Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Featured ArticleArticles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions

Kong-Fatt Wong and Xiao-Jing Wang
Journal of Neuroscience 25 January 2006, 26 (4) 1314-1328; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3733-05.2006
Kong-Fatt Wong
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Xiao-Jing Wang
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Additional Files
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Reduction of a biophysical neuronal decision-making model. The original model (top) is endowed with strong recurrent excitation between neurons with similar stimulus selectivity, and effective inhibition between them via shared inhibition. NS and I denote the nonselective excitatory (black) and inhibitory (green) pools of cells, respectively. Arrows, Excitatory connections; circles, inhibitory connections. I1 and I2 are inputs from external stimulus to selective neural populations 1 (blue) and 2 (red). Brown arrows, Background noisy inputs. w+ denotes enhanced excitatory connections within each selective neural pool. The numbers on the right displays the total number of dynamical equations involved in the model. First step, Mean-field approach reduces 2000 spiking neurons into four neural units (with a total of 11 dynamical variables). Second step, Simplify the linear input–output relation (F–I curves) of the cells: (1) fit the input–output relation (F–I curve) of the spiking neuronal model with a simple function (Abbott and Chance, 2005); (2) linearize F–I curve for I cells; and (3) assume constant activity of NS cells. The final step involves the assumption that all fast variables of the system reach steady states earlier than that of NMDAR. The final reduced two-variable model (bottom) consists of two neural units, endowed with self-excitation and effective mutual inhibition.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Time course with two different motion strengths. Motion coherence of 0% (black traces) and 51.2% (light gray traces) each with 10 sample trials. Firing rates that ramp upward (bold traces) are for saccades made toward the RF of the neuron, whereas downward (dashed traces) are for saccades away from RF. Ramping is steeper for higher coherence level. The prescribed threshold is fixed at 15 Hz. Once the firing rate crosses the threshold, a decision is made, and the decision time is the time it takes from stimulus onset (0 ms) until the threshold is crossed. The reaction time is defined as the decision time plus a nondecision latency of 100 ms. The bold horizontal line at the top of the figure denotes the duration, at zero coherence, where the firing rates toward and away from RF are indistinguishable.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Performance and reaction time of models and the experiment of Roitman and Shadlen (2002). First column, Psychometric data from experiment and the models (data are fit with a Weibull function). Second column, Reaction time from experiment and the models. Open circles joined by dashed lines, Mean reaction of error trials; filled circles, correct trials. σnoise = 0.008 nA. Experimental data are adapted from Mazurek et al. (2003).

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Random choice with stimulus at zero coherence. A, Phase-plane without stimulus. Black circles, Stable steady states (attractors); gray circles, saddle-type unstable steady states. The green and orange lines are the nullclines for the synaptic dynamical variables S1 and S2. Using Equation 8, a threshold at 15 Hz would correspond to S = 0.49 in phase space. B, With an unbiased stimulus of 30 Hz, the two unstable steady states together with the low stable steady state disappear, and a new symmetric unstable steady state is formed. The black line with arrows toward (away) from the saddle point is the stable (unstable) manifold of this saddle point. The stable manifold is exactly the boundary between the two basins of attraction of the two choice attractors (when there is no noise). Superimposed are two typical single-trial trajectories (blue and red lines) of the state of the system from simulations. Color labeling is the same as in Figure 1. C, Schematic diagram of a generic saddle-like steady state (gray circle) and the local flows (arrows) around it. The lines directly toward (magenta) and away (brown) the steady state (gray) are its stable and unstable eigenvectors, respectively, with an exponential temporal dynamics determined by τstable (brown) and τunstable (magenta). D, A diagram of how a one-dimensional decision “landscape” changes with stimulus inputs in a single trial, illustrating decision computation and working memory by the same network. See Results for detailed description.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Basins of attraction with stimulus at nonzero coherence (c′ > 0%). A, Phase-plane without stimulus as in Figure 4A. B, The stable manifold is tilted away from the spontaneous state and toward the less favored attractor when c′ is nonzero (6.4%). As a result, at the onset of stimulus, the system starts in a resting state that has a higher chance of falling in the basin of attraction of the favored attractor state. The blue and red lines are typical single-trial trajectories for correct and error choices, respectively. C, Stronger bias between the basins of the two competing attractor states with a larger c′ (=51.2%). D, When c′ is sufficiently large, the saddle steady state annihilates with the less favored attractor, leaving only one choice attractor. c′ = 100%.

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Decision time and local dynamics in the vicinity of a saddle point. Zero coherence level from A to C. A, Longer reaction time for smaller stimulus strength μ0. Error bars indicate SD. B, Typical time courses: ramping is faster for larger stimulus strength, μ0. C, Time constants of saddle-like unstable steady state with different μ0. For μ0 > 17 Hz, τstable is larger than τunstable, whereas the opposite is true for μ0 < 17 Hz. D, Time constants of the unstable saddle as function of coherence level c′ (μ0 fixed at 30 Hz). The unstable time constant is essentially constant up to c′ ∼ 70%. The sudden increase in τunstable happens just before the bifurcation point at which the saddle coalesces with the less favored attractor and disappears (see Fig. 5).

  • Figure 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 7.

    Dependence of decision-making behavior on the AMPA:NMDA ratio at recurrent synapses. A, Typical time courses: faster ramping neural activity at larger AMPA:NMDA ratios. Top black (gray) horizontal bar denotes the duration where the firing rates are not distinguishable [i.e., the trajectory lies along the stable manifold of the saddle point, when AMPA:NMDA is 35:65 (0:100)]. B, Reaction time is shorter with a higher AMPA:NMDA ratio. C, The performance, however, becomes less accurate. Accuracy data are fitted by a Weibull function. D, For c′ = 0%, a higher AMPA:NMDA ratio decreases the reaction time for the entire range of stimulus strengths μ0. x-axis, Difference between μ0 and μ*0, which is the bifurcation point at which the saddle steady state appears and whose value depends on the AMPA:NMDA ratio. C and D have the same symbolic notations as in B. Error bars indicate SD.

  • Figure 8.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 8.

    Decision time with only AMPA at recurrent synapses (c′ = 0%). A, A sample time course with very fast dynamics (t = 0 is stimulus onset). μ0 = μ*0 + 15 Hz, where μ*0 is the value of μ0 when the saddle point is just created. Bottom horizontal bar denotes the duration where the firing rates are indistinguishable; trajectory lies near the stable manifold of the saddle point. B, Reaction time as a function of μ0 − μ*0. Note that reaction times longer than 200 ms can hardly be realized even with parameter fine-tuning. Error bars indicate SD.

  • Figure 9.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 9.

    Neural firing activity in delayed-response task in which a motion stimulus presentation is followed by a memory period. A, Sample time courses within a trial with different coherence levels. Black and dashed lines are for saccades moving toward and away from the response field of the neuron, respectively. The coherence level is shown at the top of each panel. Shaded regions, Motion viewing period. The black horizontal line at the bottom indicates the time epoch when the two firing rates are indistinguishable. B, Dependence of neural activity on motion strength in different epochs. Opened and filled circles, Saccades toward and away from the response field of the neuron. The largest dependence on the motion strength, as well as the greatest difference in the two neural responses, correspond to the late phase (0.5–1 s epoch) of stimulus presentation. In the third epoch (early delay period), there is a still a residual effect of the dependence of neural response on motion strength. Figures are calculated using correct trials only and averaged over 2000 trials.

  • Figure 10.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 10.

    Bifurcation diagram of a selective population with stimulus strength μ0 as parameter (c′ = 0%). Bold lines, Stable steady states; dashed lines, saddle steady states. Spontaneous state before stimulus presentation is denoted by the filled square. With a μ0 = 30 Hz stimulus, the spontaneous stable state loses stability, and a saddle steady state appears (open square). The state either goes toward the upper or lower stable state (filled circles). The population wins the competition if the upper branch is chosen, and loses otherwise. When stimulus is removed, hysteresis of the upper stable branch allows the activity to persist (memory storage of a decision choice). Arrow with an asterisk, Point where spontaneous state loses stability. Arrow with double asterisks, Saddle point turns into an attractor.

  • Figure 11.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 11.

    Dependence of integration time on the relative strength of recurrent excitation. A, Sample time courses: faster ramping activity with stronger recurrent strengths, w+. B, The unstable time constant of a saddle point dominates the dynamics when recurrent strength w+ is weak. C, Reaction time decreases with increasing w+. Error bars indicate SD. D, Accuracy of performance decreases with increasing w+. Data are fit with a Weibull function.

  • Figure 12.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 12.

    Distinct modes of operation in the two parameter space with zero coherence. In general, there are three types of regions. Bistable (red) region, A symmetric and two asymmetric attractors coexist; blue competition region, one saddle with two asymmetric competing attractors; monostable region, only one attractor. Depending on the strength of recurrent excitation w+, the network responds to a stimulus (of suitable intensity μ0) in four different ways, shown as regimes I, II, III, IV in insets. Regime I and II do not support working memory (of decision). Regime I, No decision making nor memory. Regime II, The network can produce a binary decision during stimulation but cannot store it in working memory. Regime III, The network is capable of both decision-making computation and working memory (our standard parameter set). Regime IV, For any μ0, there is always a stable symmetric stable state. Dark and dashed branches denote loci of stable and unstable steady states, respectively. A and AS are labels for branches with symmetric and asymmetric steady states, respectively.

  • Figure 13.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 13.

    Decision making without short-term memory in a network with low recurrent strength (w+ = 1.59). A, A typical trial showing slow ramping up activity for both neural populations. Note that the two firing rates are indistinguishable for many hundreds of milliseconds (indicated by the black horizontal bar), before they separate by a modest difference. Stimulus is applied from time 0–2 s (gray region). B, Phase-plane without stimulus has only one low stable attractor. C, The response of the system to a stimulus with zero coherence, in two trials, plotted as trajectories in the decision space (red and blue). Stimulus intensity is μ0 = 45 Hz lasting for 2 s. D, Comparison between stable and unstable time constants of the saddle-type unstable steady state (μ0 = 45 Hz).

Additional Files

  • Figures
  • Supplemental data

    Files in this Data Supplement:

    • supplemental material - Supplemental material
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 26 (4)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 26, Issue 4
25 Jan 2006
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions
Kong-Fatt Wong, Xiao-Jing Wang
Journal of Neuroscience 25 January 2006, 26 (4) 1314-1328; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3733-05.2006

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions
Kong-Fatt Wong, Xiao-Jing Wang
Journal of Neuroscience 25 January 2006, 26 (4) 1314-1328; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3733-05.2006
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Appendix
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Memory Retrieval Has a Dynamic Influence on the Maintenance Mechanisms That Are Sensitive to ζ-Inhibitory Peptide (ZIP)
  • Neurophysiological Evidence for a Cortical Contribution to the Wakefulness-Related Drive to Breathe Explaining Hypocapnia-Resistant Ventilation in Humans
  • Monomeric Alpha-Synuclein Exerts a Physiological Role on Brain ATP Synthase
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Influence of Reward on Corticospinal Excitability during Movement Preparation
  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.