Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Featured ArticleArticles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Brain Activation during Input from Mechanoinsensitive versus Polymodal C-Nociceptors

Belinda Susanne Ruehle, Hermann Otto Handwerker, Jochen Klaus Maria Lennerz, Ralf Ringler and Clemens Forster
Journal of Neuroscience 17 May 2006, 26 (20) 5492-5499; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2059-05.2006
Belinda Susanne Ruehle
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hermann Otto Handwerker
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jochen Klaus Maria Lennerz
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ralf Ringler
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Clemens Forster
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

C-nociceptors mediating cutaneous pain in humans can be distinguished in mechano-heat-responsive units (CMH) and mechano-insensitive units (CMi). However, if sensitized in damaged tissue, CMi play an important role in inflammatory pain. CMi differ from CMH by higher electrical thresholds and by mediating the axon reflex. Using these properties, we established two stimulation paradigms: (1) transcutaneous stimulation (TCS) of low current density below the CMi threshold and (2) intracutaneous stimulation (ICS) of high current density that excites CMi. This was proven by the quantification of the axon-reflex flare. Applying these stimulation paradigms during functional magnetic resonance imaging, we investigated whether nociceptor stimulation that recruits CMi leads to different cerebral activation than stimuli that do not recruit CMi. Brain activation by CMi was inferred by subtraction. Both stimuli recruited multiple afferents other than CMi, and we expected a common network of regions involved in different aspects of pain perception and motor nocifensive reactions in both stimuli. ICS that additionally recruited CMi should activate regions with low acuity that are involved in pain memory and emotional attribution. Besides a common network of pain in both stimuli, TCS activated the supplementary motor area, motor thalamic nuclei, the ipsilateral insula, and the medial cingulate cortex. These regions contribute to a pain processing loop that coordinates the nocifensive motor reaction. CMi nociceptor activation did not cause relevant activation in this loop and does not seem to play a role in withdrawal. The posterior cingulate cortex was selectively activated by ICS and is apparently important for the processing of inflammatory pain.

  • pain
  • nociception
  • electric stimulation
  • fMRI
  • imaging
  • cortical
  • brain

Introduction

It has been known since Erlanger and Gasser that nociceptors comprise thin and slowly conducting nerve fibers (Gasser, 1935). Most of them are unmyelinated C-fibers, and recently a subclass with peculiar properties has been described in human skin (Schmidt et al., 1995; Schmelz et al., 1997). These nociceptors are unresponsive to mechanical stimulation, including pinprick as long as the surrounding tissue is intact. They become mechano responsive in the course of inflammation (Schmelz et al., 1994). This behavior earned them the poetic label “sleeping nociceptors” (Schaible and Schmidt, 1988). First described in the monkey (Meyer and Campbell, 1981; Garell et al., 1996), these cutaneous mechano-insensitive C-nociceptors (CMi) comprise ∼30% of afferent C-units in human skin (Schmidt et al., 1995) and are different from polymodal mechano-heat-sensitive C-nociceptors (CMH) in many respects. CMi have larger receptive fields (Schmidt et al., 2002) and higher heat thresholds (Weidner et al., 1999). Some are particularly sensitive to histamine and mediate itch sensation (Schmelz et al., 1997). CMi are more sensitive to stimulation by inflammatory mediators than CMH (Schmelz et al., 2000). Given these characteristics, it seems likely that CMi are rather involved in hyperalgesia and inflammatory pain than in acute nocifensive reactions (Schmidt et al., 2000). Interestingly, thresholds for transcutaneous electrical stimulation are significantly higher in CMi (Weidner et al., 1999; Schmidt et al., 2002). Based on this knowledge, Schmelz et al. (2000) demonstrated that an axon-reflex flare (Lynn et al., 1996) is only elicited in human skin when the stimulus strength is high enough to recruit CMi.

The clearly separated threshold distribution opens experimental approaches to study the role of CMi. Electrical stimuli can be chosen to excite receptor populations including or excluding CMi. We will show here that the critical parameter is not voltage but current density at the nociceptor terminals.

To gain insight into cerebral processing of CMi, we established two stimulation paradigms: (1) transcutaneous stimulation (TCS) of low current density below the CMi threshold and (2) intracutaneous stimulation (ICS) of high current density that excites CMi, evidenced by quantification of the axon-reflex flare.

Although the central mechanisms of hyperalgesia have been studied previously (Mailhoefner et al., 2004; Mailhoefner and Handwerker, 2005), CMi contribution to the emerging cerebral activation pattern is unclear. We investigated activation patterns of ICS (including CMi) versus TCS (excluding CMi) by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Subsequent subtraction provides indirect evidence of the cerebral correlates of CMi input. Zambreanu et al. (2005) recently suggested a role of the brainstem, cerebellum, and other subcortical and limbic areas in hyperalgesia. We focused on these areas.

According to CMi function, brain areas activated by ICS should play a role in hyperalgesia and inflammatory pain. We expected these to be regions with low acuity such as the cingulate cortex and limbic areas involved in pain memory and emotional attribution (Bromm, 2001; Vogt, 2005). Because both stimuli excite “non-CMi” fibers (e.g., CMH and Aδ) in different quantities (TCS > ICS), TCS and ICS should activate regions with high acuity for nocifensive reactions (e.g., motor integrative and cerebellar areas).

Materials and Methods

General.

Thirteen healthy subjects, age 20–30 years, participated in each the psychophysical/laser-Doppler experiments and in the fMRI study. Nine of them were female. Four subjects, three females, participated in both studies. Subjects gave their informed consent and were instructed that they could withdraw from the experiment at any time. The study was approved by the local ethics committee.

Before the experiment, subjects became familiar with the stimulation procedure and with the experimental protocol. The studies were single blind. Individual sensation and pain thresholds were determined before every experimental session. To determine pain quality, we asked subjects to describe the sensation in their own words. Thresholds were acquired by gradually increasing the current and were analyzed for differences in magnitude and variance between the different experiments and between genders by F and t tests. Data on the female cycle were not collected.

Stimulus design and application.

Electrical stimuli (width, 0.5 ms; frequency, 1 Hz) were applied from a constant current source (Type DS7A, serial D127A; Digitimer, Welwyn Garden City, UK) for 20 s periods. For TCS, two epicutaneous ECG electrodes (17 mm; ARBO; Adshead Ratcliffe & Company, Derbyshire, UK) were attached 1.5 cm apart to the dorsum of the foot. For ICS, two hypodermic needles (0.5 × 25 mm; Microlance, number 18; Becton Dickinson, Mountain View, CA) were inserted intracutaneously 1.5 mm apart and each to a depth of 2 mm.

Psychophysics and laser Doppler imaging.

Intracutaneous needle electrodes were inserted in the right (n = 6) or left (n = 5) dorsum of the foot. Epicutaneous electrodes were attached to the opposite foot. A 30 min period was allowed between needle insertion and stimulation to let the flare by needle insertion completely fade. Subjects were instructed to rate each stimulation on a 0–10 numeric pain rating scale. TCS started with 5 mA. The current was increased stepwise (5, 10, 15, 20, 25, 30, 40, 50, 60, and 70 mA) until the pain was rated 7 on a scale from 1 to 10 or until the ceiling of 70 mA was reached. ICS started with 1 mA (1, 2, 4, 6, 8, 10, 13, 16, 20, and 24 mA). The cutoff was either a pain rating of 7 or the ceiling 24 mA. Subjects who reported pain levels <7 at the maximal stimulation intensity were excluded from the study.

Blood flux measurements were performed using a Laser Doppler Imager (Moor Instruments, Wilmington, DE) positioned over the dorsum of the foot. The scanned field (size, 4.5 cm2; scan time for each image, 30 s) completely covered the stimulation area. After recording a 30 s baseline (first image), stimulation was applied during the first 20 s of the second image. Subsequently, an additional seven 30 s images were obtained. For each stimulus level, a series of nine pictures (4.5 min) was recorded.

Laser Doppler imaging (LDI) software (LDI version 3.08; Moor Instruments) was used for evaluation. The baseline was subtracted from all poststimulus scans. The average flux increase was determined in a region of interest (ROI) (see Fig. 2). For ICS, the ROI was chosen at a distance from the needles to avoid the erythema caused by needle insertion. Flux increase and flux variance in the two genders were analyzed by the F test and t test to detect intersexual differences.

fMRI of the brain.

Epicutaneous electrodes for TCS were attached to the dorsum of the right foot. Intracutaneous needle electrodes for ICS were positioned between the epicutaneous electrodes. Although the stimulation area in TCS was larger, this positioning granted topographically identical stimulation sites. Stimulation strengths were determined before each session to obtain a pain rating of 7. Each fMRI session consisted of two separate trials for either TCS or ICS. In each trial, five 20 s stimuli of the same type (either ICS or TCS) were applied. The baseline time between stimuli was varied to avoid bias by pain anticipation (Ploghaus et al., 1999). The fMRI session was started randomly with the TCS trial in nine cases. To avoid task-related bias, we did not obtain a simultaneous rating. Instead, the same protocol was applied in a separate session without imaging to all subjects, and an on-line rating through the five stimulation periods was recorded with a visual rating scale.

Imaging was performed using a 1.5 T Sonata MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). For each subject, a T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo data set was recorded with a high three-dimensional resolution covering the entire brain (160 sagittal slices; thickness, 1.5 mm; in-plane resolution, 0.98 × 0.98 mm2; field of view, 250 × 250 mm2; data matrix, 256 × 256). Functional data were collected using a multislice echo planar imaging (EPI) technique. Each functional data set consisted of 93 blocks, visualizing 16 slices (Fig. 1) [repetition time, 114 ms; echo time, 62 ms; flip angle, 90°; scan time, 2.5 s per block of 16 slices; slice thickness, 4 mm; field of view, 220 × 220 mm2; data matrix, 128 × 128] (Ringler et al., 2003). Stimulation was administered during blocks 14–20, 29–35, 46–52, 60–66, and 77–83.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Positioning of the EPI slices for the collection of the functional data. The block consisted of 16 slices, each with a thickness of 4 mm. The scan time was a repetition time of 2.5 s per block.

The field of view was chosen to completely cover the thalamus, brainstem, cerebellum, and the following cortical areas often described to be involved in the pain network: supplementary motor area (SMA), secondary motor cortex (S2), medial and posterior insular cortices, and medial and posterior cingulate cortices. As a consequence of this selection, however, S1, the anterior pole of the insula, and the perigenuate cingulate cortex were outside of this block.

fMRI data were statistically analyzed with BrainVoyager software version 4.8 (Brain Innovations, Maastricht, The Netherlands). Functional and anatomical images of all subjects were superimposed and transformed into Talairach space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988). The three-dimensional preprocessing of the functional data included head movement assessment, slice scan time correction, high-frequency filtering, and removal of linear trends. Changes in the blood oxygenation level (BOLD) signal were statistically evaluated using a general linear model (GLM) with the stimulus function as a predictor that corresponded to a boxcar design. Data were low-pass filtered (cutoff frequency, 0.2 Hz) to account for the hemodynamic response delay (Boynton et al., 1996). A minimal cluster size of 80 pixels was used as a cutoff to eliminate very small or random activations. Clusters were assigned manually to anatomical loci by a professional viewer based on a printed brain atlas (Kretschmann and Weinrich, 1992; Damasio, 1995). The t values obtained from the GLM analysis were used to compare activation patterns. p values <0.05 were regarded as significant. Findings were verified by processing the centric coordinates with the Talairach Daemon Client internet software (http://ric.uthscsa.edu/projects/talairachdaemon.html). Group analyses, which included the data of all subjects during either type of stimulus, were performed separately on each of the two trials. A third group analysis was performed for contrast evaluation. It was done with the z-transformed data of both trials, and the predictors were stimulus onset and type of stimulus. To characterize the most relevant activated regions, two parameters reflecting the difference between baseline and fMRI signal during the stimuli were selected: BOLD contrast and t value. For each brain ROI, the number of subjects showing significant activation was listed. Data of male versus female subjects were analyzed in a set of two Multi Studies to detect gender differences.

Results

Pain perception

ICS-induced pain was uniformly described as “stinging,” whereas TCS-induced pain was perceived as “deep,” “pounding,” or “dull.” Because these descriptions were given in an unstructured form, no statistics can be provided. However, no subject described ICS as deep, pounding, or dull or described TCS as stinging. Four of 17 subjects felt TCS to be more unpleasant, 5 subjects felt ICS to be more unpleasant, whereas 8 subjects found both types of stimulation likewise unpleasant. Pain thresholds varied widely. During the LDI experiments, TCS became painful at 18.9 mA, on average, (range, 11.1–29.1). Pain ratings increased to 7 when the current was raised to 40.8 mA, on average (range, 20.0–68.3). TCS thresholds during the fMRI session were lower at 10.7 mA, on average (range, 4.0–20.6), and the current required to reach a rating of 7 was 30.3 mA, on average (range, 15.0–44.1). For ICS, thresholds and rating 7 currents were equal in both experiments. The mean pain threshold was 2.7 mA (range, 0.2–6.0), and rating 7 was reached at 14.9 mA, on average (range, 6.3–49.3). There was no significant difference in pain perception regarding quality, threshold variance (F test), or threshold (t test) between the two genders. A simultaneous rating over the five stimulation periods of the fMRI stimulation protocol showed a consistent rating of 7 during all five stimulation periods and a complete cessation of pain during baseline periods.

LDI: local stimulation effects

In the LDI experiments, one subject had to be excluded because a pain rating of 7 was not reached at the maximal stimulation intensity of 24 mA for ICS. Two subjects were excluded for movement artifacts. Consequently, the data from 10 subjects were analyzed. No erythema was observed even at the highest TCS stimulus levels (70 mA), and no subject showed TCS-induced flux increases in LDI. Before ICS measurements were started, the erythema caused by the insertion of the needles had completely faded in all subjects. Baseline LDI measurements still showed an increased flux and were subtracted. After ICS stimulation, a visible erythema developed again. This was especially pronounced at higher stimulus intensities. The LDI flux values clearly rose after the stimulus and reached a maximum ∼90 s after stimulation. Maximal and integrated flux was positively correlated to stimulation intensity for ICS (Fig. 2), resulting in a maximal difference between ICS and TCS flux values at rating 7. Integrated and maximal flux after ICS was greater than after TCS in every single subject (integrated flux after baseline subtraction at rating 7: ICS: mean, 576.5; SE 58.0; TCS: mean, 274.4; SE, 10.1.) Integrated flux (t test) and variance (F test) were not significantly different between the two genders.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Flare reactions in the skin of the dorsum of the foot. a, Baseline-subtracted LDIs from one representative subject for ICS (top block of records) and TCS (bottom block of records). Recording of each scan started at the time in seconds indicated in the white boxes. The ROI and electrode positions are depicted in the first image (baseline). A stimulus that lasted 20 s (ICS, 8 mA; TCS, 25 mA; rating 7) was applied during the second picture. Pictures 3–7are poststimulus scans. Altogether, a time frame of 210 s after stimulation is covered. Flux values are encoded by the pixel color. The corresponding flux values are displayed in the scale on the right. After ICS, a pronounced flux increase could be observed, whereas TCS did not provoke changes in the LDF signal. b, Mean flux time courses of all subjects (n = 10) at rating 7 (squares) and rating 3 (triangles) for ICS (filled symbols) and TCS (open symbol). SEs are displayed. The black box marks the stimulus application time. ICS provokes a significant increase of flux with a maximum at 90–120 s after the onset of stimulation. The increase is significantly larger at rating 7 than at rating 3. TCS does not provoke flux increase at any level of stimulation. Thus, only flux at rating 7 is displayed. c, Map of the electrode positions for the LDI trial: needle electrodes for ICS are exemplary displayed on the dorsum of the left foot, and surface electrodes for TCS exemplary displayed on the dorsum of the right foot. Scanned fields are plotted in blue. These scanned fields correspond to the displayed area in a.

Cerebral responses: BOLD effects

Two subjects were excluded because of movement artifacts, and accordingly, the data from 11 subjects were analyzed. Table 1 lists activations in the most relevant ROIs during ICS and TCS stimulation. In addition, we found that motor, visual, and auditory cortical areas were activated in single subjects. Because these activations occurred only in single subjects, they were not included in the tables. A group analysis using the predictor gender showed no significant differences in activation between male and female subjects.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Stimulus-dependent activation in the brain ROIs

Analysis of signal change over time revealed a second peak after stimulus termination in most brain regions. Interestingly, time courses displayed a symmetrical pattern within the ROI with bilateral activation. Different stimuli, however, evoked distinct reactions in the same ROI. Example time courses are plotted in Figures 3⇓–5.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Multi Studies of activations by both stimuli in a standardized Talairach brain. Activation by ICS is plotted in red; activation by TCS is plotted in green. Common activation by ICS and TCS is shown in yellow. This sagittal slice shows activation in the cingulate cortex, thalamus, and cerebellar vermis. Only ICS activates the PCC, whereas TCS dominates in the MCC. The mean relative changes of the BOLD signals in the MCC and PCC during the stimuli are plotted on the left: the time courses over the five stimulation periods are averaged for all subjects. They include 15 s before the 20 s stimulation period and 20 s after stimulation. TCS (green frame) and ICS (red frame) activations are linked to the corresponding clusters. The BOLD signal of the left hemisphere is plotted in black, and the BOLD signal of the right side is plotted in blue. Stimulation periods are indicated by the gray bars on the x-axis.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Multi Studies as in Figure 3. Activation by ICS and by TCS is in red and green, respectively, and common activation is in yellow. BOLD signals of the left hemisphere are plotted on the right side and are contralateral to the stimulus. a, This axial slice shows that insular activation by ICS is strictly contralateral, whereas TCS activates the MIC in both hemispheres. S2 activation is found bilaterally during both types of stimulation. b, This axial slice shows bilateral thalamus activation in both stimuli. Thalamus activation by TCS activation is rostral to ICS activation. Activation over time is plotted for the medial and PIC (a) and the thalamus (b). The time courses over the five stimulation periods are averaged for all subjects. They include 15 s before the 20 s stimulation period and 20 s after stimulation. TCS (green frame) and ICS (red frame) activations are linked to the corresponding clusters. The BOLD signal of the left hemisphere is plotted in black, and the BOLD signal of the right side is plotted in blue. Stimulation periods are indicated by the gray bars on the x-axis.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Multi Studies as in Figure 3. Activation by ICS and TCS is in red and green, respectively, and common activation is in yellow. a, This axial slice shows activation in the contralateral brainstem and the cerebellar vermis. b, This axial slice shows bilateral activation in the AC during both types of stimulation. Activation over time in is plotted for brainstem (a) and cerebellum (b). The time courses over the five stimulation periods are averaged for all subjects. They include 15 s before the 20 s stimulation period and 20 s after stimulation. TCS (green frame) and ICS (red frame) activations are linked to the corresponding clusters. The BOLD signal of the left hemisphere is plotted in black, and the BOLD signal of the right side is plotted in blue. Stimulation periods are indicated by the gray bars on the x-axis.

Although most ROIs were bilaterally activated, there were some remarkable exceptions. During ICS stimulation, only the contralateral medial cingulate cortex (MCC) [Brodmann’s area (BA) 24] and medial (BA 13) and posterior (BA 14, 15, 16) insular cortex and brainstem were activated. TCS lead to an exclusive contralateral activation in the posterior insular cortex (PIC) and brainstem.

Most ROIs were responsive to both types of stimulation. Generally, TCS resulted in larger clusters, stronger changes in BOLD contrast, and higher t values.

However, some areas were selectively or predominantly activated by one stimulus type. Table 2 lists the significant contrasts of ICS and TCS including Talairach coordinates. In cases of differently located activations by ICS and TCS, both clusters are listed.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Contrast evaluation of brain ROIs

Most notably, the SMA (BA 6) was activated only by TCS, whereas the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) (BA 23, 29, 30, 31) was only activated by ICS (Fig. 3). Contrast analysis emphasizes the finding in the PCC and the left SMA. TCS activation in the right SMA does not produce a significant contrast to ICS. Some other areas, in particular the medial insular cortex (MIC) and MCC, showed ipsilateral activation only during TCS (Fig. 4). However, contrast analysis could only verify a significant dominance of TCS in the MIC. Figure 3 shows selective activation in the PCC by ICS and activation in the MCC.

Although Multi Studies analysis does not imply major differences in S2, left PIC, cerebellar vermis (vermis), and left cerebellar hemisphere (Fig. 4, 5), contrast analysis reveals a significant dominance of TCS in these regions (Table 2).

Even when both types of stimuli excited the same ROI, the activated fields were not always identical, particularly in the thalamus (Fig. 4) and brainstem (Fig. 5). For these areas, contrast analysis reveals significant clusters with slightly different coordinates for both stimuli.

Discussion

This study differentiates parts of the cerebral network of pain that are responsible for input from cutaneous afferents including or excluding mechano-insensitive C-nociceptors. To separate CMi, we used ICS to excite cutaneous nerve endings including CMi, as evidenced by the axon-reflex flare (Fig. 2). In contrast, TCS recruited no or only a negligible amount of CMi, as evidenced by the absence of an axon reflex. These stimulation paradigms have implications that deserve further comment. The voltage of TCS had to be higher to achieve equal pain intensities. Therefore, TCS induced a greater current spread and recruited more cutaneous units including Aδ, CMH, and other afferents. Low-threshold muscle afferents were probably not recruited because, at the dorsum of the foot, no superficial musculature is close by. In comparison, ICS with its high current density recruited the same type of afferents plus CMi. This is known from studies on primary afferents and evidenced by the axon reflex. Our group has studied the afferent and efferent innervation of the lower leg and foot with microneurography for years (Torebjörk et al., 1996). Among hundreds of C-fibers recorded, no low-threshold C-mechanoreceptors (Olausson et al., 2002) have been encountered in this region. They are probably characteristic for the hairy skin in other parts of the limbs. C-cold units are rare compared with nociceptors (Serra et al., 1999) and have electrical thresholds that are lower or in the same range as CMH. Although Aδ units were not as systematically studied as C-fibers, their electrical thresholds were always lower than those of CMi and usually also lower than those of CMH (our unpublished observations). It cannot entirely be excluded that unknown types of Aδ units exist and are recruited by ICS but not by TCS. Studies in the monkey have shown that some Aδ units have both myelinated and unmyelinated terminal branches (Peng et al., 1999). However, the thickest of these branches have the lowest electrical thresholds and are recruited by TCS. Even if unknown fiber types exist, our stimulus paradigms differentiate between a group of nociceptors responsible for the axon reflex flare versus those that induce pain but no axon reflex flare. When brain regions were preferentially activated by TCS, it was certainly caused by the quantitatively greater input. However, preferential activations by ICS must be attributed to the qualitative (presumably CMi input) and not the quantitative differences.

We differentiated three parts of the cerebral network of pain responsible for the following: both painful stimuli (ICS plus TCS), preferential input from CMH and Aδ (TCS), and input from CMi (applying subtraction ICS minus TCS). Besides a common network in both stimuli, we found that mainly input from TCS activates regions involved in nocifensive motor reactions, whereas ICS activates PCC, suggesting a role in processing CMi input.

Common cerebral network of pain

The common network comprises S2 (BA 43), insular cortex, MCC, thalamus, brainstem, vermis, and anterior cerebellar hemisphere (AC). These areas belong to the most consistently activated regions in pain imaging studies along with S1, perigenuate cingulate cortex, and anterior insula (Peyron et al., 2001; Helmchen et al., 2003; Apkarian et al., 2005). These three regions, however, were not included in our measurements. Our focus lay on insula, cingulate, and thalamus in addition to subcortical areas including the cerebellum and brainstem. Zambreanu et al. (2005) recently pointed out their eminent role in pain perception.

We found contralateral brainstem activation during both stimuli. Given the proximity to subthalamic nuclei and substantia nigra, this activation might be a correlate of motor inhibition because subjects had to depress nocifensive movements. It might also reflect ascending pathways to the thalamus because TCS activation is located rostral to ICS in the brainstem and thalamus. However, an exact allocation to specific tracts or nuclei cannot be made for anatomical variation.

Pain processing network activated by TCS

Motor thalamic nuclei, SMA, and ipsilateral MIC are selectively activated and S2, MCC, vermis, and ipsilateral AC are predominantly activated by TCS. They apparently contribute to a pain processing loop involved in the nocifensive motor response.

We found overlapping TCS and ICS activation in the posterior ventral and lateral thalamic nuclei. These nuclei manage sensory integration and project to S1, S2, and the parietal association cortex. TCS additionally activated the anterior and lateral ventral nuclei with integrative motor function and prominent projections to the primary motor cortex, premotor cortex, and SMA.

Both stimuli activated bilateral S2 that is involved in sensory discrimination and integration. Contrast analysis revealed a significant dominance of TCS that may be attributed to spatial encoding of the exact pain location to plan withdrawal.

The insula is known as a central station of pain processing, important in visceral (Augustine et al., 1996), sympathetic (Sandroni et al., 1998), emotional (Morris, 2002; Kong et al., 2006), memory (Treede et al. 2000), and discriminative (Coghill et al., 1999; Peyron et al., 1999; Hua et al., 2005) aspects of pain perception. We found two foci of insular cortex activation: MIC and PIC (Figs. 4, 5). Both stimuli evoked contralateral MIC and PIC activation. This might reflect the memory and emotional component of pain or account for sympathetic activation. However, TCS dominated in the contralateral PIC that accounts for discrimination (Brooks et al., 2005). Activation threshold differences between the ipsilateral and contralateral MIC suggest that pain (Mailhofner et al., 2004; Peyron et al., 2004) or even pain intensity (Coghill et al., 2001) is encoded in this region. Although likewise painful, TCS evoked bilateral MIC activation, whereas ICS was strictly contralateral. This suggests a role of MIC beyond pain intensity encoding. Considering the functional differences of both stimuli (acute vs inflammatory pain) and the different cortical activation patterns, we suggest that MIC is involved in the setup of withdrawal.

The MIC has multiple connections with areas involved in pain perception (Jasmin et al., 2004). Besides S2 and the thalamus (Craig et al., 1994), MIC directly interacts with MCC (BA 24) (Friedman and Murray, 1986; Friedman et al., 1986). In concert with the insular response, we found bilateral MCC activation in TCS, whereas ICS only activated the contralateral hemisphere. However, contrast analysis revealed no significant activation for TCS over ICS. The MCC is involved in motor nocifensive and autonomic reactions (Bromm, 2001), skeleto-motor orientation, and fear avoidance (Vogt, 2005). Morecraft et al. (1992) found BA 23c and 24c (MCC) projections to the SMA in the rhesus monkey that suggest a functional circuit.

The SMA is only active during TCS. Previous studies have shown SMA activation by noxious heat (Coghill et al., 1994; Becerra et al., 1999; Chen et al., 2002) and capsaicin (Iadarola et al., 1998). Coghill et al. (1994) suggested a role of the SMA in movement planning and readiness to withdraw.

Helmchen et al. (2003) have postulated a specific role in pain perception for regions in the anterior cerebellum and the vermis. We found vermis activation in both stimuli. This is consistent with previous pain imaging studies (Casey et al., 1994, 1996; Becerra et al., 1999; Brooks et al., 2002) and probably caused by direct spinocerebellar input (Ekerot et al., 1991; Iadarola et al., 1998). Consistent with previous reports (Coghill et al., 2001; Bingel et al., 2002), both stimuli evoked bilateral AC activation. Contrast analysis revealed significant dominance of TCS in the vermis and ipsilateral AC, a possible correlate of motor integration (Saab and Willis, 2003) and conditioning of nocifensive responses (Ploghaus et al., 1999, 2000).

The interconnections of S2, MIC, motor thalamic nuclei, MCC, and SMA combined with the coincidence of activation during TCS and the dominance of TCS in the cerebellum suggest that these regions comprise a pain processing loop. According to the main functions of the contributing regions, this loop is involved in the coordination of motor nocifensive behavior. Only TCS evoked this activation pattern, elicited by recruitment of a larger number of cutaneous afferents.

Pain processing network activated by ICS

Interestingly, only ICS activated the PCC. We suggest that this is as a result of the additional input from CMi. According to Vogt et al. (2003), 84% of cingulate activation during noxious heat is found in the MCC. This inconsistent activation of the PCC (Becerra et al., 1999; Chen et al., 2002; Valet et al., 2004) by thermal stimulation may be attributed to the higher heat thresholds of CMi compared with CMH (Weidner et al., 1999). The PCC seems to play an important role in the processing of CMi input and thereby inflammation pain and hyperalgesia.

In conclusion, our stimulation paradigms excite different nociceptor subclasses that are primarily processed in brain regions well known to be involved in pain perception. However, there are important differences between ICS and TCS. Regions involved in nocifensive motor reactions are preferentially activated by TCS. In contrast, only ICS activated PCC, generally thought to be involved in pain memory and aversion (Bromm, 2001) and emotional processes (Vogt, 2005). Our data suggest a role of PCC in processing CMi input and provide insight into the cerebral integration of hyperalgesia and inflammatory pain.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft. We gratefully acknowledged the help of R. Carr and K. Zimmermann in the preparation of this manuscript.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Clemens Forster, Institute of Physiology and Pathophysiology, University of Erlangen/Nuernberg, Universitaetsstrasse 17, D-91054 Erlangen, Germany. Email: forster{at}physiologie1.uni-erlangen.de

References

  1. ↵
    Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede RD, Zubieta JK (2005) Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain 9:463–484.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. Augustine JR (1996) Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 22:229–244.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Becerra LR, Breiter HC, Stojanovic M, Fishman S, Edwards A, Comite AR, Gonzalez RG, Borsook D (1999) Human brain activation under controlled thermal stimulation and habituation to noxious heat: an fMRI study. Magn Reson Med 41:1044–1057.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Bingel U, Quante M, Knab R, Bromm B, Weiller C, Buchel C (2002) Subcortical structures involved in pain processing: evidence from single-trial fMRI. Pain 99:313–321.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Boynton GM, Engel SA, Glover GH, Heeger DJ (1996) Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J Neurosci 16:4207–4221.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Bromm B (2001) Brain images of pain. News Physiol Sci 16:244–249.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Brooks JC, Nurmikko TJ, Bimson WE, Singh KD, Roberts N (2002) fMRI of thermal pain: effects of stimulus laterality and attention. NeuroImage 15:293–301.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Brooks JC, Zambreanu L, Godinez A, Craig AD, Tracey I (2005) Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging. NeuroImage 27:201–209.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Casey KL, Minoshima S, Berger KL, Koeppe RA, Morrow TJ, Frey KA (1994) Positron emission tomographic analysis of cerebral structures activated specifically by repetitive noxious heat stimuli. J Neurophysiol 71:802–807.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Casey KL, Minoshima S, Morrow TJ, Koeppe RA (1996) Comparison of human cerebral activation pattern during cutaneous warmth, heat pain, and deep cold pain. J Neurophysiol 76:571–581.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    Chen AC, Niddam DM, Crawford HJ, Oostenveld R, Arendt-Nielsen L (2002) Spatial summation of pain processing in the human brain as assessed by cerebral event related potentials. Neurosci Lett 328:190–194.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Coghill RC, Talbot JD, Evans AC, Meyer E, Gjedde A, Bushnell MC, Duncan GH (1994) Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human brain. J Neurosci 14:4095–4108.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  13. ↵
    Coghill RC, Sang CN, Maisog JH, Iadarola MJ (1999) Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism. J Neurophysiol 82:1934–1943.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    Coghill RC, Gilron I, Iadarola MJ (2001) Hemispheric lateralization of somatosensory processing. J Neurophysiol 85:2602–2612.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    Craig AD, Bushnell MC, Zhang ET, Blomqvist A (1994) A thalamic nucleus specific for pain and temperature sensation. Nature 372:770–773.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Damasio H (1995) In: Human brain anatomy in computerizes images New York: Oxford UP.
  17. ↵
    Ekerot CF, Garwicz M, Schouenborg J (1991) Topography and nociceptive receptive fields of climbing fibres projecting to the cerebellar anterior lobe in the cat. J Physiol (Lond) 441:257–274.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    Friedman DP, Murray EA (1986) Thalamic connectivity of the second somatosensory area and neighboring somatosensory fields of the lateral sulcus of the macaque. J Comp Neurol 252:348–373.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Friedman DP, Murray EA, O’Neill JB, Mishkin M (1986) Cortical connections of the somatosensory fields of the lateral sulcus of macaques: evidence for a corticolimbic pathway for touch. J Comp Neurol 252:323–347.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Garell PC, McGillis SL, Greenspan JD (1996) Mechanical response properties of nociceptors innervating feline hairy skin. J Neurophysiol 75:1177–1189.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    Gasser HS (1935) Conduction in nerves in relation to fiber types. Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Med Dis 15:35–59.
    OpenUrl
  22. ↵
    Helmchen C, Mohr C, Erdmann C, Petersen D, Nitschke MF (2003) Differential cerebellar activation related to perceived pain intensity during noxious thermal stimulation in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett 335:202–206.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    Hua LH, Strigo IA, Baxter LC, Johnson SC, Craig AD (2005) Anteroposterior somatotopy of innocuous cooling activation focus in human dorsal posterior insular cortex. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 289:R319–R325.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    Iadarola MJ, Berman KF, Zeffiro TA, Byas-Smith MG, Gracely RH, Max MB, Bennett GJ (1998) Neural activation during acute capsaicin-evoked pain and allodynia assessed with PET. Brain 121:931–947.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Jasmin L, Burkey AR, Granato A, Ohara PT (2004) Rostral agranular insular cortex and pain areas of the central nervous system: a tract-tracing study in the rat. J Comp Neurol 468:425–440.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Kong J, White NS, Kwong KK, Vangel MG, Rosman IS, Gracely RH, Gollub RL (2006) Using fMRI to dissociate sensory encoding from cognitive evaluation of heat pain intensity. Hum Brain Mapp in press.
  27. ↵
    Kretschmann H-J, Weinrich W (1992) In: Cranial neuroimaging and clinical neuroanatomy New York: Thieme Medical Publishers.
  28. ↵
    Lynn B, Schutterle S, Pierau FK (1996) The vasodilator component of neurogenic inflammation is caused by a special subclass of heat-sensitive nociceptors in the skin of the pig. J Physiol (Lond) 494:587–593.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    Maihofner C, Handwerker HO (2005) Differential coding of hyperalgesia in the human brain: a functional MRI study. NeuroImage 28:996–1006.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Maihofner C, Schmelz M, Forster C, Neundorfer B, Handwerker HO (2004) Neural activation during experimental allodynia: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Eur J Neurosci 19:3211–3218.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Meyer RA, Campbell JN (1981) Evidence for two distinct classes of unmyelinated nociceptive afferents in monkey. Brain Res 224:149–152.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. Morecraft RJ, Van Hoesen GW (1992) Cingulate input to the primary and supplementary motor cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for somatotopy in areas 24c and 23c. J Comp Neurol 322:471–489.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Morris JS (2002) How do you feel. Trends Cogn Sci 8:317–319.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    Olausson H, Lamarre Y, Backlund H, Morin C, Wallin BG, Starck G, Ekholm S, Strigo I, Worsley K, Vallbo AB, Bushnell MC (2002) Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and project to insular cortex. Nat Neurosci 9:900–904.
  35. ↵
    Peng YB, Ringkamp M, Campbell JN, Meyer RA (1999) Electrophysiological assessment of the cutaneous arborization of Adelta-fiber nociceptors. J Neurophysiol 82:1164–1177.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Peyron R, Garcia-Larrea L, Gregoire MC, Costes N, Convers P, Lavenne F, Mauguiere F, Michel D, Laurent B (1999) Haemodynamic brain responses to acute pain in humans: sensory and attentional networks. Brain 122:1765–1780.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia-Larrea L (2001) Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000). Neurophysiol Clin 30:263–288.
  38. ↵
    Peyron R, Schneider F, Faillenot I, Convers P, Barral FG, Garcia-Larrea L, Laurent B (2004) An fMRI study of cortical representation of mechanical allodynia in patients with neuropathic pain. Neurology 63:1838–1846.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Gati JS, Clare S, Menon RS, Matthews PM, Rawlins JNP (1999) Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science 284:1979–1981.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Clare S, Gati JS, Rawlins JN, Matthews PM (2000) Learning about pain: the neural substrate of the prediction error for aversive events. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:9281–9286.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Ringler R, Greiner M, Kohlloeffel L, Handwerker HO, Forster C (2003) BOLD effects in different areas of the cerebral cortex during painful mechanical stimulation. Pain 105:445–453.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Saab CY, Willis WD (2003) The cerebellum: organization, functions and its role in nociception. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 42:85–95.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. Sandroni P (1998) Testing the autonomic nervous system. Int Assoc Study Pain Newsletter November.
  44. ↵
    Schaible HG, Schmidt RF (1988) Excitation and sensitization of fine articular afferents from cat’s knee joint by prostaglandin E2. J Physiol (Lond) 403:91–104.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    Schmelz M, Schmidt R, Ringkamp M, Handwerker HO, Torebjörk HE (1994) Sensitization of insensitive branches of C nociceptors in human skin. J Physiol (Lond) 480:389–394.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    Schmelz M, Schmidt R, Bickel A, Handwerker HO, Torebjörk HE (1997) Specific C-receptors for itch in human skin. J Neurosci 17:8003–8008.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    Schmelz M, Michael K, Weidner C, Schmidt R, Torebjörk HE, Handwerker HO (2000) Which nerve fibers mediate the axon reflex flare in human skin? NeuroReport 11:645–648.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Schmidt R, Schmelz M, Forster C, Ringkamp M, Torebjörk E, Handwerker H (1995) Novel classes of responsive and unresponsive C nociceptors in human skin. J Neurosci 15:333–341.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  49. ↵
    Schmidt R, Schmelz M, Torebjörk HE, Handwerker HO (2000) Mechano-insensitive nociceptors encode pain evoked by tonic pressure to human skin. Neuroscience 98:793–800.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Schmidt R, Schmelz M, Weidner C, Handwerker HO, Torebjörk HE (2002) Innervation territories of mechano-insensitive C nociceptors in human skin. J Neurophysiol 88:1859–1866.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    Serra J, Campero M, Ochoa J, Bostock H (1999) Activity-dependent slowing of conduction differentiates functional subtypes of C fibres innervating human skin. J Physiol (Lond) 515:799–811.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) In: Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain, 3-dimensional proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging New York: Thieme Medical Publishers.
  53. ↵
    Torebjörk HE, Schmelz M, Handwerker HO (1996) Functional properties of human cutaneous nociceptors and their role in pain and hyperalgesia. In: Neurobiology of nociceptors (Belmonte C, Cervero F, eds) pp. 349–369. New York: Oxford UP.
  54. ↵
    Treede RD, Apkarian AV, Bromm B, Greenspan JD, Lenz FA (2000) Cortical representation of pain: functional characterization of nociceptive areas near the lateral sulcus. Pain 87:113–119.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Valet M, Sprenger T, Boecker H, Willoch F, Rummeny E, Conrad B, Erhard P, Tolle TR (2004) Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain–an fMRI analysis. Pain 109:399–408.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Vogt BA (2005) Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:533–544.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Vogt BA, Berger GR, Derbyshire SW (2003) Structural and functional dichotomy of human midcingulate cortex. Eur J Neurosci 18:3134–3144.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Weidner C, Schmelz M, Schmidt R, Hansson B, Handwerker HO, Torebjörk HE (1999) Functional attributes discriminating mechano-insensitive and mechano-responsive C nociceptors in human skin. J Neurosci 19:10184–10190.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    Zambreanu L, Wise RG, Brooks JC, Iannetti GD, Tracey I (2005) A role for the brainstem in central sensitisation in humans. Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Pain 114:397–407.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 26 (20)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 26, Issue 20
17 May 2006
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Brain Activation during Input from Mechanoinsensitive versus Polymodal C-Nociceptors
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Brain Activation during Input from Mechanoinsensitive versus Polymodal C-Nociceptors
Belinda Susanne Ruehle, Hermann Otto Handwerker, Jochen Klaus Maria Lennerz, Ralf Ringler, Clemens Forster
Journal of Neuroscience 17 May 2006, 26 (20) 5492-5499; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2059-05.2006

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Brain Activation during Input from Mechanoinsensitive versus Polymodal C-Nociceptors
Belinda Susanne Ruehle, Hermann Otto Handwerker, Jochen Klaus Maria Lennerz, Ralf Ringler, Clemens Forster
Journal of Neuroscience 17 May 2006, 26 (20) 5492-5499; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2059-05.2006
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Memory Retrieval Has a Dynamic Influence on the Maintenance Mechanisms That Are Sensitive to ζ-Inhibitory Peptide (ZIP)
  • Neurophysiological Evidence for a Cortical Contribution to the Wakefulness-Related Drive to Breathe Explaining Hypocapnia-Resistant Ventilation in Humans
  • Monomeric Alpha-Synuclein Exerts a Physiological Role on Brain ATP Synthase
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Musical Expertise Induces Audiovisual Integration of Abstract Congruency Rules
  • The Laminar Development of Direction Selectivity in Ferret Visual Cortex
  • Individual Differences in Amygdala-Medial Prefrontal Anatomy Link Negative Affect, Impaired Social Functioning, and Polygenic Depression Risk
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.