Abstract
Brief monocular deprivation (≤3 d) induces a rapid shift in the ocular dominance of binocular neurons in the juvenile rodent visual cortex but is ineffective in adults. Here, we report that persistent, rapid, juvenile-like ocular dominance plasticity can be reactivated in adult rodent visual cortex when monocular deprivation is preceded by visual deprivation. Ocular dominance shifts in visually deprived adults are caused by a rapid depression of the response to stimulation of the deprived eye, previously only reported in juveniles, and a simultaneous potentiation of the response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye. The enhanced ocular dominance plasticity induced by visual deprivation persists for days, even if binocular vision precedes monocular deprivation. Visual deprivation also induces a significant decrease in the level of GABAA receptors relative to AMPA receptors and a return to the juvenile form of NMDA receptors in the visual cortex, two molecular changes that we propose enable the persistent reactivation of rapid ocular dominance plasticity.
Introduction
The ocular dominance of binocular neurons in the visual cortex is actively maintained by competition between synapses serving the two eyes. Depriving one eye of vision induces a shift in ocular dominance toward the nondeprived eye (Hubel and Wiesel, 1970). In juvenile rodents, brief (≤3 d) monocular deprivation (MD) induces a rapid depression of the response to stimulation of the deprived eye, followed by a slower potentiation of the response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye (Frenkel and Bear, 2004). These effects of monocular deprivation are blocked by antagonists of the NMDA subtype of glutamate receptor (Bear et al., 1990) and occlude long-term depression induced by low-frequency stimulation (Heynen et al., 2003). In adults, brief monocular deprivation does not induce an ocular dominance shift (Gordon and Stryker, 1996; Sawtell et al., 2003). However, prolonged (≥5 d) monocular deprivation effectively shifts ocular dominance attributable entirely to a potentiation of the response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye. The effects of monocular deprivation in adults share many features with the long-term potentiation (LTP) of synaptic strength induced by high-frequency stimulation, including dependence on NMDA receptor (NMDAR) activation (Heynen and Bear, 2001; Sawtell et al., 2003).
A primary determinant of the level of synaptic plasticity available within the visual system is the balance between cortical inhibition and excitation (I/E) (Fagiolini and Hensch, 2000; Rozas et al., 2001). Over the course of postnatal development, there is an increase in the strength of GABAergic inhibition relative to glutamatergic excitation and a concomitant decrease in the level of ocular dominance plasticity (Huang et al., 1999; Rozas et al., 2001; Desai et al., 2002). In addition, there is developmental regulation of NMDAR composition and function (Carmignoto and Vicini 1992; Quinlan et al., 1999), resulting in changes in the properties of NMDAR-dependent synaptic plasticity. Depriving an animal of vision from birth maintains the visual system in an immature state, characterized by low stimulus selectivity and large receptive fields (Fagiolini et al., 1994). Dark rearing also attenuates the development decline in ocular dominance plasticity (Cynader, 1983), the developmental increase in inhibition relative to excitation (Desai et al., 2002; Morales et al., 2002), and the maturation of NMDARs (Carmignoto and Vicini, 1992; Quinlan et al., 1999). The effects of visual deprivation in a normally reared adult are less well understood. Here, we show that visual deprivation in adulthood reactivates a persistent, juvenile-like response to brief MD, evident as a rapid depression of the response to stimulation of the deprived eye and a potentiation of the response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye. In addition, visual deprivation induces a significant decrease in the level of GABAA receptors (GABAARs) relative to AMPA receptors (AMPARs) and a return to the juvenile form of NMDARs, two changes that we propose enable the persistent reactivation of rapid ocular dominance plasticity in adults.
Materials and Methods
Adult [postnatal day 70 (P70) to P100] male and female pigmented Long–Evans rats were raised on a 12 h light/dark cycle, with food and water available ad libitum. Subjects were moved into a light-tight dark room at indicated ages for visual deprivation. All procedures conform to the guidelines of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services and the University of Maryland Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Monocular deprivation.
Animals were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine (50 mg/10 mg/kg, i.p.). The margins of the upper and lower lids of one eye were trimmed and sutured together. The animals were returned to their home cages for 3 d and disqualified in the event of suture opening or infection.
Visually evoked potential recordings.
Animals were anesthetized with urethane (1.6 g/kg, i.p.). Body temperature and dominant EEG frequency (in response to blank screen) were used to monitor anesthetic depth. A bilateral craniotomy was performed over binocular visual cortex (centered at 7.0 mm posterior to bregma and 4 mm lateral to midline), keeping dura intact. Visually evoked potentials (VEPs) were recorded from the binocular visual cortex with tungsten microelectrodes (0.5 MΩ; MPI, Gaithersburg, MD) relative to a reference electrode in the neck muscle. Assessment of the VEP in response to stimulation of ipsilateral eye confirmed electrode placement in binocular visual cortex.
The visual stimulus was a 1 Hz full-screen vertical square wave grating (0.04 cycles/°) with 96.28% maximal contrast. The amplitude of the primary positive component was used to assess the cortical response to visual stimulation. VEPs were amplified (1000×), filtered (0.5 and 300 Hz), and averaged (100 repetitions) in synchrony with the stimulus using an IBM-compatible computer with Enfant software (Enfant 4010; NeuroScientific, Farmingdale, NY). The data were then digitally filtered with a 60 Hz low-pass filter before analysis.
Quantitative immunoblots.
Binocular visual and frontal cortices were dissected bilaterally after anesthetization with sodium pentobarbital (75 mg/kg, i.p.). Synaptoneurosomes and quantitative immunoblotting were performed as described previously (Quinlan et al., 1999; He et al., 2004). Primary antibodies included anti-GluR2 (clone 6C4; Zymed, San Francisco, CA), anti-GABAAβ2/3 (clone 62-3G1; Upstate Biotechnology, Lake Placid, NY), anti-actin (clone JLA20; Oncogene Sciences, Uniondale, NY), anti-NR2A (rabbit polyclonal; Upstate Biotechnology), and anti-NR2b (rabbit polyclonal; Upstate Biotechnology). Data are expressed as a ratio of optical densities from sequential probes of the same immunoblot, circumventing the need to normalize to a gel-loading control. The relationship between optical density and protein concentration is linear along the range used in this study (Heynen et al., 2000). All immunoblots were performed with the experimenter blind to experimental conditions.
Results
Visual deprivation reactivates persistent, juvenile-like ocular dominance plasticity in adults
Pigmented Long–Evans rats were raised in a normal lighted environment (12 h light/dark cycle) until adulthood (>P60), then placed into a light-tight dark room for 10 d of visual deprivation. VEPs were used to measure the response of the binocular visual cortex to monocular deprivation. In rodents, ∼90% of retinal ganglion cell axons decussate at the optic chiasm, resulting in larger VEPs in response to contralateral (C) versus ipsilateral (I) eye stimulation (Porciatti et al., 1999; Sawtell et al., 2003; Pham et al., 2004). As expected, brief (3 d) monocular deprivation did not affect the VEP C/I in the visual cortex ipsilateral (Ipsi) or contralateral (Contra) to the occluded eye (average ± SEM: Ipsi, 2.48 ± 0.13; Contra, 2.38 ± 0.18; n = 5; p > 0.05, paired t test) (Fig. 1A), confirming the absence of rapid ocular dominance plasticity in adults. However, when brief monocular deprivation was preceded by 10 d of visual deprivation, we observed a significant difference in the contralateral bias in the visual cortex contralateral versus ipsilateral to the occlusion (average ± SEM: Contra, 0.82 ± 0.04; Ipsi, 3.13 ± 0.17; n = 5; p < 0.01, paired t test) (Fig. 1B), demonstrating the reactivation of rapid ocular dominance plasticity in adults.
Visual deprivation reactivates rapid, juvenile-like ocular dominance plasticity in the adult visual cortex. A, Brief (3 d) MD does not induce an ocular dominance shift in light-reared adults. Left, VEP amplitudes (C/I) recorded from each subject in the visual cortex ipsilateral and contralateral to the occluded eye. Right, Summary data reveal no significant difference in VEP C/I across two hemispheres. Inset, Representative VEPs (average of 100 trials) recorded from each hemisphere in response to stimulation of the occluded (filled circle) and open (open circle) eye. Calibration: 100 ms, 50 μV. B, Brief MD after 10 d of visual deprivation induces a significant ocular dominance shift. Left, VEP C/I from each subject. Right, Summary data reveals a significant difference in VEP C/I across two hemispheres, indicating an ocular dominance shift (**p < 0.01, paired t test). Inset, Representative VEPs (average of 100 trials). C, In visually deprived adults, brief MD induces a significant depression in the response to stimulation of the occluded eye and a significant potentiation in the response to stimulation of open eye in the visual cortex (VCtx) contralateral to the occlusion (**p < 0.01 vs light-reared controls, unpaired t test). No difference in the response to stimulation of either eye was observed in the visual cortex ipsilateral to the occlusion (p > 0.05, unpaired t test). amp, Amplitude. Error bars represent SEM.
The rapid ocular dominance shift that we observe in visually deprived (VD) adults may be because of (1) a depression of the response to stimulation of the deprived eye, typically seen only in juveniles or (2) a potentiation of the response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye, which emerges slowly in juveniles and adults. To distinguish between these two possibilities, we compared VEP amplitudes in response to stimulation of each eye after brief monocular deprivation. To facilitate comparison across animals, VEP amplitudes (microvolts) at each recording site were normalized to the sum of the response to stimulation of the contralateral and ipsilateral eye (C + I), which did not differ across hemispheres (p > 0.05, paired t test). VEPs recorded in the visual cortex contralateral to the occlusion revealed two responses to brief monocular deprivation in visually deprived adults: a decrease in the response to stimulation of the occluded eye [average ± SEM (norm to C + I): light-reared (LR), 0.67 ± 0.02, n = 5; VD, 0.46 ± 0.02, n = 5; p < 0.01, unpaired t test] (Fig. 1C) and an increase in the response to stimulation of the open eye [average ± SEM (norm to C + I): LR, 0.33 ± 0.02; VD, 0.54 ± 0.02; p < 0.01, unpaired t test]. We observed no effect in the hemisphere ipsilateral to the occlusion in response to stimulation of the open eye [average ± SEM (norm to C + I): LR, 0.71 ± 0.01; VD, 0.74 ± 0.01; p > 0.05, unpaired t test] or occluded eye (LR, 0.29 ± 0.01; VD, 0.26 ± 0.01; p > 0.05, unpaired t test) (Fig. 1C).
We next tested the durability of the enhanced ocular dominance plasticity induced by visual deprivation by imposing a period of binocular vision (BV) between visual deprivation and monocular deprivation. Remarkably, brief monocular deprivation continued to effectively induce an ocular dominance shift even when 1–3 d of binocular vision followed visual deprivation. The response to brief monocular deprivation in subjects receiving 7 d of binocular vision was indistinguishable from controls (VEP C/I in visual cortex contralateral to the occlusion: VD, 0.82 ± 0.04, n = 5; VD-BV1, 0.94 ± 0.05, n = 5; VD-BV3, 1.19 ± 0.09, n = 5; VD-BV7, 2.86 ± 0.57, n = 4; LR, 2.38 ± 0.18, n = 5; one-way ANOVA, F(4,16) = 8.858, p < 0.01 post hoc analysis) (Fig. 2). We observed no difference in the contralateral bias of the visual cortex ipsilateral to the occlusion across experimental conditions (VD, 3.13 ± 0.17, n = 5; VD-BV1, 3.32 ± 0.08, n = 5; VD-BV3, 3.13 ± 0.47, n = 5; VD-BV7, 2.76 ± 0.38, n = 4; LR, 2.48 ± 0.13, n = 5; one-way ANOVA, F(4,16) = 0.516, p > 0.05). This suggests that visual deprivation promotes a persistent reactivation of juvenile-like ocular dominance plasticity in adult visual cortex.
Visual deprivation induces persistent reactivation of rapid ocular dominance plasticity in adults. Adults received brief MD immediately after 10 d of visual deprivation or when 1, 3, or 7 d of binocular vision was allowed after visual deprivation. C/I VEPs reveal that brief MD continued to effectively induce an OD shift in the visual cortex (VCtx) contralateral to the occluded eye, when 1 or 3 d, but not 7 d, of binocular vision follows visual deprivation (**p < 0.01, one-way ANOVA with post hoc analysis). We see no difference in the C/I VEP in the visual cortex ipsilateral to the occluded eye. Error bars represent SEM.
Visual deprivation reverses two molecular correlates of cortical maturation
To begin to explore the mechanisms by which visual deprivation enhances ocular dominance plasticity, we examined levels of known molecular correlates of cortical maturation in visually deprived juveniles and adults. An antibody that recognizes the β2 and the β3 subunits of the GABAAR was used to track changes in inhibition, because the majority of fast cortical inhibition is mediated by GABAARs containing these subunits (Li and De Blas, 1997). Similarly, we used an antibody that recognizes the GluR2 subunit of the AMPAR to track changes in excitation, because the majority of fast cortical excitation is mediated by AMPARs containing this subunit (Wenthold et al., 1992). Quantitative immunoblotting revealed a significant decrease in GABAAβ2/3 relative to GluR2 in visual cortex in juveniles (P28) dark reared (DR) from birth, compared with age-matched light-reared controls (average ± SEM: LR, 1.15 ± 0.08, n = 7; DR, 0.67 ± 0.21, n = 7; p < 0.05, unpaired t test) (Fig. 3A). To examine the effect of visual deprivation on the adult visual cortex, quantitative immunoblotting was performed on the visual cortex of light-reared adults (P70) or age-matched littermates visually deprived for 10 d beginning at P60. We see a significant decrease in the level of GABAAβ2/3 relative to GluR2 in visually deprived versus light-reared adults (average ± SEM: LR, 1.36 ± 0.13, n = 8; VD, 1.07 ± 0.08, n = 8; p < 0.05, unpaired t test) (Fig. 3A). No difference in GABAAβ2/3 relative to GluR2 was observed in the frontal cortex of these subjects (average ± SEM: LR, 1.41 ± 0.12, n = 8; VD, 1.48 ± 0.14, n = 8; p > 0.05, unpaired t test).
Visual deprivation in adults reverses two molecular correlates of cortical maturation. A, Top, Representative immunoblots for GABAAβ2/3 and GluR2 from juvenile and adult visual cortical homogenate. Bottom, Summary data demonstrate that visual deprivation induces a significant decrease in GABAAβ2/3 relative to GluR2 in juveniles and adults (*p < 0.05, unpaired t test). B, Top, Representative immunoblots for NR2b and NR2a from juvenile and adult visual cortical synaptoneurosomes. Bottom, Summary data demonstrate that visual deprivation induces a significant increase in NR2b relative to NR2a in juveniles and adults (*p < 0.01, unpaired t test). Data were normalized to age-matched LR for display. Error bars represent SEM.
Another well documented consequence of experience-dependent cortical maturation is the decrease in the level of NR2b- relative to NR2a-containing NMDARs in the visual cortex. Quantitative immunoblotting for NR2b and NR2a was performed on synaptoneurosomes prepared from the visual cortex of dark-reared juveniles (P28) or visually deprived adults (P70) and their light-reared age-matched controls. We see a significant increase in NR2b relative to NR2a in dark-reared versus light-reared juveniles (average ± SEM: LR, 0.85 ± 0.09, n = 12; DR, 1.20 ± 0.13, n = 13; p < 0.02, unpaired t test) (Fig. 3B) as a result of a decrease in NR2a [average ± SEM (norm to standard): LR, 0.64 ± 0.06; DR, 0.49 ± 0.06; p < 0.05, unpaired t test] rather than an increase in NR2b [average ± SEM (norm to standard): (LR, 0.51 ± 0.04; DR, 0.56 ± 0.06; p > 0.05, unpaired t test]. In addition, we see a significant increase in NR2b relative to NR2a in visually deprived versus light-reared adults (average ± SEM: LR, 0.82 ± 0.11, n = 6; VD, 1.20 ± 0.07, n = 9; p < 0.01, unpaired t test). Interestingly, the high level of NR2b relative to NR2a in visually deprived adults is attributable to an increase in NR2b [average ± SEM (norm to standard): LR, 0.62 ± 0.10; VD, 1.04 ± 0.12; p < 0.02, unpaired t test] rather than a decrease in NR2a [average ± SEM (norm to standard): LR, 0.75 ± 0.06; VD, 0.89 ± 0.12; p > 0.05, unpaired t test]. We see no significant difference in NR2b relative to NR2a in the frontal cortex of these subjects (average ± SEM: LR, 0.86 ± 0.12, n = 6; VD, 1.03 ± 0.06, n = 9; p > 0.05, unpaired t test).
Discussion
The shift in ocular dominance induced by monocular deprivation is a sensitive index of the synaptic plasticity available in the binocular visual cortex. In juveniles, brief (≤3 d) monocular deprivation induces a rapid depression in the response to stimulation of the deprived eye, followed by a slowly emerging potentiation of the response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye (Frenkel and Bear, 2004). In adults, brief monocular deprivation is ineffective. However, a longer period of monocular deprivation (≥5 d) can induce a shift in ocular dominance, attributable entirely to potentiation of the response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye (Sawtell et al., 2003). Here, we show that persistent, juvenile-like ocular dominance plasticity can be restored in the adult visual cortex by visual deprivation. In visually deprived adults, brief monocular deprivation induces a rapid depression in the response to stimulation of the deprived eye, previously only reported in juveniles, and an acceleration of the potentiation of the response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye, previously found to emerge slowly in juveniles and adults. This noninvasive manipulation provides additional evidence that the adult visual cortex maintains significant potential for synaptic plasticity and that ocular dominance plasticity can be enhanced by visual deprivation throughout life.
Our ability to demonstrate reactivation of rapid, juvenile-like ocular dominance plasticity in the adult visual cortex relied on the use of VEPs to assay the effects of monocular deprivation. Previous experiments have demonstrated that VEPs allow for reliable detection of ocular dominance (Porciatti et al., 1999; Sawtell et al., 2003) and that measurements of visual acuity obtained from VEPs agree with data obtained from behavioral assays in all species examined, including humans (Allen et al., 1986). Curiously, ocular dominance shifts in response to prolonged monocular deprivation have been revealed in the adult visual cortex of rats, cats, and primates by VEP analysis, immediate-early gene expression, and α-calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II staining (Hendry and Kennedy, 1986; Sawtell et al., 2003; Pham et al., 2004; Tagawa et al., 2005) but have not been detected by single-unit recordings. Emerging evidence suggests that ocular dominance plasticity in adults, but not juveniles, can be acutely masked by sodium pentobarbital (Pham et al., 2004), an anesthetic typically used for single-unit recordings. We assay VEPs under urethane anesthesia (Guidel stage III-3, characterized by 0.5–3 Hz spontaneous activity), which may allow the detection of synaptic changes that are masked by sodium pentobarbital. Developmental changes in the sensitivity of GABAARs to the agonist properties of sodium pentobarbital may underlie these differences and explain the success of the VEP method for the study of adult ocular dominance plasticity.
Increasing evidence suggests that the balance between inhibition and excitation may determine the level of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity available to cortical circuits (Fagiolini and Hensch, 2000; Rozas et al., 2001). We showed recently that deafferentation-induced remapping of somatosensory cortex involves multiple adjustments of I/E in the deafferented cortex of adult raccoons (He et al., 2004). The low level of GABAARs relative to AMPARs that we observe in the visual cortex of juveniles dark-reared from birth is consistent with small IPSCs and large EPSCs recorded from visual cortical pyramidal neurons in dark-reared rodents (Desai et al., 2002; Morales et al., 2002). A low level of GABAARs relative to AMPARs was also observed in the adult visual cortex after visual deprivation, suggesting that even in adulthood, visual deprivation can drive the visual system to a more immature state. A low level of inhibition relative to excitation may be required to allow the visual cortex to respond rapidly to manipulations in visual input.
Activation of the NMDA subtype of glutamate receptor is required for many forms of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity, including deprivation-induced ocular dominance shifts (Bear et al., 1990; Sawtell et al., 2003). Here, we show that visual deprivation at any age results in NMDARs with an immature composition, characterized by a high level of the NR2b subunit relative to NR2a. However, in juveniles, visual deprivation appears to prevent the insertion of NR2a-containing NMDARs into the synapse (Quinlan et al., 1999), whereas in adults, visual deprivation appears to stimulate the delivery of NR2b-containing NMDARs to the synapse. In both cases, the deprivation-induced regulation of NMDAR subunit composition would significantly impact the characteristics of NMDAR-dependent synaptic plasticity. Interestingly, NR2a knock-out mice have a significantly reduced ocular dominance shift in response to 4-5 d monocular deprivation (Fagiolini et al., 2003).
It has been proposed previously that the ratio of synaptic NR2b- to NR2a-containing NMDARs determines the threshold for NMDAR-dependent long-term potentiation (Gold and Bear, 1994; Quinlan et al., 1999; Tang et al., 1999). In the juvenile visual cortex, when NR2b-containing NMDARs prevail, the threshold for NMDAR-dependent LTP is low. Over the course of development, there is an experience-dependent decrease in NR2b-containing NMDARs and an associated increase in the threshold for LTP (Kirkwood et al., 1996). The correlation between high levels of NR2b-containing NMDARs and a low threshold for LTP has also been demonstrated after learning (Quinlan et al., 2004), after transgenic overexpression of NR2b (Tang et al., 1999), and after cortical lesions (Huemmeke et al., 2004). The increase in the ratio of NR2b to NR2a observed in visually deprived adults is predicted to lower the threshold for activity-dependent synaptic potentiation, consistent with the rapid potentiation observed in response to stimulation of the nondeprived eye. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that the observed increase in NR2b occurs in a perisynaptic population (Li et al., 1998; Tovar and Westbrook, 2002; Yashiro et al., 2005), contributing to synaptic efficacy independently of synaptic NMDARs (Liu et al., 2004; Massey et al., 2004), or that the direction of the synaptic response is independent of NMDAR subunit composition (Berberich et al., 2005; Zhao et al., 2005).
Ocular dominance plasticity in adults has been shown previously to be facilitated by enzymatic degradation of extracellular matrix components (Pizzorusso et al., 2002), demonstrating that the potential for synaptic plasticity in the visual cortex persists throughout lifetime. Here, we show that 10 d of visual deprivation is sufficient to reactivate rapid, juvenile-like ocular dominance plasticity. Surprisingly, the enhanced ocular dominance plasticity that we observe after visual deprivation persists, even after 3 d of binocular vision. Such a noninvasive method to restore rapid ocular dominance plasticity holds potential therapeutic promise, because the ability to reverse the visual deficits in human amblyopia decreases with age (Mitchell and MacKinnon, 2002).
Note added in proof.
Evidence that the potential for ocular dominance plasticity can be regulated by visual experience was recently provided by Hofer et al., (2006) using optical imaging of intrinsic signals in the mouse visual cortex to demonstrate that monocular deprivation in juveniles facilities ocular dominance plasticity in adults.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant EY13818 and Whitehall Foundation Grant 05-72. We thank Feng Rong for expert technical assistance and Dr. Hey-Kyoung Lee for critical comments on this manuscript.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Elizabeth M. Quinlan, 3245 Biology/Psychology Building #144, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742. Email: equinlan{at}umd.edu