Abstract
Large-scale changes in luminance are known to exert a significant suppressive or masking effect on visual perception, but the neural substrate for this effect remains unclear. In this report, we describe the results of experiments using in vivo intracellular recording to explore the impact of luminance transients on the responses of orientation-selective neurons in layer 2/3 of tree shrew primary visual cortex. By measuring changes in excitatory and inhibitory conductances, we find that instantaneous changes in luminance evoke strong cortical inhibition. When combined with visual stimuli that would otherwise yield strong excitatory responses, luminance transients produce significant reductions in excitation as well as increases in inhibition. As a result, luminance transients significantly delay the emergence of orientation tuned cortical responses, and virtually eliminate ongoing responses to effective stimuli. We conclude that cortical inhibition is a critical factor in luminance-evoked cortical suppression and the likely substrate for luminance-induced visual masking phenomenon.
Introduction
During experience with natural scenes, the visual system is subject to rapid, large-scale changes in luminance imposed by alterations in observer position or gaze angle as well as changes in the environment itself (movement of clouds, shadows of moving objects, etc.). Such spatially uniform changes convey relatively little information about the details of the visual scene; nevertheless, when present, they exert a powerful influence on the observer's ability to perceive the features signaled by local image contrast. For example, a sudden change in mean luminance can mask the appearance of visual targets presented slightly before, at the same time, or just after the luminance transient (Crawford, 1947; Macknik and Livingstone, 1998; Breitmeyer and Ogmen, 2000; Ogmen et al., 2003). Moreover, performance on visual discrimination tasks is substantially reduced by concurrent changes in luminance (Huang et al., 2005).
The suppressive impact of luminance transients on perception accords with the impact of large-scale changes in luminance on the responses of neurons in primary visual cortex (Kayama et al., 1979; Komatsu et al., 1996; Kammer et al., 1999; Kinoshita and Komatsu, 2001; Haynes et al., 2004; Peng and Van Essen, 2005). Increments or decrements in luminance are accompanied by short latency changes in cortical activity including a prominent transient (tens to hundreds of milliseconds) suppression of neural response whose duration is consistent with the perceptual effects of luminance transients (Bridgeman, 1975; Macknik and Livingstone, 1998; Gawne and Martin, 2002; Huang and Paradiso, 2005). However, the neural substrate that mediates luminance-evoked suppression of cortical response remains unresolved. Lateral geniculate neurons generate transient responses to changes in luminance (Schiller, 1968; Sherman, 2001; Martinez-Conde et al., 2002; Denning and Reinagel, 2005), raising the possibility that cortical response suppression arises from a reduction in the activity of excitatory feedforward inputs. However, evidence that visual masking phenomena exhibit interocular transfer (a masking stimulus presented to one eye can mask the appearance of a stimulus presented to the other eye) suggests that luminance-evoked suppression may depend on intracortical circuits, perhaps reflecting the preferential recruitment of cortical inhibitory neurons (Kolers and Rosner, 1960; Schiller, 1965; Macknik and Martinez-Conde, 2004).
Here, we describe the results of experiments using in vivo intracellular recordings to explore the effect of spatially uniform luminance transients on the responses of neurons in tree shrew visual cortex. Our goal was to characterize the subthreshold response of cortical neurons to luminance change, and to determine how these uniform changes in luminance affect the response to structured visual stimuli, in particular, the response to gratings. For this reason, we focused our attention on neurons in cortical layer 2/3 that are well tuned to stimulus orientation, a tuning that arises via inputs from non-orientation-selective layer 4 neurons (Chisum et al., 2003; Mooser et al., 2004). Our results demonstrate that changes in luminance evoke strong cortical inhibition, which delays the emergence of orientation tuned responses in layer 2/3 neurons and eliminates ongoing responses to effective stimuli. We conclude that cortically derived inhibition is a critical factor in luminance-evoked cortical suppression and the likely basis for luminance-induced perceptual masking.
Materials and Methods
Tree shrew preparation.
Surgical preparations for tree shrews (Tupaia belangeri) have been described previously (Bosking et al., 1997). Briefly, adult tree shrews (4–12 months) were initially anesthetized before surgery with ketamine (200 mg/kg) and xylazine (4.7 mg/kg) by intramuscular injection and a 2:1 mixture of N2O/O2 supplemented with 1.5% halothane, delivered through a tracheostomy by artificial respiration. Procedures for intracellular recording included creating a small craniotomy (0.5–1.0 mm diameter) overlying V1, retracting dura, and performing a bilateral pneumothorax to reduce pulsations attributable to respiration. All wound margins, incisions, and pressure points were treated with lidocaine. Before recording, halothane levels were reduced to 0.75%, N2O/O2 mixture set to 1:1, and the animal was paralyzed with pancuronium bromide administered through an intraperitoneal cannula. The animal was secured in a modified stereotaxic frame, and the eyes were protected by planar contact lenses. Temperature was maintained at 38°C, expired CO2 at 3.5%, and the electrocardiogram was monitored throughout the experiment. All experimental procedures were approved by the Duke University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and were done in compliance with guidelines published by the National Institutes of Health.
Visual stimuli.
Visual stimuli were generated by programs written in Matlab with Psychophysics Toolbox extensions (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) on a Macintosh G4 computer, and presented at 120 Hz on a calibrated Sony (Tokyo, Japan) GDM-C520 cathode ray tube monitor, having a luminance range of 0.3–120 cd/m2. Orientation tuning curves were determined by randomly presenting equiluminant full-field static (nondrifting) square-wave gratings at 18 different orientations between 0 and 180° and fitting subthreshold responses with a Gaussian curve [y = y0 + (A/(w sqrt(π/2))) exp(−2(x − xc)2/w2)], where y0 is the baseline offset, xc is the center of the peak, A is the total area under the curve from the baseline, and w is the width (2σ). Visual stimuli were presented for 1 s at interstimulus intervals of 1 s, synchronized to the vertical refresh signal of the monitor, allowing repeated trials to be aligned with submillisecond accuracy.
Because luminance-evoked responses are generated by transitions between two visual stimuli differing in mean luminance, we used two methods to create visual stimuli containing various combinations of grating contrast and luminance. In the first method, we varied the luminance of the first stimulus (the preceding uniform field) while the second stimulus remained constant. For example, a high contrast grating (100% contrast; black bars, 0.3 cd/m2; white bars, 120 cd/m2; overall mean luminance, 60 cd/m2) preceded by a black uniform field yielded a transition with increasing luminance (e.g., see Fig. 4C), whereas the same grating preceded by a gray uniform field (60 cd/m2) gave an equiluminant transition (e.g., see Fig. 4B). In the second method, the first stimulus remained constant as we varied the luminance of the second stimulus. For example, low contrast (10%) gratings with alternating light gray (66 cd/m2) and dark gray (54 cd/m2) bars were made equiluminant to a preceding uniform gray field (see Fig. 7A). A grating with the same contrast (i.e., difference between “light” and “dark” bars was equal to 12 cd/m2) was produced with alternating white (120 cd/m2) and light gray (108 cd/m2) bars (see Fig. 7C), which gave an increasing luminance transition relative to the preceding flat gray field (from 60 to 114 cd/m2). The first method was necessary to investigate the effect of luminance on high contrast gratings, having black and white bars that used the full luminance range of the monitor (i.e., luminance step gratings). The second method was needed to present isolated transitions in contrast and luminance (i.e., delayed luminance step gratings) (see Figs. 5A, 7E). Because the majority of neurons in layer 2/3 of tree shrew have complex receptive fields (our unpublished data), after determining the preferred orientation of the neuron, we selected a single grating phase that generated a vigorous response, and used the same phase for all subsequent measurements.
Intracellular recordings.
Intracellular recordings followed standard methods. Sharp electrodes (80–130 MΩ) were fabricated using borosilicate capillary glass tubing (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL) and an electrode puller (Sutter Instrument, Novato, CA). For standard (nonconductance) recordings, electrodes were filled with 2 m potassium acetate. For conductance recordings, electrodes were filled with 2 m cesium acetate and 50 mm lidocaine N-ethyl bromide (QX-314) (Calbiochem, La Jolla, CA) to block intrinsic sodium and potassium conductances, permitting recordings to be obtained during large positive current injections, uncontaminated by action potentials and other voltage-gated conductances. The effect of these ion-channel blockers was typically evident several minutes after impalement, because action potentials initially broadened and then disappeared. In a few cases (e.g., see Figs. 4Fii, 5Aii), data used in conductance calculations were obtained before blockade was complete, and where necessary, action potentials were clipped and filtered before analysis.
Electrodes were stabilized by applying 4% agar to the craniotomy, reducing brain pulsations caused by heartbeat and respiration, and advanced by hydraulic and mechanical micromanipulators (Soma Scientific, Irvine, CA). Recordings were targeted to neurons in superficial layers of cortex, having estimated depths of 0.2–0.8 mm below the pial surface. Intracellular recordings were obtained in current-clamp (bridge) mode on an Axoclamp-1D (Molecular Devices, Union City, CA), filtered at 3 kHz, and digitized at 10 kHz with a CED Power 1401 (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) connected to a personal computer running Spike2 software. After obtaining a stable recording, small current steps were delivered to facilitate bridge balance and capacitance neutralization, and these settings were rechecked every few minutes to verify stability of recordings, which were commonly maintained for 30–120 min. The time constants of electrodes were typically an order of magnitude faster than that of cells, allowing precise compensation of electrode resistance both before and after impalement. Neurons selected for analysis had stable resting potentials below −65 mV, action potentials >50 mV above threshold, and input resistances >50 MΩ. All were identified as regular-spiking neurons, exhibiting spike-frequency adaptation, lack of intrinsic bursts, and broad action potentials (full-width at half-maximum, >0.6 ms).
Conductance calculations.
Synaptic conductances were determined by a previously published method (Hirsch et al., 1998; Anderson et al., 2000). Recordings were obtained at three to five different levels of current injection between −0.4 and +0.8 nA, maintaining the membrane potential at various levels between −120 and +20 mV, and up to 20 trials were acquired at each setting.
The resting input conductance (Grest) of the neuron was determined by the following equation: V = V0 + Iinj/Grest, where V is the membrane potential, Iinj is the injected current, and V0 is the resting membrane potential in the absence of current injection. Similarly, the total conductance of the neuron, Gtotal(t), was determined for the entire time course of response by the formula V(t) = Vvisual(t) + Iinj/Gtotal(t), where V(t) is the time-varying stimulus-evoked membrane potential response at the associated level of current injection, and Vvisual(t) is the excursion in membrane potential for the stimulus-evoked synaptic response in the absence of injected current (Iinj = 0). To determine how excitatory and inhibitory conductances [Gexc(t) and Ginh(t)] contribute to the total conductance, we assume that the total conductance can be attributed to three independent factors according to the following formula: The impact that each of these conductances exerts on the membrane potential depends on its reversal potential and the total conductance according to the following formula: Although inhibitory currents are normally carried by both chloride and potassium, in our experiments potassium channels are blocked by intracellular cesium, so we assume that all inhibitory currents are carried by chloride through GABAergic channels with a reversal potential of −75 mV (Vi), and all excitatory currents are carried by glutamate-mediated nonselective cation channels having a reversal potential of 0 mV (Ve). Equations 1 and 2 provide two equations with two unknowns (Gexc and Ginh) which are solved at each point in time by simple linear methods using custom Matlab programs. All values are reported as the mean ± 1 SD.
Results
Subthreshold response of cortical neurons to large-scale changes in luminance
Our initial step in investigating the effect of changes in luminance on cortical neuronal circuits was to characterize the subthreshold responses of layer 2/3 neurons to full screen (73 × 55°, width by height) changes in luminance. Changes in luminance evoked multiphasic responses, typically consisting of a weak, short latency depolarization followed by a strong hyperpolarization lasting up to several hundred milliseconds (Fig. 1). For approximately one-half of the sample (n = 18), equivalent responses were found for luminance increments and decrements (Fig. 1A,B). For the remainder of the population, responses to increments and decrements were distinct, either because the shape of the multiphasic response was substantially different (Fig. 1C), or because response to one sign of luminance change was hyperpolarizing, whereas the response to the other was depolarizing (Fig. 1D) (n = 15). Despite these differences, a strong hyperpolarizing response to luminance steps was present in all of the cells in our sample.
Both the amplitude and duration of the hyperpolarization were correlated with the magnitude of the luminance step: larger luminance steps (either increments or decrements) produced larger and longer hyperpolarizing responses (Fig. 1A–D). To examine this relationship across the population, we normalized the negative peak of each response to the largest hyperpolarization within the series of luminance steps for each cell (Fig. 1E). The resulting curve shows that the amplitude of the hyperpolarizing responses grows progressively with the magnitude of luminance change. The onset latency of the luminance response varied with the magnitude of the luminance step (Fig. 1F), occurring 16 ± 8 ms earlier for the large versus small steps (100% luminance step, 23 ± 4 ms; 10%, 39 ± 13 ms). Although the initial brief depolarization was a common feature of the luminance-evoked response, the cell-to-cell variability in its amplitude made it difficult to draw strong conclusions regarding its relationship to the magnitude of the luminance change.
Luminance-evoked hyperpolarization reflects an increase in inhibition
To determine the contribution of various sources of synaptic excitation and inhibition to luminance-driven cortical responses, we used electrodes filled with a solution containing QX-314 and cesium to block both voltage-gated sodium and potassium channels, and varied the membrane potential between −120 and +20 mV by injecting current through the electrode, altering the driving forces on synaptic currents. At rest, luminance-evoked changes in membrane potential are dominated by excitatory synaptic events, because the membrane potential is far from the reversal potential for cationic currents gated by glutamate receptors and near the reversal potentials for chloride- and potassium-mediated inhibition. Conversely, when the cell is depolarized near zero millivolts, luminance-evoked changes in membrane potential are dominated by inhibitory synaptic events.
Luminance-evoked hyperpolarizations were observed to be relatively small near the resting membrane potential, but their magnitude grew substantially larger as the membrane potential was progressively depolarized toward zero millivolts, yielding amplitudes commonly fivefold greater than those at rest (Fig. 2A). Linear fits to response amplitudes from each cell showed average reversal potentials of −85 ± 6 mV (n = 11) (Fig. 2B), a value consistent with the reversal potentials for inhibitory synaptic currents. This suggests that the hyperpolarizing responses evoked by changes in luminance are dominated by increases in inhibition with little or no contribution of changes in excitation.
From these data, we were able to obtain a more quantitative assessment of the synaptic components of luminance-driven responses by calculating the relative changes in excitatory and inhibitory conductances (Fig. 2C). Conductance calculations revealed that luminance changes cause a large increase in the total conductance of the cell, which is almost entirely attributable to increases in inhibition. After changes in luminance, the excitatory conductance was either unchanged or slightly diminished throughout the response, although in some cases there was a small, brief increase in excitatory conductance that occurred very early in the response at the same point in time as the short latency depolarization, mentioned previously. During the negative-most aspect of the hyperpolarization, inhibitory conductance, ΔGinh, increased 33 ± 10% and excitatory conductance, ΔGexc, decreased −5 ± 4%, relative to the total conductance of the neuron, Gtotal, at rest (n = 11) (see Fig. 5B).
Luminance transitions evoke a transient hyperpolarization that is not found with equiluminant transitions
The strong net inhibitory response of layer 2/3 neurons to a change in luminance was not observed when stimulus transitions were equated for average luminance. This point is illustrated in Figure 3, where the response of individual cortical neurons to a luminance step is compared with their response to a square wave grating of the preferred orientation, whose mean luminance was equivalent to that of the preceding uniform field stimulus (equiluminant transition; mean luminance of both grating and preceding uniform field, 50% gray). As described above, luminance steps consistently evoked strong transient hyperpolarizing responses (Fig. 3A), but this was never observed for equiluminant transitions, regardless of the contrast of the grating (n = 15) (Fig. 3B). Instead, equiluminant gratings of the preferred orientation always evoked net depolarizing responses whose magnitude and rise time varied systematically with contrast.
Conductance calculations for high contrast equiluminant transitions revealed rapid, simultaneous increases in both inhibitory and excitatory conductances (Fig. 3C), consistent with observations from intracellular recordings in visual cortex of the cat (Ferster, 1986; Hirsch et al., 1998; Anderson et al., 2000; Monier et al., 2003). To compare the results across the population of cells, conductances were quantified by measuring their value at the peak of the depolarization, where both excitatory and inhibitory conductances increased by about one-third (ΔGexc, 34 ± 10%; ΔGinh, 37 ± 4%; n = 10) over the initial level of the resting total conductance, Gtotal (see Fig. 5C). Thus, the major difference in synaptic inputs between equiluminant and luminance step transitions lies in excitatory conductance changes: both stimulus conditions yield an increase in inhibitory conductances, but only equiluminant conditions result in significant increases in excitatory conductances (compare with Fig. 2C).
Luminance transients veto the response of layer 2/3 neurons by evoking strong reductions in excitatory inputs as well as increases in inhibition
What accounts for the failure of luminance transitions to evoke increases in excitatory conductances like that seen with equiluminant transitions? A parsimonious explanation would be that luminance increments evoke inhibition in layer 2/3 neurons and in the cortical neurons that supply their excitatory inputs. Indeed, the weak depolarizing response to luminance change that frequently precedes the strong hyperpolarizing response in layer 2/3 neurons is consistent with luminance evoked inhibition of cortical excitation. However, stronger support for this interpretation comes from an analysis of the impact of luminance change on the response of layer 2/3 neurons to a stimulus that, presented in isolation, produces a robust increase in excitatory drive.
Figure 4 compares in the same neuron, responses generated by luminance steps, equiluminant gratings, and luminance step gratings, a high contrast grating of the preferred orientation preceded by a uniform field stimulus having a substantially different mean luminance (higher or lower) than the grating. When a luminance step was combined with the presentation of a grating stimulus, the initial response was remarkably similar in appearance to that produced by presentation of an equivalent luminance step alone (Fig. 4, compare A, C). The small depolarization and subsequent hyperpolarization that were prominent features of the response to isolated luminance steps were clearly evident in the initial response to luminance step gratings. As a result, the first spike produced by the luminance step grating was significantly delayed relative to that evoked by equiluminant gratings (average delay, 75 ± 20 ms; equiluminant grating, 47 ± 6 ms; luminance step grating, 122 ± 59 ms; p < 0.01, paired t test; n = 7). Consistent with this result, the peak depolarization evoked by luminance step gratings was also significantly delayed, relative to those evoked by both high and low contrast equiluminant gratings, and preceded by a prominent hyperpolarization (Fig. 4D,E) (luminance step grating, 117 ± 15 ms; high contrast equiluminant grating, 56 ± 11 ms; p < 0.001; low contrast equiluminant grating, 94 ± 23 ms; p < 0.02, independent t test; n = 15; peak measured at 90% of maximal amplitude).
The depolarizations evoked by luminance step gratings frequently exhibited different peak amplitudes than those evoked by equiluminant gratings. In some cases, the peak response to the luminance step grating was greater than that of the equiluminant grating (Fig. 4D), whereas in other cases it was less (see Fig. 7A,C). These differences were correlated with the response of the cell to pure luminance change: luminance step gratings evoked diminished responses in cells having solely hyperpolarizing responses to changes in pure luminance (Fig. 1B), but they generated augmented responses in cells exhibiting secondary (rebound) depolarization (Fig. 1A,C,D). Averaging across the cells in our sample, the difference in peak depolarization to equiluminant and luminance step conditions was not significant (luminance step, 9 ± 4 mV; equiluminant, 14 ± 6 mV; p < 0.13, independent t test; n = 15).
These results indicate that luminance transitions exert a powerful suppressive effect on the excitatory drive to layer 2/3 neurons, an effect that is confirmed by conductance calculations (Fig. 4F). The total change in conductance induced by the luminance step grating consists of two stages that are distinguished by different contributions of excitatory and inhibitory conductances. The initial increase in total conductance that follows the presentation of a luminance step grating is almost entirely attributable to an increase in inhibitory conductance. This is followed by a second increase in total conductance that involves an increase in both excitatory and inhibitory conductance. Thus, in terms of the relative contribution of excitatory and inhibitory conductance changes, the initial response to the luminance step gratings resembles the response to isolated changes in luminance, and lacks the strong increase in excitatory conductance that is found with equiluminant grating transitions (Fig. 4G). Note also that the initial change in inhibitory conductance evoked by the luminance step grating is less than that evoked by an equiluminant grating. Thus, luminance steps appear to veto the full complement of synaptic inputs that are conveyed to layer 2/3 neurons after the presentation of an equiluminant grating, perhaps as a consequence of blocking excitatory drive at an early site in the cortical circuit. In the three cells for which conductance calculations could be made, inhibitory conductance at the initial peak increased 17 ± 6% without a change in excitatory conductance (ΔGexc = 3 ± 2%) relative to the resting total conductance (Fig. 5D).
Changes in luminance actively suppress ongoing grating-evoked cortical excitation
Although the onset of the luminance step and the grating were simultaneous, the response to this stimulus appears as two distinct components, an early component that has the conductance profile associated with isolated changes in luminance and a later component that exhibits the properties of an equiluminant grating response. This raises the possibility that the strong impact of the luminance step on the cortical response to a grating stimulus may depend at least in part on the fact that luminance-driven inhibition of cortical circuits arrives early, before the high levels of synaptic activity generated by a grating stimulus can be recruited. If privileged temporal access to cortex is a major factor in the luminance veto of the grating response, then the impact of luminance-evoked suppression should be significantly reduced by delaying the onset of the luminance step, allowing cortical circuits time to build a strong grating-evoked response.
To test this possibility, we delayed the presentation of luminance steps such that they would occur at the peak of grating-evoked responses (delayed luminance step grating stimuli). Namely, equiluminant gratings were preceded by a uniform gray field, and ∼100 ms later, near the peak depolarization, the visual stimulus was changed to a grating with the same orientation and contrast but having a higher (or lower) mean luminance. In these recordings, after the transition to the luminance step grating, the ongoing grating-evoked depolarization was abolished for tens to hundreds of milliseconds as the membrane potential transiently hyperpolarized beneath the resting potential, creating a prominent notch (Fig. 5A). Conductance calculations for delayed luminance step gratings revealed that, after the luminance transition, inhibitory conductance rose sharply by 33 ± 18% and excitatory conductance fell abruptly by −14 ± 10% relative to the total conductance (n = 3) (Fig. 5E). These results emphasize that changes in luminance exert a powerful veto on the response of cortical circuits regardless of their ongoing level of activity, and do so via complementary changes in excitatory and inhibitory conductances.
Lateral geniculate neurons generate sustained responses to changes in luminance
The complementary changes in inhibition and excitation recorded in individual layer 2/3 neurons are consistent with a mechanism in which luminance steps drive increases in the activity of cortical inhibitory neurons that, in turn, silence the activity of cortical excitatory neurons. Such a mechanism requires that luminance steps evoke an increase in the activity of thalamic inputs supplying excitatory drive to cortical inhibitory neurons that is of sufficient duration to account for the time course of cortical suppression. To determine whether large-scale luminance steps are accompanied by strong and sustained increases in the activity of lateral geniculate (LGN) neurons, extracellular recordings were used to evaluate the responses of on- and off-center neurons in the LGN to sign-appropriate changes in luminance. In response to full-field changes in luminance, geniculate neurons generated robust responses lasting up to several seconds (n = 4 on-center; n = 2 off-center). The initial response was a high frequency burst followed by sustained spiking that decayed slowly to the spontaneous firing rate (Fig. 6A,B). The transient and sustained components were quantified in the normalized and averaged population response, which was best fit by a double exponential function having fast (τ1 = 65 ms) and slow (τ2 = 3.6 s) time constants (n = 6) (Fig. 6C,D). These results indicate that LGN neurons maintain strong excitatory drive to cortical circuits throughout the time course of the luminance-evoked suppression of cortical response.
The impact of luminance steps on orientation tuned responses
The previous results show that changes in luminance suppress responses driven by gratings at the preferred orientation (Figs. 4, 5), but they do not address how luminance steps impact the operation of cortical circuits that are responsible for generating orientation-selective responses. Here, we quantify the impact of luminance steps on orientation tuning functions by presenting a full range of orientations (0–180°) for both equiluminant, luminance step, and delayed luminance step gratings.
After equiluminant grating presentations, orientation selectivity was evident from response onset, because each grating orientation evoked a distinct trajectory of depolarization, causing responses to diverge from their earliest time points (Fig. 7A,B). In contrast, the initial responses to luminance step gratings were virtually identical for all orientations, yielding overlapping multiphasic waveforms during the first several tens of milliseconds of response (n = 7) (Fig. 7C,D). To quantify the initial differences in the orientation tuning for equiluminant and luminance-evoked responses in our sample population, we selected the time of the negative peak of the hyperpolarization (79 ± 12 ms) to fit responses to Gaussian functions and quantify parameters of orientation tuning (Fig. 7G). At this time, responses to equiluminant gratings were well tuned (width, 48 ± 10°; offset, 0.4 ± 0.1 units; area, 24 ± 6 unit-degrees; n = 7), but those evoked by luminance step gratings were virtually flat, exhibiting no significant orientation preference and an offset below baseline attributable to the hyperpolarization (offset, −0.08 ± 0.04 units). In some cases (four of seven cells), the response to the preferred orientation was slightly greater than that evoked by other orientations during the luminance-evoked hyperpolarization (Fig. 7C); however, this trend was not consistent across the population.
Although orientation tuning was virtually absent throughout the luminance-evoked hyperpolarization, it developed rapidly as responses depolarized, eventually becoming comparable with that evoked by equiluminant gratings. At the time of the peak depolarization (t = 122 ± 25 ms), responses evoked by luminance step gratings were well tuned for orientation (width, 51 ± 12°; area, 23 ± 7 unit-degrees; offset, 0.34 ± 0.04 units), values that were similar to those for equiluminant responses at their peak (t = 64 ± 22 ms; width, 45 ± 5°; area, 27 ± 4 unit-degrees; offset, 0.4 ± 0.03 units). Thus, despite potent suppression evoked by changes in luminance, orientation-selective responses recover completely within 43 ± 15 ms after the negative peak of the hyperpolarization.
The rapid recovery of orientation tuning after luminance steps was also evident in the response to the delayed luminance step stimulus (Fig. 7E,F). At the negative peak of the hyperpolarization after the onset of the luminance step, responses to all orientations were grouped closely together and differed by only a few millivolts, indicating a profound impact on orientation tuning. At the negative peak of the hyperpolarization, the preferred orientation of the Gaussian fit was relatively unchanged from that of equiluminant responses, but the area was markedly reduced, the tuning width was substantially narrower, and the offset was negative because of the hyperpolarization (Fig. 7H) (delayed luminance step: width, 20 ± 8°; area, 4 ± 2 unit-degrees; offset, −0.01 ± 0.01 units; equiluminant: width, 45 ± 5°, area, 27 ± 4 unit-degrees; offset, 0.4 ± 0.03 units). From the negative hyperpolarizing peak, responses reversed course and depolarized rapidly to a second peak 32 ± 12 ms later. At this time, orientation tuning was mostly restored and virtually identical to the equiluminant values at the same time point (width, 45 ± 8°; offset, 0.44 ± 0.03 units; area, 21 ± 4 unit-degrees).
Discussion
These results demonstrate that large-scale changes in luminance evoke brief but powerful suppression that dominates layer 2/3 neuron response to concurrent visual stimulation. In the sections below, we consider the evidence that this response derives from the activation of cortical inhibitory neurons, speculate on the circuitry that mediates this response, and relate these observations to visual masking and related psychophysical phenomenon.
Luminance-evoked suppression is a product of cortically derived inhibition
The suppressive effects of luminance change reported here are consistent with previous studies using extracellular recordings in which it was shown that changes in luminance, although generally ineffective in driving cortical response, could reduce the discharge rate of cortical neurons to effective stimuli (Bridgeman, 1975; Kayama et al., 1979; Kretz et al., 1986; Macknik and Livingstone, 1998; Gawne and Martin, 2000, 2002; Huang and Paradiso, 2005; Peng and Van Essen, 2005). Although extracellular recordings provide a clear picture of the impact of luminance on spike discharge rate, they provide little insight into the excitatory and inhibitory events that mediate this suppression. By using intracellular recordings in layer 2/3, we were able to explore the response of cortical neurons to luminance change in the absence of additional stimuli, and these results demonstrate strong increases in total conductance that are almost entirely accounted for by increases in inhibitory conductance. Moreover, because inputs from the LGN are strictly excitatory, this observation rules out the possibility that luminance-evoked suppression is a subcortical phenomena (i.e., the luminance signal must be conveyed to cortical circuits as an increase in LGN activity to drive inhibitory cortical neurons).
Indeed, extracellular recordings in the LGN revealed that changes in luminance generate robust responses that persist throughout the time course of hyperpolarization in layer 2/3 (Fig. 6). These data are consistent with previous results showing that stimulation by diffuse light generates long-lasting responses in the LGN (Hubel, 1960; Hubel and Wiesel, 1961; Jacobs and Yolton, 1970; Mukhametov and Rizzolatti, 1970; Marrocco, 1972; Kayama et al., 1979; Valberg et al., 1985), sometimes preceded by transient, high frequency bursts (Sherman, 2001). Although changes in luminance generally produce weaker responses in the LGN than those evoked by gratings or stimulation of the receptive field center alone (Hubel and Wiesel, 1961), LGN activity does not decline below spontaneous levels after luminance steps, indicating that luminance-evoked cortical hyperpolarization is not attributable to a transient removal of feedforward excitation.
Luminance-evoked suppression in layer 2/3 neurons involves both increased inhibition and decreased excitation: circuit implications
Although inhibition is the predominant response of layer 2/3 neurons to luminance steps in isolation, when combined with stimuli that would otherwise yield strong excitatory drive, luminance steps evoke significant changes in both excitation and inhibition. This can be appreciated by comparing the response to equiluminant gratings, which evoked large increases in excitation (Fig. 3C), and the response to luminance step gratings, which did not (Fig. 4F). An even more dramatic illustration is the sharp reduction in excitatory conductance produced by a luminance step presented during the ongoing response to an effective stimulus (Fig. 5A). Under these conditions, the powerful suppressive effect of luminance change is mediated by complementary changes in both excitatory and inhibitory conductances, presumably the result of an increase in the activity of inhibitory neurons as well as a decrease in the activity of excitatory neurons that synapse with layer 2/3 neurons.
Given that a major source of excitatory drive to layer 2/3 neurons arises from spiny stellate neurons in cortical layer 4 (Lund, 1988; Muly and Fitzpatrick, 1992; Fitzpatrick, 1996; Callaway, 1998; Lund et al., 2003; Douglas and Martin, 2004; Mooser et al., 2004), a parsimonious explanation for these effects is that luminance steps evoke inhibition in layer 2/3 neurons and in the spiny stellate neurons of layer 4 that supply feedforward excitatory inputs. Moreover, the fact that increases in the inhibitory conductance of layer 2/3 neurons occur without increases (or during decreases) in excitatory conductance suggests that the synapses providing luminance-evoked inhibition onto layer 2/3 neurons arise from layer 4 neurons that receive direct LGN input. Thus, the impact of luminance change on the responses of layer 2/3 neurons could be explained by the activation of layer 4 neurons that supply feedforward inhibition to layer 2/3 neurons and to the layer 4 neurons that supply layer 2/3 neurons with their excitatory drive. This hypothesis is consistent with evidence for a class of GABAergic smooth dendritic neurons in layer 4 of neocortex that receive direct thalamic inputs and supply feedforward inhibition to both layer 2/3 and to other neurons in layer 4 (Gibson et al., 1999; Porter et al., 2001; Bruno and Simons, 2002; Beierlein et al., 2003; Hirsch et al., 2003; Swadlow, 2003). Such a mechanism could also explain the weak transient depolarization that frequently precedes luminance-evoked inhibition in layer 2/3 neurons (Fig. 1): the monosynaptic activation of layer 4 excitatory neurons by LGN inputs would be expected to occur before silencing by disynaptic feedforward inhibition.
Although this discussion has focused on the cortical circuitry likely to mediate luminance-evoked suppression, it presumes some difference in the response of LGN neurons to equiluminant and luminance step stimuli that could elicit such distinct cortical response patterns. One interesting possibility is suggested by the observation that large-scale changes in luminance frequently evoke high-frequency burst discharges in LGN neurons (Schiller, 1968; Ramcharan et al., 2001; Sherman, 2001; Lesica and Stanley, 2004; Alitto et al., 2005; Denning and Reinagel, 2005). Similar burst discharges in somatosensory thalamus have been shown to be effective in driving the responses of layer 4 cortical inhibitory neurons (Swadlow and Gusev, 2001). If one assumes that luminance steps evoke synchronous bursts in large populations of LGN relay neurons, this pattern of activity could preferentially recruit layer 4 inhibitory neurons, which in turn would deliver a strong synchronous burst of feedforward inhibition to target neurons in layer 4 and layer 2/3. Indeed, the synchronous activation of large populations of LGN neurons by electrical stimulation produces biphasic subthreshold responses in cortical neurons that are virtually identical to the waveforms we observe with changes in luminance (Ferster and Jagadeesh, 1992; Ferster et al., 1996; Kara et al., 2002).
Turning off cortical circuits: perceptual significance
Alterations in detection and discrimination of visual targets presented in close temporal proximity to large-scale changes in luminance have been well described and are likely the perceptual manifestation of the suppressive events described here (Crawford, 1947; Boynton and Kandel, 1957; Breitmeyer and Ogmen, 2000; Huang et al., 2005). The similarity in the time courses of perceptual masking and luminance-driven inhibition suggests that visual stimuli are masked from perception when their representation by cortical circuits is damped by strong intracortical inhibition. Perceptual masking is not limited to large-scale changes in luminance; spatially localized flashed stimuli also produce masking effects and these are sensitive to the locations and configuration of the mask and target stimuli (Bridgeman, 1971; Leopold and Logothetis, 1996; Macknik et al., 2000; Wilke et al., 2003). The extent to which these topographically restricted forms of visual masking rely on cortically derived inhibition remains to be determined.
Although the perceptual effects induced by changes in luminance have been a useful tool for exploring the temporal properties of visual processing in the laboratory, presumably such interruptions in the cortical representation of visual information are a common occurrence in natural vision as changes in gaze angle induce concurrent changes in target stimuli and the background in which they appear. Given that large-scale changes in luminance carry little information about the fine structure of visual scenes, transient cortical inhibition may act as a filter reducing the impact of luminance flux on cortical circuits responsible for extracting visual scene details. If left unabated, large-scale changes in luminance would likely exert a more protracted and disruptive impact on the information processing capabilities of cortical circuits, than that induced by luminance-evoked suppression. Although there may be a cost associated with these self-imposed gaps in visual perception, the fact that we are generally unaware of these gaps suggests (ironically) that transiently turning off cortical circuits plays an important role in insuring the continuity of visual experience.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by National Eye Institute Grant EY06821. We thank members of the Fitzpatrick laboratory for helpful discussion and comments on this manuscript.
- Correspondence should be addressed to David Fitzpatrick, Duke University Medical Center Box 3209, Department of Neurobiology, Durham, NC 27710. fitzpat{at}neuro.duke.edu