Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

Mirror Neurons and the Lateralization of Human Language

Daniel R. Lametti and Andrew A. G. Mattar
Journal of Neuroscience 21 June 2006, 26 (25) 6666-6667; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1452-06.2006
Daniel R. Lametti
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada H3A 1B1
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Andrew A. G. Mattar
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada H3A 1B1
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Editor's Note: These short reviews of a recent paper in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to mimic the journal clubs that exist in your own departments or institutions. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

In the premotor cortex of the macaque there are neurons whose firing rates increase when the monkey makes goal directed movements and when the monkey observes similar movements being made by others. These cells are named “mirror neurons.” It has been suggested that by mapping observation of movements onto cells involved in the production of similar movements, mirror neurons help individuals understand and imitate motor goals (Gallese et al., 1996). Several studies have used functional imaging techniques to determine whether mirror neurons exist in humans. Neural activation in response to both movement production and observation has been described in Brodmann area 44 (BA44), located in the pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus (Iacoboni et al., 2005). In the left hemisphere, BA44 contains Broca's area, a brain region that has been the focus of intense study because of its role in language processing and speech production. The close anatomical proximity of mirror neurons to Broca's area has led some to speculate that mirror neurons may have played a role in the lateralization of human language. If true, the human mirror neuron system might also be left lateralized.

In their Journal of Neuroscience article, Aziz-Zadeh et al. (2006) used functional magnetic resonance imaging to determine the extent to which the human mirror neuron system is lateralized. Blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) neural activity was recorded in each of four experimental conditions. In each condition, subjects saw movies of left and right hands (each hand was presented independently) with the index finger extended between two targets. In the static observation condition, subjects observed a stationary hand. In the action observation condition, subjects watched as the index finger moved toward one of the targets. In the execution condition, the observed hand was stationary, but the subjects moved their own index finger when instructed by an abstract cue. In the imitation condition, the observed index finger moved toward one of the targets and subjects did the same [Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2006, their Fig. 1 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/11/2964/FIG1)]. Responses were made with the same hand as was observed. Images of left hands were presented to the left visual field so that sensory input was confined to the right hemisphere and vice versa for images of right hands. By controlling the hemisphere involved in visual processing and movement production, the authors were able to determine whether mirror neurons were lateralized.

The authors found that observation, execution, and imitation of finger movements resulted in reliable increases in BOLD activity within the pars opercularis. Neural activation increased linearly from observation to execution to imitation conditions, consistent with the properties of mirror neurons. Both left and right visual-field stimuli resulted in bilateral activation of mirror neurons [Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2006, their Fig. 2 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/11/2964/FIG2)]. Stronger activation was seen in mirror neurons ipsilateral to the stimuli: left visual-field stimuli led to stronger activations in the left hemisphere whereas right hemisphere mirror neurons responded to right visual-field stimuli. These findings suggest that cells in the pars opercularis of both the left and right hemisphere demonstrate mirror-neuron like properties and, hence, that mirror neuron activity is not lateralized.

The carefully controlled studies of Aziz-Zadeh et al. (2006) confirm previous findings identifying mirror neurons in the pars opercularis, but also report a novel finding that mirror neuron activation is not lateralized to the left hemisphere. The authors conclude that because mirror neurons in the pars opercularis show bilateral activation they were not precursors to the development of lateralized language function in humans. However, functional imaging studies that shed light on the current organization of the human brain do not necessarily provide insights into the origins of that organization.

The idea that mirror neurons play a role in human language is an intriguing one that to date is based only on the presence of mirror neurons in Broca's area. The close proximity of these two systems may be indicative of functional linkage or it may be a coincidence. Broca's area is classically known as a language area but it is also active during actions such as swallowing (Mosier et al., 1999). The functional role of mirror neurons in Broca's area is, thus, unclear.

Through the action of mirror neurons, subjects can learn new movements simply by observing others (Stefan et al., 2005). This suggests that mirror neurons help humans acquire novel patterns of movement control. In a study that used a robot to perturb the path of the jaw, the nervous system corrected altered patterns of movement when the perturbations were applied during speech (Tremblay et al., 2004). Thus, speaking involves specific motor goals that the nervous system works to achieve. Together, these results suggest that precise control of movements during speech can be learned through observation. If true, this may indicate that mirror neurons in Broca's area aid in the acquisition of novel movement patterns required for speech.

Footnotes

  • Review of Aziz-Zadeh et al. (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/11/2964)

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Andrew Mattar, McGill University, Department of Psychology, 1205 Dr. Penfield Avenue, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1B1. Email: andrew.mattar{at}mail.mcgill.ca

References

  1. ↵
    Aziz-Zadeh L, Koski L, Zaidel E, Mazziotta J, Iacoboni M (2006) Lateralization of the human mirror neuron system. J Neurosci 26:2964–2970.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119:593–609.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (2005) Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol 3:1–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Mosier KM, Liu WC, Maldjian JA, Shah R, Modi B (1999) Lateralization of cortical function in swallowing: a functional MR imaging study. Am J Neuroradiol 20:1520–1526.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    Stefan K, Cohen LG, Duque J, Mazzocchio R, Celnik P, Sawaki L, Ungerleider L, Classen J (2005) Formation of a motor memory by action observation. J Neurosci 25:9339–9346.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Tremblay S, Shiller DM, Ostry DJ (2004) The somatosensory basis of speech production. Nature 423:866–869.
    OpenUrl
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 26 (25)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 26, Issue 25
21 Jun 2006
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Mirror Neurons and the Lateralization of Human Language
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Mirror Neurons and the Lateralization of Human Language
Daniel R. Lametti, Andrew A. G. Mattar
Journal of Neuroscience 21 June 2006, 26 (25) 6666-6667; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1452-06.2006

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Mirror Neurons and the Lateralization of Human Language
Daniel R. Lametti, Andrew A. G. Mattar
Journal of Neuroscience 21 June 2006, 26 (25) 6666-6667; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1452-06.2006
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • The Oscillatory Nature of Movement Initiation
  • Adult-Born Granule Cells Contribute to Dentate Gyrus Circuit Reorganization after Traumatic Brain Injury
  • Lateral Preoptic Hypothalamus: A Window to Understanding Insomnia
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.