in which the influence of a strong stimulus was assessed (Figs. 7, 8), and we describe results from buildup neurons in the experiments in which the influence of a weak stimulus was assessed (Fig. 9). Second, we describe the results from visual-tonic neurons in the same two stimulus configurations (Figs. 10, 11).

We recorded from 60 buildup neurons in the Go/No-Go task. Forty-four neurons were recorded with the trial conditions randomly interleaved, and 47 neurons were recorded with the trial conditions blocked. Thirty-one of these neurons were recorded in both the interleaved and blocked conditions (Table 1).

  Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Cueing a strong stimulus influences buildup neuron activity. The temporal arrangement of the task is shown by the horizontal bars along the top, and the line labeled “eye” is a schematic of eye position. The panel on the left is aligned on the onset of the stimuli indicated by the vertical dashed line and arrowhead. The middle panel is aligned on the onset of the cue, and the right panel is aligned on the onset of the saccade also indicated by the vertical dashed line and the arrowhead. The arrangement and color scheme are the same as in Figure 3, b and c. a, Single neuron example recorded in the Go/No-Go task. b, Mean spike density function from 42 buildup neurons. Filled rectangles along the bottom of the panels indicate significant differences across the four conditions (ANOVA, p < 0.05).

Influence of cueing a strong stimulus in buildup neurons

In Figure 7, we show a single neuron example (Fig. 7a) and an average of 42 buildup neurons (Fig. 7b) recorded in the blocked version of the Go/No-Go task. We found three differences in the neuronal activity in this task compared with the Go-Go task (Figs. 3c, 7b). First, immediately before the saccade interval, the level of activity in the 2T condition matched the level of activity in the 1T condition more closely in the Go-Go task than in the Go/No-Go task (Figs. 3c, 7b, right panels, green and red traces). Second, immediately before the saccade interval, the neuronal activity in the 2S condition matched the neuronal activity in the 1S condition more closely in the Go-Go task than in the Go/No-Go task (Figs. 3c, 7b, right panels, yellow and blue traces). Third, and expectedly, there was no saccade-related burst in the 2S condition in the Go/No-Go task, although there was a saccade-related burst in the 2S condition of the Go-Go task because a saccade was made only in the latter task (Figs. 3c, 7b, right panels, yellow traces).

  Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Cueing a strong stimulus influences most buildup neurons. Neuronal activity in the 1T condition is plotted against the neuronal activity in the 2T condition for buildup neurons (47 blocked and 44 interleaved). a-d show the different intervals, visual, delay 1, delay 2, and saccade. In e-g, the neuronal activity measured in the 2T condition was plotted against the neuronal activity measured in the 2S condition for the visual, delay 1, and delay 2 intervals. Each symbol is taken from a single neuron and is the mean of at least 20 -50 trials. The arrangement and color scheme of this figure is the same as that shown in Figure 4. Circles are from blocked trials, and triangles are from interleaved trials. The filled symbols indicate that the differences between the conditions were statistically significant (p < 0.05). Open symbols indicate differences were not statistically significant. Blue circles and triangles are data from the visual interval (0 -200 ms after stimulus onset). Green circles and triangles are data from the delay 1 interval (400 ms before cue onset). Red circles and triangles are data from the delay 2 interval (0 - 600 ms after cue onset). Black circles and triangles are data from the saccade interval (100 ms before to 100 ms after saccade onset). Wilcoxon's signed-rank test was performed across the sample of neurons and was significant for all intervals except the saccade interval.

As we observed for the Go-Go task, in the Go/No-Go task, when two stimuli were presented in the same RF of buildup neurons, the activity was lower than when a single stimulus was presented during the visual, delay 1, and delay 2 intervals (Fig. 8a-c, blue, green, and red circles, blocked; blue, green, and red triangles, interleaved) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). At the time the cue was provided, indicating that a saccade should be prepared in the case of the triangle cue, or that the monkey should remain fixating in the case of the square cue, the neuronal activity changed. For the sample of 47 neurons recorded in the blocked condition, 23 of 47 (49%) had a significantly (t test, p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) higher level of activity in the 2T condition compared with the 2S condition during the visual interval (Fig. 8e, blue circles, blocked; blue triangles, interleaved). Twentyeight of 47 (60%) showed this enhancement in the delay 1 interval (Fig. 8f, green circles, blocked; green triangles, interleaved) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Thirty-six of 47 (77%) showed the enhancement during the delay 2 interval (Fig. 8g, red circles, blocked; red triangles, interleaved) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). Thus, a cue indicating a shift of gaze to a strong stimulus reduced the influence of a weak stimulus on SC buildup neurons. Over the entire sample, we found significant differences in all of the intervals except the saccade intervals (Wilcoxon's test; p < 0.001, Fig. 8a; p < 0.001, Fig. 8b; p < 0.001, Fig. 8c; p = 0.083, Fig. 8d; p < 0.001, Fig. 8e; p < 0.001, Fig. 8f; p < 0.001, Fig. 8g).

  Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

Cueing a weak stimulus influences buildup neuron activity. a, The temporal arrangement of the task is shown by the horizontal, open bars at the top. The line labeled “eye” is a schematic of eye position. The alignment of each panel is indicated by the vertical dashed lines and the arrowheads. The left panel is aligned on stimuli onset. The middle panel is aligned on the cue onset, and the right panel is aligned on saccade onset. Each trace is an average of 33 buildup neurons. The spike density functions (σ = 15 ms) are superimposed for the different stimulus conditions. Red traces are from 1T trials, blue traces are from 1S trials, green traces are from 2T trials, and yellow traces are from 2S trials. Note that, in this version of the task, the triangle stimulus was placed at the edge of the RF, whereas the square (No-Go) stimulus was placed at the center of the RF. The filled rectangles along the bottom of each panel indicate the measurement intervals for which the conditions were significantly different (ANOVA p < 0.05). b-d, The neuronal activity measured in the 2T condition was plotted against the neuronal activity measured in the 2S condition for buildup neurons (33 blocked and 29 interleaved, 29 in both blocked and interleaved). b, Visual; c, delay 1; d, delay 2. Each symbol is taken from a single neuron and is the mean of at least 20-50 trials. Points above the unity line indicate that the neuronal activity in the 2T condition was greater than the 2S condition. The arrangement is the same as in Figure 8e-g.

  Figure 10.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 10.

Cueing a strong stimulus influences visual-tonic neuron activity. a, The horizontal bars at the top of the figure show the temporal arrangement of the task. The line labeled “eye” is a schematic of eye position. The vertical dashed lines and the arrowheads in each panel indicate the alignment time. The left panel is aligned on stimuli onset, the middle panel is aligned on cue onset, and the right panel is aligned on saccade onset. The color scheme is the same as in Figure 3. Red traces are from 1T trials, blue traces are from 1S trials, green traces are from 2T trials, and yellow traces are from 2S trials. The rectangular bars along the bottom of each panel indicate the measurement intervals and, if filled, indicate that there were significant differences (p<0.05) between all four conditions. b-d, The neuronal activity measured in the 2T condition was plotted against the neuronal activity measured in the 2S condition. Each symbol is taken from a single neuron and is the mean of at least 20-50 trials. The arrangement and color scheme of this figure is the same as that shown in Figure 9. Circles are from blocked target trials, and triangles are from interleaved target trials. The filled symbols indicate that the differences between the conditions were statistically significant (p < 0.05). Open symbols indicate that differences were not statistically significant. Wilcoxon's signed-rank test was performed across the sample, and the differences were significant for all intervals.

  Figure 11.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 11.

Cueing a weak stimulus influences visual-tonic neuron activity. a, The temporal arrangement of the task is shown by the horizontal, open bars at the top of the figure. The line labeled “eye” is a schematic of eye position. The alignment of each panel is indicated by the vertical dashed lines and the arrowheads. The left panel is aligned on stimuli onset. The middle panel is aligned on the cue onset, and the right panel is aligned on saccade onset. Each trace is an average of 12 visual-tonic neurons. The spike density functions (σ = 15 ms) are superimposed for the different stimulus conditions. Red traces are from 1T trials, blue traces are from 1S trials, green traces are from 2T trials, and yellow traces are from 2S trials. Note that, in this version of the task, the triangle stimulus was placed at the edge of the RF, and the square (No-Go) stimulus was placed at the center of the RF. b-d, The neuronal activity measured in the 2T condition was plotted against the neuronal activity measured in the 2S condition for the visual-tonic neurons (12 blocked and 7 interleaved). Each symbol is taken from a single neuron and is the mean of at least 20 -50 trials. Circles are from blocked target trials, and triangles are from interleaved target trials. The filled symbols indicate that the differences between the conditions were statistically significant (p < 0.05). Open symbols indicate that differences were not statistically significant. Blue circles and triangles are data from visual interval (0 -200 ms after stimulus onset), green circles and triangles are data from the delay 1 interval (400 ms before cue onset), and red circles and triangles are data from the delay 2 interval (0 - 600 ms after cue onset). Wilcoxon's signed-rank test across the sample indicated that only the delay 2 interval differences were significant.

Influence of cueing a weak stimulus in buildup neurons

In these experiments, the target associated with a saccade (triangle) was presented at the edge of the RF. The No-Go target (square) was presented in the center of the RF, making it a strong stimulus for the neuron. Accordingly, the biased competition model applied to the SC predicts that a cue to shift gaze to the edge or “weak” stimulus should reduce the influence of the stimulus located in the center of the RF. Therefore, the activity levels in the 2T and 2S conditions should be much closer than what was observed in Figure 7.

Figure 9 shows the average response profile of the 33 buildup neurons recorded in this version of the task. The same neurons were recorded in the previous configuration. During all three intervals, the neuronal activity in the 2T and 2S trial types were much closer than that seen in the 2T and 2S condition with the opposite stimulus configuration (Fig. 9, left side of middle panel, blue, green and yellow traces). Note that, for the same data in this condition, we are interested in the values that were not significantly different or were lower in the 2T compared with the 2S condition. In the blocked condition, 31 of 33 (94%) buildup neurons had the same or lower activity in the 2T condition compared with the 2S condition during the visual interval (Fig. 9b, open and filled blue circles, blocked; open and filled blue triangles, interleaved). Twenty-three of 33 (67%) showed the same or lower activity during the delay 1 interval (Fig. 9c, open and filled green circles, blocked; open and filled green triangles, interleaved). In the delay 2 interval, 20 of 33 (61%) buildup neurons had the same or lower activity for the 2T trials compared with the 2S trials (Fig. 9d, open and filled red circles, blocked; open and filled red triangles, interleaved). Note also that the 1S condition was high at the time of the cue because there continued to be a stimulus in the center of the RF. Thus, cueing a weak stimulus, although it was still within the excitatory region of the SC neuron, did not produce the same enhancement as seen when a strong stimulus was cued. We will explore this phenomenon more thoroughly in the section below (see Direct comparison with the biased competition model).

Go/No-Go task: behavior of visual-tonic neurons

We recorded from 20 visual-tonic neurons in the Go/No-Go task (Fig. 10). Thirteen were recorded in interleaved trials, and 18 were recorded in blocked trials. Eleven were recorded in both types (Table 1). The results were qualitatively identical to those seen in buildup neurons.

Influence of cueing a strong stimulus in visual-tonic neurons

Again, in this condition, we predicted that, if a cue to shift gaze resulted in neuronal activity that biased the competition between locations, then 2T activity should be higher than 2S activity. Across our sample of visual-tonic neurons, respectively, 13 of 18 (72%) and 14 of 18 (78%) had significantly higher activity for the 2T condition during the visual and delay 1 intervals in the blocked condition (Fig. 10b,c, filled blue and green circles, blocked; filled blue and green triangles, interleaved) (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05). When monkeys were provided with a cue indicating that they should prepare a saccade (delay 2), 14 of 18 (78%) visual-tonic neurons in the blocked condition (Fig. 10d, filled red circles, blocked; filled red triangles, interleaved) had a significantly (t test, Bonferroni corrected, p < 0.05) higher activity for 2T compared with 2S. Across the sample, significant differences were found in all three intervals (Wilcoxon's signedrank test; p = 0.003; p < 0.001; p = 0.002).

Influence of cueing a weak stimulus in visual-tonic neurons

Recall that, in this condition, we predicted that, if a cue to shift gaze to the edge (weak) stimulus resulted in neuronal activity that biased the competition between locations, then 2T activity should be the same or lower than 2S activity. We recorded from 12 visual-tonic neurons in this version of the task (Fig. 11). These 12 neurons were also recorded in the opposite configuration. Many visual-tonic neurons showed no significant difference for 2T compared with the 1T condition (Fig. 11b-d, open blue, green, and red circles, blocked; open blue, green, and red triangles, interleaved). Across the sample, only delay 1 showed significant differences (Wilcoxon's signed-rank test; p = 0.147; p = 0.036; p = 0.260).

  Figure 12.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 12.

Cue to shift gaze influences sensory interactions in SC neurons. The sensory interaction index is plotted against the selectivity index for buildup (n = 25 recorded in both the center and edge conditions in interleaved trials) and visual-tonic (n = 7 recorded in center and edge conditions in interleaved trials) neurons in the Go/No-Go task. The same neurons are displayed in all four panels. For the sensory interaction measure, the mean discharge rate of neurons during the presentation of two stimuli in a single RF was measured (pair), and the mean discharge rate of the same neurons when only a single stimulus was presented at the center (reference) of the RF was subtracted from the pair measure. We then divided this difference value by the sum of the same two quantities. For the selectivity index, we measured the mean discharge rate of the same neurons when a single stimulus was located at the edge (probe) of the RF and at the center of the RF (reference). The difference of these two quantities was divided by the sum of these two quantities. a, Sensory interaction plotted against the selectivity index for delay 1 using the trials in which the center (reference) stimulus was the square (no saccade) and the edge (probe) stimulus was the triangle (saccade) during delay 1, before the cue to make or withhold a saccade was provided. b, The same data were plotted for the delay 2 interval after the cue to make a saccade to the edge stimulus was provided. c, The sensory interaction index is plotted against the selectivity index for the Go/No-Go trials in which the center (reference) stimulus was the triangle and the edge (probe) stimulus was the square. Delay 1, before the cue, measurement interval was used. d, The same as in c, except for the delay 2 interval after the cue to make a saccade. Each circle is an average of at least 20 -50 trials.

Direct comparison with the biased competition model

To provide an explicit test of whether the biased competition model explained the results we obtained in SC, we adopted an analysis that was used to assess V2 and V4 neurons (Reynolds et al., 1999). A selectivity index was compared with a sensory interaction index. The values of both indices ranged from -1 to 1. In extrastriate cortex, stimuli were arbitrarily defined as reference or probe stimuli. If a neuron had a negative selectivity index, the neuron had a smaller response to a probe stimulus compared with a reference stimulus. If a neuron had a positive selectivity index, the neuron had a larger response to the probe than to the reference. The sensory interaction index determined how the neurons behaved in the presence of the two stimuli together. A negative interaction index indicated that the neuronal response to the pair was dominated by the neuronal response to reference stimulus, whereas a positive interaction index meant that the pair response was dominated by the probe stimulus. The slope of the line relating the sensory interaction and selectivity indices indicated which stimulus dominated the pair response. In the feature attention experiments (Reynolds et al., 1999), comparing the slope of these two variables with and without attention provided a way to assess the influence of attention. When attention was directed to the probe stimulus, the response of the probe dominated the pair response and the slope increased. When attention was directed to the reference stimulus, the probe had a reduced influence on the pair response and the slope was reduced. Thus, attention acted on the underlying sensory interactions (Reynolds et al., 1999, their Figs. 4, 10, 11).

To determine whether a cue to shift gaze also acted on sensory stimulus interactions within the SC, we computed a sensory interaction index and plotted this against a selectivity index (Fig. 12). We used 32 neurons (25 buildup and 7 visual-tonic) recorded in both configurations of the task in the interleaved trial types. We measured the neuronal activity for the neurons when both stimuli were present (pair) and when only a single stimulus was present in the center of the RF (reference). We then measured the sum of these two activities. The ratio of the pair - reference activity to the pair + reference activities was our measure of the sensory interaction. Negative values indicate that the response of the neuron is suppressed in the pair condition. Positive values indicate that the response of the neuron is enhanced in the pair condition. Importantly, the interaction index was computed during the interval before the presentation of the centrally located cue indicating whether or not a saccade would be required (delay 1) and after the cue (delay 2). This measure was computed for the two visual stimulus configurations of the Go/No-Go task. The configuration in which the triangle (Go cue) was located at the edge of the RF is shown in Figure 12, a and b. The configuration in which the triangle (Go cue) was located in the center of the RF is shown in Figure 12, c and d. In both cases, the center stimulus was defined as the reference stimulus, and the edge stimulus was defined as the probe. During delay 1 when the square was located in the center of the RF and the triangle was located at the edge of the RF, the slope of the sensory interaction versus the selectivity index was 0.13 (Fig. 12a). This indicated that the pair response was little influenced by the probe (the triangle on the edge of the RF). However, during delay 2, when the triangle was cued for a saccade to the edge of the RF, the slope increased to 0.60. The influence of this cue was to pull the response of the pair toward a sensory interaction value that favored the probe stimulus response (the points fell more negative, resulting in an increased slope). Using multiple regression, we determined that the difference between these slopes was significant (p < 0.05).

In the Go/No-Go task in which the triangle was located in the center of the RF (reference) and the square was at the edge (probe), we computed the same indices. In this case, during delay 1, the slope of the line was positive (0.54) (Fig. 12c). This was because the reference term in the sensory interaction index was larger in these trials even before the cue, because it was likely that the triangle would be cued for a saccade. After the cue to make a saccade to the reference stimulus, during delay 2, the slope decreased to -0.05 (Fig. 12d). This indicated that the cue to the reference stimulus changed the sensory interaction. The neuronal response became dominated by the reference stimulus (the points became less negative, resulting in a decrease in the slope). The difference in slopes in delay 1 and delay 2 were statistically significant (p < 0.05). Thus, we conclude that a cue to shift gaze, like attention in extrastriate cortex, influences stimulus interactions in SC neurons.

  Figure 13.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 13.

Dynamics of multistimulus integration in buildup neurons. The predicted discharge rate of buildup neurons in the 2T condition if they summed the discharge rates from the 1T and 1S conditions is plotted against the actual discharge rates measured in the 2T condition. The data are taken from blocked target trials in which the monkeys performed the task correctly. a, b, c, and d show the predictions for each of the measurement intervals, visual, delay 1, delay 2, and saccade, respectively. Red + are data from the Go-Go task, and blue × are taken from the Go/No-Go task trials. e-h are the same data and intervals plotted using an averaging model. For this, the mean discharge rate was computed by summing the discharge rates in the 1T and 1S conditions and dividing by 2. The solid lines in all panels are the lines of unity. Points falling along this line indicate that the actual discharge rate was predicted accurately from the modeled discharge rate.

Two stimulus interactions: summation or averaging

By presenting two stimuli within single RFs of SC neurons, we could test whether a summation or an averaging model best explained the influence of multiple stimuli on SC neurons. In doing so, we could compare the behavior of SC neurons with the known behavior of neurons within the visual cortex (Ferrera and Lisberger, 1995, 1997; Treue and Maunsell, 1996; Recanzone et al., 1997; Britten and Heuer, 1999; Reynolds et al., 1999). For the summation model, we measured the discharge rate of neurons in the single stimulus cases separately (1T and 1S) and summed these two activities. We then measured the actual discharge rates in the 2T condition and constructed plots of the predicted discharge rate against the actual discharge rate. This computation was performed across different intervals of the task so that we also could explore the dynamics (Fig. 13a-d). We performed the same computations for visual-tonic neurons (Fig. 14a-c). For the averaging model, we computed the predicted discharge rates by summing the neuronal activity for the 1S and 1T conditions and dividing by 2. We then measured the actual discharge rates observed in the 2T condition and plotted the predicted rate against the actual rate. Again, this was performed across multiple intervals (Fig. 13e-h) and was done also for visual-tonic neurons (Fig. 14d-f).

In the plots of predicted against actual discharge rates, if the model explained the data well, we expected to see the points fall along the line of unity, indicating that the two values were similar. In the visual interval, the summation model did not explain the observations in SC buildup neurons for either the Go-Go task or the Go/No-Go task (Fig. 13a, + and × symbols). Indeed, the summation model was a poor predictor of the neuronal activity for all of the intervals in both task conditions (Fig. 13a-d, red + and blue × symbols). The averaging model, in contrast, was much better (Fig. 13e-h). For the visual, delay 1, and delay 2 intervals, the neuronal activity in the two stimulus condition, in both the Go-Go and the Go/No-Go task, was very well explained by the average of the neuronal discharge seen in the single stimulus conditions (Fig. 13e-g, red + and blue × symbols). During the saccade interval, however, the pattern changed. Few of the points fell along the averaging line; rather, most fell below the line, indicating that the neuronal activity during this time was greater in the 2T condition than would be predicted by averaging the two single stimulus condition responses (Fig. 13h, red + and blue × symbols). Indeed, the activity measured in the 2T condition was most similar to that seen in the 1T condition during the saccade interval for both the Go-Go task (Fig. 4d) and the Go/No-Go task (Fig. 8d). This is consistent with a winner-take-all readout at the time the saccade is initiated. Interestingly, this was more apparent for the neuronal activity measured in the Go/No-Go task than the Go-Go task (Fig. 13h, red + and blue × symbols).

In visual-tonic neurons, the pattern was similar. For neither the Go-Go task nor the Go/No-Go task was the summation model satisfactory to explain the data (Fig. 13a-c, red + and blue × symbols). In contrast, the averaging model was better in all three intervals, visual, delay 1, and delay 2 (Fig. 14c-e, red + and blue × symbols). Interestingly, even the averaging model did not explain completely the results for these neurons in the Go-Go task, particularly during the visual interval (Fig. 14a, red +).

Distribution of saccade parameters

Because the stimuli in our task were very close to one another, often within 5°, it was possible that the modulations we observed between the 1T and 2T conditions indicated changes in saccade parameters, such as accuracy, precision, or velocity. To test this, we measured the endpoint and velocity of saccades in the 1T and the 2T conditions of the Go/No-Go task. We computed the mean and SD of the endpoints and velocities from 16 experiments in which the neuronal activity in the 1T condition was significantly greater than in the 2T condition during the saccade interval (Fig. 8d, filled black circle). We compared the vertical and horizontal eye position for the 1T and the 2T conditions and found that the endpoint accuracy was statistically indistinguishable in these two conditions (t test, p = NS; data not shown). As a measure of saccade precision, we computed the SD of the endpoints for the 1T and 2T conditions. We found here also that the SDs were statistically indistinguishable in the two conditions (t test, p = NS.; data not shown). Finally, we found no significant differences in saccade velocity between the two conditions (t test, p = NS; data not shown). We drew two conclusions from these results. First, our monkeys were making accurate saccades to the individual targets in the different conditions. Second, variations in saccadic velocity, endpoint accuracy, or precision cannot explain the modulations we observed in the SC neuronal activity.

  Figure 14.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 14.

Dynamics of multistimulus integration in visual-tonic neurons. The predicted discharge rate of visual-tonic neurons in the 2T condition if they summed the discharge rates from the 1T and 1S conditions is plotted against the actual discharge rates measured in the 2T condition. The data are taken from blocked target trials in which the monkeys performed the task correctly. a-c show the predictions for each of the measurement intervals, visual, delay 1, and delay 2. Red + are data from the Go-Go task, and blue × are taken from the Go/No-Go task trials. e-g are the same data and intervals plotted using an averaging model. For this, the mean discharge rate was computed by summing the discharge rates in the 1T and 1S conditions and dividing by 2. The solid lines in all panels are the lines of unity. Points falling along this line indicate that the actual discharge rate was predicted accurately from the modeled discharge rate.

Timing of SC neuronal activity modulations

An important, unresolved question is whether the modulations seen in the SC arise from mechanisms within the SC itself or whether they are reflections of processing occurring elsewhere and are simply passed on to the SC. We reasoned that we could explore this by comparing the time course of neuronal activity changes in the different conditions of our task. Moreover, we could compare the timing of neuronal activity changes in visual-tonic neurons to the changes in buildup neurons to determine whether the times differed for these two neuronal classes.

To address this, first we compared the time that neuronal activity separated in the initial visual interval of the 1T and the 2T conditions. Second, we measured the time when the 2T and 2S activities separated after the cue in the Go/No-Go task. Finally, we compared the statistically significant times (as determined by a permutation test) measured in visual-tonic neurons to those measured in buildup neurons.

We applied ROC analysis to measure time courses. Considering that top-down modulations may influence stimulus interactions in the blocked condition, we used only the data from the interleaved condition that were significantly different in the 1T and the 2T conditions during the visual interval (Fig. 8a, filled blue triangle). We plotted the normalized spike density functions for buildup (n = 16) and visual-tonic (n = 5) neurons recorded in the 1T and 2T trial types (Fig. 15a). Using the permutation test, we found that the neuronal activity measured in the two conditions separated at a mean time of 132 ms (Fig. 15a, top, dashed and solid lines) (p < 0.05). Visual-tonic neurons had separable curves at a mean time of 146 ms (Fig. 15a, bottom, dashed and solid lines) (p < 0.05). We next compared the time course of separation between the 2T and 2S trials for the neurons that had a significant difference in the 2T and 2S conditions (buildup, 25; visual-tonic, 8). We found that buildup neurons had a mean separation time of 127 ms (Fig. 15b, top, dashed and solid lines) (p < 0.05). Visual-tonic neurons reached significance at 152 ms (Fig. 15b, bottom, dashed and solid lines) (p < 0.05).

Discussion

We recorded from buildup and visual-tonic neurons within the SC of monkeys during the performance of Go-Go and Go/No-Go saccade tasks. Our experiments differ from previous experiments (Basso and Wurtz, 1998; Paré and Wurtz, 2001) in two important ways. First, the stimuli were presented within single RFs of SC neurons. Second, the cue to make or withhold a saccade to a target was presented at the fixation point rather than in the periphery. This required a shape to location transformation.

We draw two conclusions from our results. First, the influence of saccade selection on SC neuronal activity is similar to the influence of attention on sensory interactions seen in extrastriate cortex neurons. Second, during the sensorimotor transformation of a visual stimulus to a saccade, the activity of SC neurons is dynamic. The readout for saccade generation is initially a vector average and only later becomes winner-take-all. Each of these conclusions will be addressed below.

Stimulus interactions and saccade selection

Placing two stimuli within a single RF of a V4, V2 (Moran and Desimone, 1985; Motter, 1994a,b; Luck et al., 1997; Reynolds et al., 1999), or middle temporal area MT neuron (Treue and Maunsell, 1996; Recanzone et al., 1997; Seidemann and Newsome, 1999; Recanzone and Wurtz, 2000) reduces the activity of a neuron compared with the presentation of a single stimulus alone. When cued to attend to one of two stimuli, the neuronal activity moves toward the response of the neuron when the attended stimulus is presented in isolation. These results led to the hypothesis that attention acts to change the sensory responsiveness of neurons to stimuli that are not relevant for behavior, the biased competition model of attention (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Reynolds et al., 1999).

We wanted to determine whether similar stimulus interactions would influence SC neurons and whether a cue to shift gaze would bias the activity of SC neurons in a manner similar to that seen in cortex. Rationale for these ideas comes primarily from three sources. First, an influential hypothesis, called the “motor theory of attention” (Rizzolatti, 1983; Sheliga et al., 1994, 1995; Kustov and Robinson, 1996), posits that commands to shift gaze are also used to shift the location of attention. This theory is based on a large body of psychophysical data (Kowler, 1990; Kowler et al., 1995; Deubel and Schneider, 1996; Schneider and Deubel, 2002). Consistent with psychophysical studies, electrical stimulation of the FEF (Moore and Fallah, 2001, 2004) and SC (Cavanaugh and Wurtz, 2004; Muller et al., 2005) reveals that introducing artificial signals in regions of the brain controlling gaze shifts can enhance sensory processing.

  Figure 15.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 15.

Time course of SC neuronal discrimination. a, Normalized spike density function (σ = 15) is plotted against time for the 1T (solid lines) and 2T (dashed lines) conditions for buildup (n = 15 neurons) and visual-tonic neurons (n = 5). The activity for each neuron was normalized to its own maximum discharge rate during the visual interval activity. The normalized rates were then averaged across the sample. b, The same as in a, for the neuronal activity measured after the onset of the cue in the 2T (solid lines) and 2S (dashed lines) conditions. The discharge rate of each neuron was normalized to its own maximum rate during the delay 2 interval of the 2T condition. n = 19 buildup neurons; n = 7 visual-tonic neurons. Dashed vertical lines in both panels indicate the mean statistically significant time point at which the 2 conditions were distinguishable as determined by ROC and the permutation test (see Materials and Methods).

Second, experiments demonstrate the existence of long-range competitive interactions influencing SC neurons (Basso and Wurtz, 1998; McPeek and Keller, 2002). Electrophysiological studies in cats (Meredith and Ramoa, 1998) and monkeys (Munoz and Istvan, 1998) reveal long-range competitive interactions in SC (but see Özen et al., 2004). Psychophysical experiments suggest that there is competition between saccades and fixation (Walker et al., 1997; Findlay and Walker, 1999), which is thought to be mediated by mutual inhibition between the rostral and caudal SC (Munoz and Wurtz, 1993a,b).

Third, short-range inhibitory mechanisms are likely to exist within the SC (Lee et al., 1997, 2001). Presenting two stimuli in succession, within single RFs of superficial layer SC neurons, results in a suppressed response compared with when one stimulus is presented alone (Rizzolatti et al., 1973, 1974; Wurtz et al., 1980). Anatomical evidence reveals GABA neurons distributed uniformly throughout the SC (Mize, 1992; Behan et al., 2002). The present results demonstrate short-range competitive interactions influencing SC neurons. One hypothesis is that these are mediated by inhibition within the SC. A second hypothesis is that the interactions occur in cortex and are transmitted through the direct connections to the SC (Graham et al., 1979; Fries, 1984; Harting et al., 1992). The time course we measured between the 1T and 2T conditions is consistent with the hypothesis that inhibitory interactions occur simultaneously in SC and cortex [∼100-150 ms for V4 (Reynolds et al., 1999); ∼128-140 ms for FEF (Thompson et al., 1996)]. A similar conclusion was drawn for target selection experiments in SC (McPeek and Keller, 2002).

We measured SC neuronal activity after a cue was provided to either make or withhold a saccade, or, to make a saccade to differently shaped targets. Consistent with a biased competition, when a strong stimulus (defined by the preferred location) was cued, neuronal activity increased. When a weak stimulus was cued, the activity also increased, although not as much as when the strong stimulus was cued (Figs. 7, 9, 10, 11). We found an important difference by comparing the sensory interaction index with the selectivity index measured for SC neurons before and after a cue to make a saccade (Fig. 12). When two stimuli were in the RF of SC neurons, the interaction depended on the task demands. If monkeys chose a saccade target that by itself produced a weak response (located on the edge of the RF: probe), the suppression of SC neuronal activity in response to the pair of stimuli was greater after the cue compared with before the cue (Fig. 12a,b). If monkeys chose a saccade target that alone produced a strong response (located at the center of the RF: reference), SC neurons showed less suppression to the pair of stimuli after the cue compared with before the cue (Fig. 12c,d). This is reminiscent of the phenomenon in extrastriate cortex in which sensory interactions are modified by attention (Reynolds and Desimone, 1999, their Figs. 10, 11). We conclude, therefore, that saccade selection, like attention, modifies the influence of stimulus interactions on SC neurons. These results are consistent with the growing body of evidence implicating the persistent activity of SC neurons in attentional processing (Kustov and Robinson, 1996; Cavanaugh and Wurtz, 2004; Ignashchenkova et al., 2004; Muller et al., 2005).

We also found that buildup neurons discriminated faster than visual-tonic neurons. We cannot determine unequivocally the location of visual-tonic neurons but, because of the general finding that they were located more dorsal to the buildup neurons and because of the similarity between these neurons and those recorded by McPeek and Keller (2002), we suspect that these are the neurons originally described by Sparks and Mays (1980), as quasi visual neurons. Our results suggest that visual information reaches buildup neurons before visual-tonic neurons. Because of the small number of neurons contributing to the results, additional experiments are required to determine this conclusively.

Implications for population coding in SC

Models of the SC in saccade generation have been developed primarily using single-point visual stimulation (Lee et al., 1988; Van Opstal and Van Gisbergen, 1989; McIlwain, 1991; Arai et al., 1993a,b, 1994; Van Opstal and Kappen, 1993; Das et al., 1996; Bozis and Moschovakis, 1998; Quaia et al., 1998, 1999). Based on the well known fact that individual saccades are associated with a large population of active SC neurons (Schiller and Koerner, 1971; Wurtz and Goldberg, 1972; Sparks et al., 1976; Sparks and Mays, 1980; Munoz and Wurtz, 1995) and that there are excitatory connections linking large regions of the SC (McIlwain, 1982), the models fall into the general class of distributed coding models (Lee et al., 1988; McIlwain, 1991; Quaia et al., 1998, 1999). Some original models proposed that the activity across the SC map was summed linearly (Ottes et al., 1986). These models failed to replicate the well known behavior of saccade averaging (Findlay, 1982, 1992; Ottes et al., 1984, 1987). When subjects are asked to look toward one of two stimuli, they are frequently inaccurate, as if making a movement to an illusory target located between the two stimuli. To produce more realistic saccade behavior, nearby excitation and remote, lateral inhibitory mechanisms were included in subsequent models (Van Gisbergen et al., 1987; Van Opstal and Van Gisbergen, 1989).

Two issues arise. First, is the divisive inhibition required to produce averaging (Carandini et al., 1997; Britten and Heuer, 1999; Groh, 2001) performed within or downstream of the SC? Second, is the readout of SC activity dynamic? With time or additional information (Coëffé and O'Regan, 1987; He and Kowler, 1989), saccades can be made accurately, rivaling perceptual accuracy (Kowler and Blaser, 1995; Kowler et al., 1995). The results of our experiments provide insight into both of these questions. We found that the response of SC neurons to the presentation of two stimuli was an average of the response of SC neurons to a single stimulus presented alone, consistent with normalization occurring within the SC at least for the visual and the delay-period activity. In light of observations that increasing the size of a visual stimulus does not change neuronal activity in SC (Cynader and Berman, 1972; Goldberg and Wurtz, 1972a), our result also suggests that SC neurons interpret a single large stimulus differently from two individual stimuli.

We also found that the pattern of normalization changed as the trial progressed. Initially, the pattern was characteristic of averaging, but, by the time of the saccade, the activity most closely matched that seen with a single target. This is consistent with the readout of SC activity changing from a vector average to a winner-take-all.

Based on our results, we suggest that saccade averaging may result from the same mechanisms resulting in illusory visual conjunctions (Treisman and Gelade, 1980; Treisman and Sato, 1990; Treisman, 1996), namely, a failure to engage top-down mechanisms regulating bottom-up, stimulus interactions. Recently, it has been suggested that synchronous firing of neuronal populations contributes to the solution of this problem (Singer and Gray, 1995; Reynolds and Desimone, 1999) (but see Shadlen and Movshon, 1999) and that temporal patterns are important for SC function (Brecht et al., 1999, 2004). If so, a top-down selection mechanism may determine which neuronal populations to synchronize or otherwise serve to disambiguate saccade targets when they activate overlapping neuronal populations.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant EY013692 and The Esther A. and Joseph Klingenstein Fund. We are grateful to Dr. John H. Reynolds and our colleagues in the laboratory for providing critical comments on a previous version of this manuscript. We thank Drs. Michael Shadlen for sharing Matlab code and John W. McClurkin for analysis software support. We also thank Joshua Smith for expert technical assistance and the anonymous referees for their critical review.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Michele A. Basso, Department of Physiology, University of Wisconsin, Madison Medical School, 1300 University Avenue, Room 127 SM1, Madison, WI 53706. E-mail: michele{at}physiology.wisc.edu.

  • Copyright © 2005 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/05/2511357-17$15.00/0

References

  1. ↵
    Arai K, Keller EL, Edelman JA (1993a) A spatio-temporal neural network model of saccade generation. In: Proceedings of the IEEE International Joint Conference on Neural Networks, pp 70-74. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  2. ↵
    Arai K, Keller EL, Edelman JA (1993b) A neural network model of saccade generation using distributed dynamic feedback to superior colliculus. In: International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (Nagoya), pp 53-56. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  3. ↵
    Arai K, Keller EL, Edelman JA (1994) 2-dimensional neural network model of the primate saccadic system. Neural Netw 7: 1115-1135.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    Basso MA, Wurtz RH (1997) Modulation of neuronal activity by target uncertainty. Nature 389: 66-69.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Basso MA, Wurtz RH (1998) Modulation of neuronal activity in superior colliculus by changes in target probability. J Neurosci 18: 7519-7534.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Basso MA, Wurtz RH (2002) Neuronal activity in substantia nigra pars reticulata during target selection. J Neurosci 22: 1883-1894.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Behan M, Steinhacker K, Jeffrey-Borger S, Meredith M (2002) Chemoarchitecture of GABAergic neurons in the ferret superior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 452: 334-359.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Bozis A, Moschovakis AK (1998) Neural network simulations of the primate oculomotor system. III. An one dimensional, one directional model of the superior colliculus. Biol Cybern 79: 215-230.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Bradley A, Skottun B, C, Ohzawa I, Sclar G, Freeman R (1987) Visual orientation and spatial frequency discrimination: a comparison of single neurons and behavior. J Neurophysiol 57: 755-772.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Brecht M, Singer W, Engel AK (1999) Patterns of synchronization in the superior colliculus of anesthetized cats. J Neurosci 19: 3567-3579.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    Brecht M, Singer W, Engel AK (2004) Amplitude and direction of saccadic eye movements depend upon the synchronicity of collicular population activity. J Neurophysiol 92: 424-432.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    Britten KH, Heuer HW (1999) Spatial summation in receptive fields of MT neurons. J Neurosci 19: 5074-5084.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    Britten KH, Shadlen MN, Newsome WT, Movshon JA (1992) The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance. J Neurosci 12: 4745-4765.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  14. ↵
    Carandini M, Heeger DJ, Movshon JA (1997) Linearity and normalization in simple cells of the macaque primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 17: 8621-8644.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    Carello CD, Krauzlis RJ (2004) Manipulating intent: evidence for a causal role of the superior colliculus in target selection. Neuron 43: 575-583.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Cavanaugh J, Wurtz RH (2004) Subcortical modulation of attention counters change blindness. J Neurosci 24: 11236-11243.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    Coëffé C, O'Regan JK (1987) Reducing the influence of non-target stimuli on saccade accuracy: predictability and latency effects. Vision Res 27: 227-240.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Cohn TE, Green DG, Tanner WJ (1975) Receiver operating characteristic analysis: application to the study of quantum fluctuation effects in optic nerve of Rana pipiens. J Gen Physiol 66: 583-616.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Crist CF, Yamasaki DSG, Komatsu H, Wurtz RH (1988) A grid system and a microsyringe for single cell recording. J Neurosci Methods 26: 117-122.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Cynader M, Berman N (1972) Receptive-field organization of monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 35: 187-201.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    Das S, Keller EL, Arai K (1996) A distributed model of the saccadic system: the effects of internal noise. Neurocomputing 11: 245-269.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. ↵
    Desimone R, Duncan J (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18: 183-222.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    Deubel H, Schneider WX (1996) Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Res 36: 1827-1837.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Dorris MC, Paré M, Munoz DP (1997) Neuronal activity in monkey superior colliculus related to the initiation of saccadic eye movements. J Neurosci 17: 8566-8579.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Efron B, Tibshirani RJ (1998) An introduction to the bootstrap, Ed 2. Washington, DC: Chapman and Hall/CRC.
  26. ↵
    Ferrera VP, Lisberger SG (1995) Attention and target selection for smooth pursuit eye movements. J Neurosci 15: 7472-7484.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  27. ↵
    Ferrera VP, Lisberger SG (1997) Neuronal responses in visual areas MT and MST during smooth pursuit target selection. J Neurophysiol 78: 1433-1446.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Findlay JM (1982) Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements. Vision Res 22: 1033-1045.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Findlay JM (1992) Programming of stimulus-elicited saccadic eye movements. In: Eye movements and visual cognition (Rayner K, ed), pp 8-31. Berlin: Springer.
  30. ↵
    Findlay JM, Walker R (1999) A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behav Brain Sci 22: 661-721.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Fries W (1984) Cortical projections to the superior colliculus in the macaque monkey: a retrograde study using horseradish peroxidase. J Comp Neurol 230: 55-76.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Fuchs AF, Robinson DA (1966) A method for measuring horizontal and vertical eye movement chronically in the monkey. J Appl Physiol 21: 1068-1070.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    Glimcher PW, Sparks DL (1992) Movement selection in advance of action in the superior colliculus. Nature 355: 542-545.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH (1971) Changing the behavioral significance of a visual stimulus affects the response of neurons in monkey superior colliculus. Proc Intl Union Physiol Sci 9: 209.
  35. ↵
    Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH (1972a) Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. I. Visual receptive fields of single neurons. J Neurophysiol 35: 542-559.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH (1972b) Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkeys. II. Effect of attention on neuronal responses. J Neurophysiol 35: 560-574.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Graham J, Lin C-S, Kaas JH (1979) Subcortical projections of six visual cortical areas in the owl monkey, Aotus trivirgatus. J Comp Neurol 187: 557-580.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Green DM, Swets JA (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
  39. ↵
    Groh JJ (2001) Converting neural signals from place codes to rate codes. Biol Cybern 85: 159-165.
  40. ↵
    Harting JK, Updyke BV, Van Lieshout DP (1992) Corticotectal projections in the cat: anterograde transport studies of twenty-five cortical areas. J Comp Neurol 324: 379-414.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    He P, Kowler E (1989) The role of location probability in the programming of saccades: implications for “center-of-gravity” tendencies. Vision Res 29: 1165-1181.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Horwitz GD, Newsome WT (1999) Separate signals for target selection and movement specification in the superior colliculus. Science 284: 1158-1161.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Horwitz GD, Newsome WT (2001) Target selection for saccadic eye movements: prelude activity in the superior colliculus during a direction-discrimination task. J Neurophysiol 86: 2543-2558.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    Ignashchenkova A, Dicke PW, Haarmeier T, Thier P (2004) Neuron-specific contribution of the superior colliculus to overt and covert shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 7: 56-64.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Judge SJ, Richmond BJ, Chu FC (1980) Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: an improved method. Vision Res 20: 535-538.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Keppel G (1991) Design and analysis: a researcher's handbook, Ed 3. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  47. ↵
    Kowler E (1990) The role of visual and cognitive processes in the control of eye movement. In: Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes (Kowler E, ed), pp 1-63. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  48. ↵
    Kowler E, Blaser E (1995) The accuracy and precision of saccades to small and large targets. Vision Res 35: 1741-1754.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Kowler E, Anderson E, Dosher B, Blaser E (1995) The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Res 35: 1897-1916.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Krauzlis RJ, Dill N (2002) Neural correlates of target choice for pursuit and saccades in the primate superior colliculus. Neuron 35: 355-363.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Kustov AA, Robinson DL (1996) Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements. Nature 384: 74-77.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Lee C, Rohrer WH, Sparks DL (1988) Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. Nature 332: 357-360.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Lee PH, Helms MC, Augustine GJ, Hall WC (1997) Role of intrinsic synaptic circuitry in collicular sensorimotor integration. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94: 13299-13304.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    Lee PH, Schmidt M, Hall WC (2001) Excitatory and inhibitory circuitry in the superficial gray layer of the superior colliculus. J Neurosci 21: 8145-8153.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    Luck SJ, Chelazzi L, Hillyard SA, Desimone R (1997) Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 77: 24-42.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    McIlwain JT (1975) Visual receptive fields and their images in superior colliculus of the cat. J Neurophysiol 38: 219-230.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    McIlwain JT (1982) Lateral spread of neural excitation during microstimulation in intermediate gray layer of cat's superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 47: 167-178.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    McIlwain JT (1986) Point images in the visual system: new interest in an old idea. Trends Neurosci 9: 354-358.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  59. ↵
    McIlwain JT (1991) Distributed spatial coding in the superior colliculus: a review. Vis Neurosci 6: 3-13.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    McPeek RM, Keller EL (2002) Saccade target selection in the superior colliculus during a visual search task. J Neurophysiol 88: 2019-2034.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    McPeek RM, Keller EL (2004) Deficits in saccade target selection after inactivation of superior colliculus. Nat Neurosci 7: 757-763.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Meredith MA, Ramoa AS (1998) Intrinsic circuitry of the superior colliculus: pharmacophysiological identification of horizontally oriented inhibitory interneurons. J Neurophysiol 79: 1597-1602.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    Mize RR (1992) The organization of GABAergic neurons in the mammalian superior colliculus. Prog Brain Res 90: 219-248.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Moore T, Fallah M (2001) Control of eye movements and spatial attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 1273-1276.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    Moore T, Fallah M (2004) Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. J Neurophysiol 91: 152-162.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    Moran J, Desimone R (1985) Selective attention gates visual processing in extrastriate cortex. Science 229: 782-784.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  67. ↵
    Motter BC (1993) Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. J Neurophysiol 70: 909-919.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    Motter BC (1994a) Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4. J Neurosci 14: 2178-2189.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  69. ↵
    Motter BC (1994b) Neural correlates of feature selectivity memory and pop-out in extrastriate area V4. J Neurosci 14: 2190-2199.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  70. ↵
    Muller JR, Philiastides MG, Newsome WT (2005) Inaugural article: microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention without moving the eyes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 524-529.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  71. ↵
    Munoz DP, Wurtz RH (1993a) Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of cell discharge. J Neurophysiol 70: 559-575.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    Munoz DP, Wurtz RH (1993b) Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. II. Reversible activation and deactivation. J Neurophysiol 70: 576-589.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. ↵
    Munoz DP, Wurtz RH (1995) Saccade-related activity in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of burst and buildup cells. J Neurophysiol 73: 2313-2333.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    Munoz DP, Istvan PJ (1998) Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 79: 1193-1209.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  75. ↵
    Ottes FP, Van Gisbergen JAM, Eggermont JJ (1984) Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: two different modes. Vision Res 24: 1169-1179.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    Ottes FP, Van Gisbergen JAM, Eggermont JJ (1986) Visuomotor fields of the superior colliculus: a quantitative model. Vision Res 26: 857-873.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    Ottes FP, Van Gisbergen JAM, Eggermont JJ (1987) Collicular involvement in a saccadic colour discrimination task. Exp Brain Res 66: 465-478.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    Özen G, Helms MC, Hall WC (2004) The intracollicular neuronal network. In: The superior colliculus: new approaches for studying sensorimotor integration (Hall WC, Moschovakis MA, eds), pp 147-158. Boca Raton, FL: CRC.
  79. ↵
    Paré M, Wurtz RH (2001) Progression in neuronal processing for saccadic eye movements from parietal cortex area LIP to superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 85: 2545-2562.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  80. ↵
    Port NL, Wurtz RH (2003) Sequential activity of simultaneously recorded neurons in the superior colliculus during curved saccades. J Neurophysiol 90: 1887-1903.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  81. ↵
    Quaia C, Aizawa H, Optican LM, Wurtz RH (1998) Reversible inactivation of monkey superior colliculus. II. Maps of saccadic deficits. J Neurophysiol 79: 2097-2110.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  82. ↵
    Quaia C, Lefèvre P, Optican LM (1999) Model of the control of saccades by superior colliculus and cerebellum. J Neurophysiol 82: 999-1018.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  83. ↵
    Recanzone GH, Wurtz RH (2000) Effects of attention on MT and MST neuronal activity during pursuit initiation. J Neurophysiol 83: 777-790.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  84. ↵
    Recanzone GH, Wurtz RH, Schwarz U (1997) Responses of MT and MST neurons to one and two moving objects in the receptive field. J Neurophysiol 78: 2904-2915.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  85. ↵
    Reynolds JH, Desimone R (1999) The role of neural mechanisms of attention in solving the binding problem. Neuron 24: 19-29.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L, Desimone R (1999) Competitive mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4. J Neurosci 19: 1736-1753.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  87. ↵
    Reynolds JH, Pasternak T, Desimone R (2000) Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron 26: 703-714.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    Rizzolatti G (1983) Mechanisms of selective attention in mammals. In: Advances in vertebrate neuroethology (Ewert J-P, Capranica R, Ingle DJ, eds). New York: Plenum.
  89. ↵
    Rizzolatti G, Camarda R, Grupp LA, Pisa M (1973) Inhibition of visual responses of single units in the cat superior colliculus by the introduction of a second visual stimulus. Brain Res 61: 390-394.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    Rizzolatti G, Camarda R, Grupp LA, Pisa M (1974) Inhibitory effect of remote visual stimuli on visual response of cat superior colliculus: spatial and temporal factors. J Neurophysiol 37: 1262-1275.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  91. ↵
    Schall JD (1991) Neuronal activity related to visually guided saccades in the frontal eye fields of rhesus monkeys: comparison with supplementary eye fields. J Neurophysiol 66: 559-579.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  92. ↵
    Schall JD (1995) Neural basis of saccade target selection. Rev Neurosci 6: 63-85.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  93. ↵
    Schall JD, Hanes DP, Thompson KG, King DJ (1995) Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation. J Neurosci 15: 6905-6918.
  94. ↵
    Schiller PH, Koerner F (1971) Discharge characteristics of single units in superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 34: 920-936.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  95. ↵
    Schneider WX, Deubel H (2002) Selection-for-perception and selection-for-spatial-motor action are coupled by visual attention: a review of recent findings and new evidence from stimulus driven saccade control. Oxford: Oxford UP.
  96. ↵
    Seidemann E, Newsome WT (1999) Effect of spatial attention on responses of area MT neurons. J Neurophysiol 81: 1783-1794.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  97. ↵
    Shadlen MN, Movshon JA (1999) Synchrony unbound: a critical evaluation of the temporal binding hypothesis. Neuron 24: 67-77.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    Sheliga BM, Riggio L, Rizzolatti G (1994) Orienting of attention and eye movements. Exp Brain Res 98: 507-522.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  99. ↵
    Sheliga BM, Riggio L, Rizzolatti G (1995) Spatial attention and eye movements. Exp Brain Res 105: 261-275.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  100. ↵
    Singer W, Gray CM (1995) Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci 18: 555-586.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  101. ↵
    Sparks DL, Mays LE (1980) Movement fields of saccade-related burst neurons in the monkey superior colliculus. Brain Res 190: 39-50.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  102. ↵
    Sparks DL, Holland R, Guthrie BL (1976) Size and distribution of movement fields in the monkey superior colliculus. Brain Res 113: 21-34.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  103. ↵
    Thompson KG, Hanes DP, Bichot NP, Schall JD (1996) Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. J Neurophysiol 76: 4040-4054.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  104. ↵
    Treisman A (1996) The binding problem. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6: 171-178.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  105. ↵
    Treisman A, Gelade G (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognit Psychol 12: 97-136.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  106. ↵
    Treisman A, Sato S (1990) Conjunction search revisited. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16: 459-478.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  107. ↵
    Treue S, Maunsell JH (1996) Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST. Nature 382: 539-541.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  108. ↵
    Van Gisbergen JAM, Van Opstal AJ, Tax AAM (1987) Collicular ensemble coding of saccades based on vector summation. Neuroscience 21: 541-555.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  109. ↵
    Van Opstal AJ, Kappen H (1993) A two-dimensional ensemble coding model for spatial-temporal transformation of saccades in monkey superior colliculus. Network 4: 19-38.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  110. ↵
    Van Opstal AJ, Van Gisbergen JAM (1989) A nonlinear model for collicular spatial interactions underlying the metrical properties of electrically elicited saccades. Biol Cybern 60: 171-183.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  111. ↵
    Walker R, Deubel H, Schneider WX, Findlay JM (1997) Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. J Neurophysiol 78: 1108-1119.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  112. ↵
    Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (1972) Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey: III. Cells discharging before eye movements. J Neurophysiol 35: 575-586.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  113. ↵
    Wurtz RH, Richmond BJ, Judge SJ (1980) Vision during saccadic eye movements. III. Visual interactions in monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 43: 1168-1181.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 25 (49)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 25, Issue 49
7 Dec 2005
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Competitive Stimulus Interactions within Single Response Fields of Superior Colliculus Neurons
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Competitive Stimulus Interactions within Single Response Fields of Superior Colliculus Neurons
Xiaobing Li, Michele A. Basso
Journal of Neuroscience 7 December 2005, 25 (49) 11357-11373; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3825-05.2005

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Competitive Stimulus Interactions within Single Response Fields of Superior Colliculus Neurons
Xiaobing Li, Michele A. Basso
Journal of Neuroscience 7 December 2005, 25 (49) 11357-11373; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3825-05.2005
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Influence of Reward on Corticospinal Excitability during Movement Preparation
  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.