Abstract
Two forms of long-term depression (LTD), triggered by activation of NMDA receptors (NMDARs) and metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs), respectively, can be induced at CA1 synapses in the hippocampus. Compared with NMDAR-LTD, relatively little is known about mGluR-LTD. Here, we show that protein tyrosine phosphatase (PTP) inhibitors, orthovanadate and phenylarsine oxide, selectively block mGluR-LTD induced by application of the group I mGluR agonist (RS)-3,5-dihydroxyphenylglycine (DHPG-LTD), because NMDAR-LTD is unaffected by these inhibitors. Furthermore, DHPG-LTD measured using whole-cell recording is similarly blocked by either bath-applied or patch-loaded PTP inhibitors. These inhibitors also block the changes in paired-pulse facilitation and coefficient of variation that are associated with the expression of DHPG-LTD. DHPG treatment of hippocampal slices was associated with a decrease in the level of tyrosine phosphorylation of GluR2 AMPA receptor (AMPAR) subunits, an effect blocked by orthovanadate. Finally, in dissociated hippocampal neurons, orthovanadate blocked the ability of DHPG to reduce the number of AMPA receptor clusters on the surface of dendrites. Again, the effects of PTP blockade were selective, because NMDA-induced decreases in surface AMPAR clusters was unaffected by orthovanadate. Together, these data suggest that activation of postsynaptic PTP results in tyrosine dephosphorylation of AMPARs and their removal from the synapse.
- long-term depression
- metabotropic glutamate receptor
- AMPA receptor
- tyrosine phosphorylation
- protein tyrosine phosphatase
- hippocampus
Introduction
The ability to regulate synaptic strength is a critical feature of the CNS and is thought to underlie processes such as learning and memory. Long-term potentiation and long-term depression (LTD) have been studied intensely to understand the cellular and molecular mechanisms of synaptic plasticity (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Bear and Abraham, 1996). In the CA1 region, two forms of LTD can be distinguished based on their induction mechanisms; one requires activation of NMDA receptors (NMDARs) (Dudek and Bear, 1992; Mulkey and Malenka, 1992; Oliet et al., 1997), and the other the activation of metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs; mGluR-LTD) (Bashir et al., 1993; Bashir and Collingridge, 1994; Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Oliet et al., 1997). These two forms of LTD can be induced by agonists selective for NMDARs (Lee et al., 1998) and group I mGluRs (Palmer et al., 1997), respectively. Indeed, the group I mGluR agonist (RS)-3,5-dihydroxyphenylglycine (DHPG) is used widely to study mGluR-LTD (Palmer et al., 1997; Fitzjohn et al., 1999; Huber et al., 2000; Rouach and Nicoll, 2003; Tan et al., 2003; Gallagher et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2004). This DHPG-induced LTD (DHPG-LTD) has the important advantage of maximizing the number of synapses affected, facilitating investigations into the underlying biochemical mechanisms.
NMDAR-LTD involves an increase in postsynaptic Ca2+, activation of serine/threonine phosphatases, dephosphorylation of the GluR1 subunit at ser845 (Lee et al., 1998), and internalization of AMPA receptors (AMPARs) (Luthi et al., 1999; Beattie et al., 2000) at extrasynaptic sites (Ashby et al., 2004). DHPG-LTD does not occlude with NMDAR-LTD (Palmer et al., 1997; Fitzjohn et al., 1999; Huber et al., 2001) and is independent of Ca2+ (Fitzjohn et al., 2001) and serine/threonine phosphatases (Schnabel et al., 2001), indicating a very different underlying mechanism. DHPG-LTD involves activation of G-proteins (Watabe et al., 2002; Huang et al., 2004), protein tyrosine phosphatases (PTPs) (Moult et al., 2002), mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) cascades (Gallagher et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2004), and postsynaptic protein synthesis (Huber et al., 2000). However, the mechanism of expression of DHPG-LTD is unclear. For example, DHPG-LTD involves alterations in paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) (Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Watabe et al., 2002; Rouach and Nicoll, 2003; Tan et al., 2003) but no change in sensitivity to uncaged l-glutamate (Rammes et al., 2003), which is suggestive of a presynaptic alteration. In contrast, DHPG-LTD is prevented by inhibitors of endocytosis or protein synthesis applied to the postsynaptic cell and involves changes in the surface distribution of GluR1 subunits in both slices and cultured hippocampal neurons (Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001; Huang et al., 2004). It has been suggested that differences in the mechanisms of DHPG-LTD expression could be explained by a developmental switch from presynaptic to postsynaptic changes occurring at ∼2 weeks of age (Nosyreva and Huber, 2005).
In the present study, we investigated the role of PTPs in the expression mechanisms of DHPG-LTD. We show that PTP inhibitors block DHPG-LTD and the associated changes in paired-pulse facilitation, tyrosine dephosphorylation, and trafficking of AMPARs.
Materials and Methods
Hippocampal slice preparation.
Hippocampal slices (400 μm thick) were obtained from female Wistar rats (10–12 weeks of age, unless otherwise stated). Animals were killed by cervical dislocation in accordance with UK Animal (Scientific Procedures) legislation. The brains were removed rapidly and placed in ice-cold artificial CSF (aCSF) consisting of the following (in mm): 124 NaCl, 3 KCl, 26 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgSO4, and 10 d-glucose (bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2). Parasaggital brain slices were then prepared and the hippocampus isolated from surrounding tissue. The CA3 region was removed in all cases. Slices were allowed to recover at room temperature in oxygenated aCSF for at least 1 h before use.
Cell culture.
Primary rat hippocampal neuronal cultures were prepared as described previously (Pickard et al., 2000, 2001) with a few modifications. The CA3–CA1 region of the hippocampus from 1- to 3-d-old rat pups was dissected at 4°C, and the neurons were recovered by enzymatic digestion with trypsin and mechanical dissociation. Cells were then plated at a density of ∼50,000 per dish onto 22 mm glass coverslips coated with poly-d-lysine (0.5 mg/ml; Sigma, Poole, Dorset, UK). Cultures were maintained at 37°C in a 95% O2, 5% CO2 humidified incubator. The culture media were composed of Neurobasal A (Invitrogen, Paisley, UK) supplemented with 2 mm glutamine, 2% B27, 10% horse serum, and 2 mg/ml gentamycin. From the second day, the B27 and horse serum content of the medium was reduced to 1%. From the third day in culture, the media were supplemented with 10 μm uradine and 10 μm 5-fluoro-2′-deoxyuridine (Sigma) to prevent glial cell proliferation. Culture media were changed twice a week. Neurons were used for experiments 13–15 d after plating.
Extracellular recordings.
Grease-gap recordings were obtained from the CA1 region as described previously (Blake et al., 1988; Moult et al., 2002). Briefly, slices were placed on a glass coverslip on the surface of an inclined temperature-controlled unit (maintained at ∼30°C). The slice was partially covered with absorbent paper and superfused at a rate of 2 ml/min with aCSF (composition as above) bubbled continuously with 95% O2/5% CO2. For DHPG-LTD experiments, the GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (50 μm) and the NMDAR antagonist L-689,560 (trans-2-carboxy-5,7-dichloro-4-phenylaminocarbonylamino-1,2,3,4-tetrahydroquinoline; 5 μm) were present throughout (Moult et al., 2002). The Schaffer collateral–commissural pathway was stimulated at 0.033 Hz at an intensity that evoked a field EPSP (fEPSP) slope of ∼50% of the maximum. Responses were digitized at 5 kHz and recorded on-line using the LTP program (Anderson and Collingridge, 2001). The maximum slope was obtained from 20–80% of the incline of the response.
Whole-cell recordings from hippocampal slices.
Slices were transferred to a submerged recording chamber maintained at 30°C and perfused continuously with aCSF (composition as above) at a rate of 2–3 ml/min. The GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (50 μm) and the NMDA receptor antagonist L-689,560 (5 μm) were present throughout. Electrodes with a resistance in the range of 4–7 MΩ were filled with Cs-based filling solution, which comprised the following (in mm): 130 CsMeSO4, 8 NaCl, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, 0.5 EGTA, 10 HEPES, and N-(2,6-dimethylphenylcarbamoylmethyl)triethylammonium chloride. The pH was adjusted to 7.2–7.3 using CsOH, while osmolarity was adjusted to 275–290 mOsm with sucrose if necessary. Where PTP inhibitors were included in the filling solution, these were used at 10-fold lower concentration than used extracellularly, and adjustments to pH and osmolarity were made accordingly. Blind whole-cell recordings were made from neurons in the CA1 pyramidal layer voltage clamped at −70 mV. The Schaffer collateral–commissural pathway was stimulated at 0.033 Hz. The stimulus intensity used was that which evoked a peak amplitude of ∼50% of the maximum. Responses were digitized at 10 kHz.
Whole-cell recordings from hippocampal cultures.
Whole-cell recordings from pyramidal neurons were obtained as previously detailed (Fitzjohn et al., 2001). Cells were voltage clamped at −70 mV using the same intracellular solutions used for recording from hippocampal slices. Cells were perfused continuously with HEPES-buffered saline (HBS) of the following composition (in mm): 119 NaCl, 5 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, 25 HEPES, 33 d-glucose, 0.005 tetrodotoxin citrate, 0.1 picrotoxin, and 0.001 glycine, pH 7.4, at a rate of 2–3 ml·min−1. DHPG was applied by addition to the HBS perfusate. Data were filtered at 2 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz. Continuous recording of miniature EPSCs (mEPSCs) was made using Axoscope software (Molecular Devices, Union City, CA) and series resistance measured using the LTP program.
Electrophysiological analysis.
fEPSPs and EPSC data from hippocampal slices were monitored on-line and analyzed off-line using the LTP program (Anderson and Collingridge, 2001). LTD was calculated 25–30 min after washout of the agonist and expressed as a percentage of the baseline. The paired-pulse ratio (PPR) was calculated as the ratio of the amplitude of the second EPSC to the first EPSC. To ensure that the measurement of the peak amplitude of the second EPSC was not contaminated by residual current from the first EPSC, the residual component was removed. This was done by extrapolation of the first EPSC and using this point to calculate the peak using the MiniAnalysis 5.6.25 program (Synaptosoft, Decatur, GA). The PPR was calculated from an average of five successive sweeps immediately before DHPG, during, and between 25 and 30 min after DHPG application ceased. The coefficient of variation (CV) was calculated as described previously (Kullmann, 1994; Fitzjohn et al., 2001). Briefly, the mean and SD were calculated for the EPSC amplitudes recorded during successive 5 min epochs (SDEPSC and meanEPSC). The SD of the background noise was also calculated for each 5 min epoch using a period immediately before electrical stimulation (SDNoise). The CV for each epoch was calculated as (SDEPSC − SDNoise)/meanEPSC.
mEPSC detection was performed using MiniAnalysis software (Synaptosoft). Events were detected by setting the threshold value for detection at three times the level of the root mean square noise, followed by visual confirmation of mEPSC detection. For construction of cumulative probability plots, 400 successive events were used from the period immediately preceding application of DHPG and from the period before 30 min after commencing washout of DHPG. For pooled cumulative probability plots, data were normalized to the median value in the pre-DHPG period.
Statistical analysis was performed using Student's t test. n signifies the number of times a given experiment was performed, with each experiment using a slice from a different rat. Experimental treatments and corresponding control experiments were interleaved throughout. Where multiple comparisons were made against one of the control groups (see Figs. 1⇓–3), an ANOVA with post hoc Bonferroni's test was applied.
Immunoprecipitation and immunoblot analysis.
Hippocampal slices were taken at the end of a 10 min DHPG application or after 30 min washout. These slices, and parallel controls, were then homogenized in Eppendorf Scientific (Westbury, NY) tubes with a pellet pestle on ice in 11% (w/v) sucrose, 10 mm HEPES, pH 7.2, 100 μm genistein, 1 mm orthovanadate in the presence of a mixture of “complete” protease inhibitors (Roche Products, Welwyn Garden City, UK) and phosphatase inhibitor mixture 1 (Sigma) to prevent degradation and dephosphorylation of proteins. Homogenized samples were centrifuged at 1000 × g for 15 min at 4°C to remove tissue debris. Supernatants were spun at 100,000 × g for 1 h at 4°C and pellets then stored at −80°C.
Membrane proteins were solubilized in 500 μl of lysis buffer [1% (v/v) Triton X-100, 0.1% (w/v) SDS, 150 mm NaCl, 20 mm HEPES, 2 mm EDTA, 100 μm genistein, and 1 mm orthovanadate in the presence of a mixture of complete protease inhibitors (Roche Products) and phosphatase inhibitor mixture 1 (Sigma)]. Samples were sonicated and rotated for 1–2 h at 4°C, followed by centrifugation at 100,000 × g for 30 min at 4°C. After removing a 50 μl aliquot for a whole input sample, supernatants were incubated with rabbit anti-GluR1–4 antibody (3.5 μl per slice) (Pickard et al., 2000) overnight at 4°C. Sepharose A beads [8 μl per slice; pretreated with 5% (w/v) BSA in lysis buffer for 30 min; Sigma] were used to separate antibody/AMPAR complexes. After 2 h incubation at 4°C, Sepharose beads were separated by centrifugation (1000 × g, 5 min, 4°C), and aliquots of the supernatants were mixed with 1:1 ratio of 2× SDS sample buffer [2% (w/v) SDS, 50 mm Tris-HCl, pH 6.8, 10% (v/v) 2-mercaptoethanol, 10% (v/v) glycerol, 0.1% (w/v) bromophenol blue] and stored at −20°C. Beads were washed with 1 and 0.1% (v/v) Triton X-100-containing lysis buffer (two times in each) and once with PBS supplemented with 100 μm genistein, 1 mm orthovanadate in the presence of a mixture of complete protease inhibitors (Roche Products) and phosphatase inhibitor mixture 1 (Sigma). Proteins associated with Sepharose beads were extracted with 2× SDS-PAGE sample buffer at 37°C for 10 min.
For the separate immunoprecipitation of GluR1 and GluR2 proteins, AMPA receptor subunits were dissociated in a modified radioimmunoprecipitation assay (RIPA) buffer [1% (v/v) Triton X-100, 1% (w/v) SDS, 0.4 (w/v) sodium deoxycholate, 2 mm EDTA, 150 mm NaCl, 50 mm Tris-HCl, pH 7.4] in the presence of protease and phosphatase inhibitors at 100°C for 5 min. Individual subunits were immunoprecipitated using mouse anti-GluR2 (0.5 mg per slice; MAB397; Chemicon, Temecula, CA), rabbit anti-GluR2 (0.4 mg per slice; AB1768–25UG; Chemicon), or rabbit anti-GluR1 (0.4 mg per slice; catalog #07-660; Upstate Biotechnology, Lake Placid, NY) antibodies using the above-described procedure.
Immunoprecipitated proteins were separated on 8% (w/v) SDS-PAGE gels followed by transfer electrophoretically onto polyvinylidene difluoride membranes using an Atto (Tokyo, Japan) HorizBlot electrophoretic transfer unit with a discontinuous buffer system for 1.5 h at room temperature, as recommended by the manufacturer. Before the immunostaining with the anti-phosphotyrosine P-Y20-horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-conjugated antibody (ICN Biomedicals, Costa Mesa, CA), membranes were blocked with TBS containing 1% (w/v) ovalbumin, 3% (w/v) BSA, and 0.1% (v/v) Tween 20 for 1 h at 4°C. Blots were blocked with 5% (w/v) milk, 0.1% (v/v) Tween 20 in TBS for 1 h at 4°C before labeling with the guinea pig anti-GluR1–4 antibody (Pickard et al., 2001) After three washes in 0.1% Tween 20 in TBS (TBST), blots were incubated with the P-Y20-HRP antibody (1:5000 dilution) in 1% ovalbumin, 3% BSA in TBST, or in guinea pig anti-GluR1–4 antibody (1:2000 dilution), rabbit anti-GluR1 (1.96 mg/ml), mouse anti-GluR2 (1.96 mg/ml), or rabbit anti-GluR2 (0.25 mg/ml) subunit-selective antibodies in 5% (w/v) milk powder in TBST, overnight at 4°C. The immunolabeling obtained with the primary antibodies were revealed with the appropriate HRP-conjugated anti-guinea pig (1:7500 dilution; Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA), HRP anti-mouse (1:7500 dilution; Sigma) or alkaline phosphatase-conjugated anti-rabbit (1:5000 dilution; Promega, Madison, WI) secondary antibodies for 2 h at room temperature. The enhanced chemiluminescence (ECL) detection system (Roche Diagnostics, Lewes, UK) was used for developing blots with HRP-conjugated antibodies. Lumi-Phos WB chemiluminescent substrate (Pierce, Rockford, IL) was used for alkaline phosphatase-conjugated antibodies. The β-actin content of each input/homogenized slice sample was analyzed using an anti-β-actin mouse monoclonal antibody (Abcam, Cambridge, UK) visualized by an alkaline phosphatase-conjugated secondary antibody (Promega) and Lumi-Phos WB chemiluminescent substrate (Pierce, Rockford). A series of antigen concentrations, primary and secondary antibody dilutions, and ECL exposure times were used to optimize our experimental conditions for the linear sensitivity range of the autoradiography film (Hyperfilm MP; Amersham Biosciences, Little Chalfont, Buckinghamshire, UK). Each immunoreactive band was subjected to densitometry using a Bio-Rad (Hercules, CA) imaging system (Quantity One) to determine the pixel volume (pixel intensity-by-area).
Immunofluorescence staining and imaging of hippocampal neurons.
Hippocampal cultures were treated with agonists, with or without antagonists, as described in Figure 9. One hour later, cultures were fixed using 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS for 15 min followed by incubation in 10 mm glycine in PBS for 5 min at 4°C. Nonpermeabilized cells were washed in HBS (119 mm NaCl, 5 mm KCl, 25 mm HEPES, 33 mm d-glucose, 2 mm CaCl2, 2 mm MgCl2, 1 μm glycine, pH 7.4; one time for 5 min) and in HBS supplemented with 50% Neurobasal A and 0.5% horse serum (three times for 10 min) at 4°C. Surface GluR1 was labeled with an extracellular N-terminal domain-specific antibody (1 μg/ml, 1 h, 4°C) (Molnar et al., 1993). After washing in 5% bovine serum albumin containing HBS (three times for 10 min), cells were probed with Alexa 488-cojugated anti-rabbit secondary antibodies in HBS with 5% BSA for 1 h at 4°C. Coverslips were mounted in Vectashield mounting medium (Vector Laboratories, Bretton, UK).
Images of immunolabeled neurons were captured using an inverted Zeiss (Oberkochen, Germany) LSM 510 Meta confocal laser scanning microscope using a 63× lens. The 488 and 633 nm laser bands of an Ar laser were used for dual dye excitation and FITC/tetramethylrhodamine isothiocyanate filters for fluorescence emission. The quantitative analysis was performed blind using NIH ImageJ (http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/index.html). The threshold for GluR1-positive fluorescent clusters (or puncta) was fixed at twice the level of background fluorescence obtained from a region of diffuse fluorescence within the dendritic shaft. Clusters were ∼1–2.5 μm is size. Only puncta lying along processes interpreted as dendrites were counted. Regions where the clear identification of neuronal processes was ambiguous were excluded from the quantification. For statistical analysis, independent group t tests were used. For quantitative comparisons, the number of puncta per 100 μm length of dendrite was expressed for the GluR1 antibody within a given field.
Results
PTP inhibitors block DHPG- but not NMDA-induced LTD
In agreement with previous studies (Palmer et al., 1997; Fitzjohn et al., 1999; Huber et al., 2000; Rouach and Nicoll, 2003; Tan et al., 2003; Gallagher et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2004), bath application of (RS)-DHPG (100 μm, 10 min) resulted in a sustained depression of synaptic transmission. Field EPSPs were depressed to 72 ± 2% of baseline, 30 min after washout of DHPG (n = 11; p < 0.05 vs baseline) (Fig. 1A). The effects of DHPG were prevented by the specific mGluR5 antagonist 2-methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)pyridine hydrochloride (MPEP) (data not shown), which is consistent with DHPG-LTD being mediated via activation of mGluR5 (Palmer et al., 1997; Fitzjohn et al., 1999; Huang et al., 2004). Interleaved with control experiments, we applied one of two structurally distinct PTP inhibitors [orthovanadate, 1 mm and phenylarsine oxide (PAO), 15 μm]. Neither compound affected baseline synaptic transmission (102 ± 1 and 101 ± 1% of baseline, respectively; data not shown), but both compounds attenuated DHPG-LTD (89 ± 3%, n = 6, p > 0.05; and 109 ± 9%, n = 6, p > 0.05, respectively) (Moult et al., 2002) (PAO data illustrated in Fig. 1B). In contrast, application of orthovanadate (1 mm) or PAO (15 μm) had no effect on pre-established DHPG-LTD; responses before and during orthovanadate application were 67 ± 4 and 74 ± 4% of baseline (n = 3; p > 0.05); responses before and during PAO application were 68 ± 4 and 73 ± 3% of control (n = 3; p > 0.05) (Fig. 1C).
To determine whether PTP inhibition selectively blocked DHPG-LTD, the two PTP inhibitors were tested against NMDAR-LTD, in slices obtained from 3-week-old rats. NMDA (20 μm, 3 min) caused a large initial depression of synaptic transmission followed by a sustained depression (71 ± 6%; p < 0.05; n = 8) (Fig. 1D). In contrast to DHPG-LTD, neither orthovanadate nor PAO blocked NMDAR-LTD (64 ± 4%, n = 5, p < 0.05; and 63 ± 5%, n = 5, p < 0.05, respectively) (PAO data illustrated in Fig. 1E). These results indicate that the block of DHPG-LTD by either orthovanadate or PAO is unlikely to be caused by some nonspecific effect.
Protein tyrosine kinase inhibitors prevent the effects of PTP inhibitors
We reported previously that the protein tyrosine kinase (PTK) inhibitors genistein and lavendustin A had no significant effect on baseline transmission or DHPG-LTD (Moult et al., 2002). Similarly, we find that the src-family PTK inhibitor 3-(4-chlorophenyl)1-(1,1-dimethylethyl)-1H-pyrazolo[3,4-d]pyrimidin-4-amine (PP2) had no effect on baseline synaptic transmission or on DHPG-LTD (70 ± 5%; n = 6; p < 0.05) (Fig. 2A). Surprisingly, however, PP2 completely prevented the inhibition of DHPG-LTD by orthovanadate (76 ± 7%; n = 6; p < 0.05) (Fig. 2B) or PAO (72 ± 7%; n = 6; p < 0.05) (Fig. 2C). This effect was not unique to PP2, because both genistein and lavendustin A prevented the inhibition of DHPG-LTD by orthovanadate [73 ± 9%, n = 6, p < 0.05 (Fig. 2D); and 66 ± 5%, n = 6, p < 0.05 (Fig. 2E), respectively] or PAO (74 ± 5%, n = 6, p < 0.05; and 67 ± 7%, n = 6, p < 0.05, respectively; data not illustrated). In all cases, these effects were not significantly different from interleaved controls (72 ± 3%; n = 8) (Fig. 2F). Thus, three structurally distinct PTK inhibitors have no direct effect on baseline transmission or DHPG-LTD but are able to completely prevent the effects of the PTP inhibitors.
Analysis of DHPG-LTD using whole-cell recording
To investigate the mechanism of DHPG-LTD, we made whole-cell recordings from hippocampal slices (Fig. 3). In agreement with the extracellular experiments, DHPG (100 μm, 20 min) was able to induce LTD (59 ± 8%; n = 13; p < 0.05) (Fig. 3A) without affecting the holding current of the cesium-loaded neurons. In interleaved experiments, DHPG-LTD was blocked by bath application of orthovanadate (96 ± 5%; n = 6; p > 0.05) (Fig. 3B) or PAO (97 ± 4%; n = 6; p > 0.05) (Fig. 3C) or by patch loading of 100 μm orthovanadate (111 ± 14%; n = 6; p > 0.05) (Fig. 3D) or 1.5 μm PAO (92 ± 9%; n = 6; p > 0.05) (Fig. 3E). This result is unlikely to be explained by diffusion of the inhibitors out of the loaded cell to inhibit presynaptic PTPs, because a 10-fold lower concentration of the inhibitors was included in the patch solution compared with bath application. These data suggest that the PTPs critical for the induction of LTD are located in the postsynaptic neuron.
Several studies have reported that DHPG-LTD is associated with changes in PPF, which is classically interpreted as an alteration in the probability of release (Pr). Such a finding is not readily compatible with the observation that postsynaptic loading of PTP inhibitors blocks DHPG-LTD. One possible explanation is that there are both presynaptic and postsynaptic mechanisms of expression of DHPG-LTD and that PTP inhibitors only affect the postsynaptic changes, which may dominate in slices taken from older animals (Nosyreva and Huber, 2005). We therefore analyzed PPF in the whole-cell recording experiments from slices prepared from 10- to 12-week-old animals (Fig. 4A–C). In each experiment, two pulses were delivered at an interval of 50 ms and the paired-pulse ratio calculated 30 min after washout of DHPG. Under control conditions, DHPG caused an increase in the PPR (from 1.5 ± 0.1 to 1.9 ± 0.1; n = 13; p < 0.05). In contrast, in neurons loaded with orthovanadate or PAO, there was no change in the PPR (in both cases, 1.6 ± 0.1 to 1.4 ± 0.03; n = 6; p > 0.05). Similar results were obtained with the whole-cell recording experiments using bath-applied inhibitors (data not shown).
We also analyzed the CV for these experiments (Fig. 4D). Under control conditions, the CV of the first EPSC of the pair increased during application of DHPG, and this increase was sustained for as long as recordings were obtained. For example, 30 min after washout of DHPG, the CV had changed from 0.22 ± 0.02 to 0.33 ± 0.03 (n = 13; p < 0.05). In contrast, in neurons loaded with orthovanadate or PAO, the CV was unchanged (orthovanadate, 0.22 ± 0.03 to 0.22 ± 0.03, n = 6, p > 0.05; PAO, 0.24 ± 0.02 to 0.23 ± 0.02, n = 6, p > 0.05). Again, similar results were obtained with the whole-cell recording experiments using bath-applied inhibitors (data not shown).
These results confirm that changes in PPF and CV are associated with DHPG-LTD. We now show that these changes, which are usually interpreted as being presynaptic in origin, are dependent on postsynaptic PTPs.
Effects of jasplakinolide on DHPG-LTD
There are, broadly speaking, two explanations for the observation that DHPG-LTD is associated with changes in PPF and CV. The first is that postsynaptic PTPs regulate neurotransmitter release, perhaps via release of a retrograde messenger. The second is that the alterations in PPF and CV reflect postsynaptic changes. To further investigate these possibilities, we applied the actin stabilizing drug jasplakinolide to the postsynaptic neuron. This compound has been shown to block AMPA receptor endocytosis induced by glutamate application (Zhou et al., 2001) and, furthermore, has been shown to block DHPG-induced LTD (Xiao et al., 2001). In agreement with this report, inclusion of jasplakinolide (2 μm) in the recording solution completely blocked DHPG-LTD (2 ± 2%; n = 4; p > 0.05) (Fig. 5A,B). Jasplakinolide also prevented the associated changes in PPF and CV (PPR, 1.6 ± 0.04 and 1.7 ± 0.04, p > 0.05; CV, 0.22 ± 0.02 and 0.23 ± 0.03, p > 0.05) (Fig. 5C,D). In two interleaved controls, DHPG-LTD was associated with changes in both PPF and CV, similar to those shown in Figure 4.
DHPG-LTD is associated with tyrosine dephosphorylation of AMPAR subunits
Because postsynaptic PTPs are involved in DHPG-LTD, a possible target for tyrosine dephosphorylation is the AMPAR itself. Therefore, we next analyzed tyrosine phosphorylation levels of AMPAR subunit proteins. We developed conditions for the complete immunoprecipitation of all AMPARs from solubilized hippocampal slices, which are suitable for quantitative comparisons (Fig. 6A). Using an HRP-conjugated anti-P-Y antibody, a single ∼105 kDa protein was identified in the immunoprecipitated fraction, which corresponds with the correct size for the AMPAR subunit proteins (Fig. 6B). The clearly detectable P-Y immunoreactivity in the immunoprecipitated fractions obtained from nontreated hippocampal slices indicates that at least some of the AMPARs are phosphorylated at tyrosine residues under basal conditions. After DHPG treatment of hippocampal slices (100 μm, 10 min), the level of P-Y AMPAR immunoreactivity was substantially decreased (56 ± 12%; n = 11; p < 0.05) (Fig. 6C,D). The DHPG-induced reduction in tyrosine phosphorylation of AMPARs was transient and returned to control levels 30 min after DHPG treatment (82 ± 13%; n = 5; p > 0.05) (Fig. 6C,D). The ability of DHPG to induce tyrosine dephosphorylation of AMPARs was blocked when DHPG was applied in the presence of orthovanadate (118 ± 24%; n = 6; p > 0.05) (Fig. 6C,D). The DHPG and/or orthovanadate treatments did not significantly change the total amount of AMPAR subunits (Fig. 6C,E). These results indicate that DHPG-LTD involves a transient tyrosine dephosphorylation of AMPARs.
To identify which AMPAR subunits are affected during DHPG-LTD, we separated individual AMPAR proteins in a RIPA buffer at 100°C before immunoprecipitation with GluR1- and GluR2-selective antibodies (Fig. 7). Immunoblot analysis of separately immunoprecipitated GluR1 and GluR2 proteins revealed that the phosphorylated tyrosine residues are mainly associated with the GluR2 subunit. Furthermore, there was a marked reduction in tyrosine phosphorylation of GluR2 after DHPG treatment without detectable changes in the phosphorylation state of GluR1. Indeed, in agreement with a previous study (Ahmadian et al., 2004), GluR1 showed virtually undetectable levels of tyrosine phosphorylation. These results demonstrate that DHPG treatment of hippocampal slices is associated with reduction in tyrosine phosphorylation of GluR2 subunits.
DHPG-induced reduction in mEPSC frequency is blocked by PTP inhibition
DHPG-LTD can also be observed in cultured dissociated hippocampal neurons, where it is manifest as a decrease in the frequency of mEPSCs (Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001). To determine whether this effect was also dependent on postsynaptic PTPs, we interleaved control experiments with experiments in which the whole-cell solution contained orthovanadate (100 μm). In agreement with our previous findings (Fitzjohn et al., 2001), DHPG resulted in a persistent decrease in the frequency of mEPSCs (frequency, 80 ± 5% of baseline 30 min after DHPG washout; p < 0.05; n = 7) (Fig. 8) but no change in the amplitude of mEPSCs (before DHPG, 17 ± 1 pA; 30 min after DHPG, 17 ± 1 pA; p > 0.05; n = 7) (Fig. 8). The effect of DHPG on mEPSC frequency was blocked by orthovanadate (30 min after DHPG application, mEPSC frequency was 102 ± 12% of baseline; average mEPSC amplitude was 17 ± 2 and 16 ± 2 pA before and 30 min after DHPG, respectively; p > 0.05; n = 9) (Fig. 8). Therefore, cultured dissociated hippocampal neurons can be used to study PTP-dependent DHPG-LTD.
DHPG-induced reduction in GluR1 cell surface clusters is selectively blocked by PTP inhibition
It has been reported previously that DHPG-LTD is associated with changes in synaptic AMPAR number (Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001; Huang et al., 2004). We analyzed the surface density of AMPAR GluR1 subunits in cultured hippocampal neurons by an extracellular N-terminal domain-specific antibody (Molnar et al., 1993) and confocal imaging (Figs. 9, 10). Treatment with DHPG resulted in a reduction in GluR1-immunopositive clusters (control, 24 ± 2 per 100 μm dendrite, n = 9; DHPG, 16 ± 2 per 100 μm dendrite, n = 9; p < 0.05). This effect of DHPG was dependent on activation of mGluR5 but not mGluR1 or NMDA receptors, because it was blocked by 10 μm MPEP (24 ± 1 per 100 μm dendrite; n = 9; p > 0.05) but not by 100 μm LY367385 (16 ± 3 per 100 μm dendrite; n = 8; p < 0.05) or 5 μm L-689,560 (14 ± 2 per 100 μm dendrite; n = 8; p < 0.05), respectively (Fig. 10). Crucially, the effect of DHPG was blocked by orthovanadate (25 ± 2 per 100 μm dendrite; n = 8; p > 0.05). NMDA treatment also reduced GluR1-immunopositive clusters compared with controls (12 ± 2 per 100 μm dendrite; n = 8; p < 0.05). However, the effect of NMDA was not blocked by orthovanadate (10 ± 2 per 100 μm dendrite; n = 4; p > 0.05).
These results indicate that the DHPG-induced reduction in density of GluR1 surface clusters is an mGluR5- and PTP-dependent process. The differential sensitivity to the PTP inhibitor orthovanadate suggests that DHPG- and NMDA-induced reduction in surface GluR1 puncta involves separate intracellular mechanisms.
Discussion
Recently, we reported that bath application of two structurally distinct PTP inhibitors (Coussens et al., 2000) can block the induction of DHPG-LTD (Moult et al., 2002). Our present observation that direct loading of a single neuron with either of these inhibitors is as effective as bath application strongly suggests that the critical PTP(s) associated with DHPG-LTD is located postsynaptically. The present study uses female rats, because our original description of DHPG-LTD (Palmer et al., 1997) and its subsequent characterization (Palmer et al., 1997; Schnabel et al., 2001; Moult et al., 2002; Rammes et al., 2003) were conducted using this sex. Although studies on male animals have reached broadly similar conclusions (Watabe et al., 2002; Rammes et al., 2003; Huang et al., 2004), it should be born in mind that possible sex linked differences could exist.
In addition to the sustained depression, DHPG induces a transient depression that is less sensitive or insensitive to PTP inhibitors. Surprisingly, although this transient depression was unaffected when the PTP inhibitors were bath applied, it was reduced when the PTP inhibitors were loaded directly into the postsynaptic cell. This might reflect a lower effective concentration of the bath-applied inhibitors at their postsynaptic site of action. However, the finding that PTP inhibitors block DHPG-LTD while having either a small or no effect on the transient depression suggest that not all group I mGluR-mediated effects may involve activation of PTPs to the same extent. The involvement of PTPs in other group I mGluR effects has also been demonstrated recently. For example, PTP inhibition can reduce the block of the slow-duration afterhyperpolarization seen with DHPG treatment in CA1 pyramidal neurons (Ireland et al., 2004) and also reduce the mGluR1-dependent slow EPSC seen in cerebellar Purkinje neurons (Canepari and Ogden, 2003). Thus, group I mGluR signaling may couple to PTP activation, producing a variety of actions. What is currently unknown is whether mGluR activation couples directly to PTP activation or whether this requires a number of intermediary steps.
Tyrosine dephosphorylation of AMPARs is involved in DHPG-LTD
Our data show that DHPG treatment decreases the level of tyrosine-phosphorylated AMPAR protein, with no change in total AMPAR expression. This effect is blocked in the presence of orthovanadate. Thus, it is likely that tyrosine dephosphorylation of AMPAR subunits is involved in the expression of DHPG-LTD, possibly via an effect on AMPAR trafficking. Because we see only a transient change in tyrosine phosphorylation of AMPARs, it is likely that this change provides a trigger for the internalization process. Our findings suggest that it is the GluR2 subunit that is regulated by changes in tyrosine phosphorylation during DHPG-LTD. GluR1 surface expression decreases with DHPG treatment, yet the tyrosine phosphorylation state of GluR1 is not altered by DHPG. This suggests that the change in GluR2 phosphorylation is sufficient to drive changes in the surface expression of heteromeric AMPA receptor assemblies. Previous work has identified the GluR2 subunit as being critically involved in AMPAR trafficking (Nishimune et al., 1998). Interestingly, during insulin-stimulated LTD, there is phosphorylation of tyrosine residues of GluR2 that is associated with AMPAR internalization (Ahmadian et al., 2004). Therefore, both tyrosine phosphorylation and dephosphorylation could regulate different aspects of AMPAR trafficking at synapses.
A surprising observation was that, although PTK inhibitors had no significant effect on DHPG-LTD per se, they were able to completely block the effects of PTP inhibitors. A similar regulation of PTPs by PTKs has been reported with respect to the inhibition of NMDAR-mediated synaptic transmission (Coussens et al., 2000). One possible mechanism to explain our current observations involves two parallel induction pathways. One path is independent of tyrosine phosphorylation and may involve, for example, a MAPK cascade (Gallagher et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2004). The second pathway involves activation of a PTP to inhibit a PTK, resulting in a dephosphorylayion of AMPARs. Both pathways must be stimulated by mGluR activation for LTD to be induced. Inhibition of PTP enables activation of the PTK to maintain the phosphorylation state of AMPARs and thereby inhibit the induction of LTD. Inhibition of PTKs is not itself sufficient to induce LTD as the parallel pathway is not activated. However, PTK inhibitors counteract the effect of PTP inhibitors, because PTP inhibitors have their effect via activation of PTK.
An unusual aspect of DHPG-LTD is that it can be reversed during the application of an mGluR antagonist long after washout of DHPG (Palmer et al., 1997). This could be because DHPG induces a constitutively active state in mGluR5, and antagonists reverse this constitutive activity for as long as they are bound to the receptor. Therefore, the activation state of PTPs might mirror that of the mGluR5, such that PTPs become persistently activated. However, we found that neither PAO nor orthovanadate was able to reverse DHPG-LTD when applied 30 min after washout of DHPG. Furthermore, we found that the decrease in tyrosine phosphorylation had essentially recovered to control levels 30 min after treatment with DHPG. These data are more compatible with a transient dephosphorylation resulting in a persistent alteration in AMPARs at the synapse.
Expression mechanism of DHPG-LTD
In agreement with previous reports, in the current study, DHPG-LTD was associated with an increase in the PPR and CV of EPSCs and a decrease in mEPSC frequency (Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Rouach and Nicoll, 2003; Tan et al., 2003). An increase in failure rate during DHPG-LTD in slices has also been reported (Fitzjohn et al., 2001). The simplest interpretation of these observations is that DHPG-LTD involves a decrease in Pr. In addition, other evidence has suggested a presynaptic mechanism of expression of DHPG-LTD in 5- to 7-week-old mice (Watabe et al., 2002). We have now found that the changes in PPR, CV, and mEPSC frequency are prevented by postsynaptic block of PTPs. One possibility, therefore, is that during DHPG-LTD, postsynaptic PTP activation leads to a decrease in transmitter release, perhaps via regulation of a retrograde messenger. Indeed, there is evidence that synaptically induced mGluR-LTD is induced postsynaptically yet is expressed presynaptically (Bolshakov and Siegelbaum, 1994; Oliet et al., 1997; Zakharenko et al., 2002) and involves retrograde signaling involving 12-lipoxygenase metabolites of arachidonic acid (Feinmark et al., 2003).
Recently, Nosyreva and Huber (2005) suggested that the site of expression of mGluR-LTD is developmentally regulated, because there is a greater change in PPF in neonatal [postnatal day 8 (P8) to P15] compared with adolescent (P21–P35) animals. Conversely, changes in surface expression of AMPARs associated with DHPG-LTD were only observed in adolescent animals. However, our electrophysiological experiments, which revealed consistent effects on PPR, were conducted in slices obtained from young adult rats of 10–12 weeks of age, and previous work has reported changes in PPF in animals ranging from 2 to 6 weeks of age (Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Rouach and Nicoll, 2003; Tan et al., 2003). Therefore, although there may be quantitative differences, changes in PPF associated with DHPG-LTD are observed across all age ranges examined.
In the present study, we confirmed that there was a decrease of surface GluR1 AMPAR subunit clusters after DHPG treatment (Snyder et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2001; Huang et al., 2004). We found that block of PTP prevented the DHPG-induced decrease in surface GluR1 AMPA receptor proteins. This effect of PTP inhibition was specific for DHPG-LTD, because the NMDA-induced decrease of GluR1 surface staining was unaffected by orthovanadate. A simple explanation for these results is that postsynaptic PTP activation leads to removal of AMPARs from the synapse. It is therefore possible that postsynaptic activation of PTP triggers a combination of presynaptic and postsynaptic expression mechanisms. An alternative explanation is that DHPG-LTD expression is entirely postsynaptic but that it appears to be presynaptic as assessed by electrophysiological measures. This scenario could occur if DHPG brought about complete silencing of synapses by removal of the postsynaptic AMPAR complement (Snyder et al., 2001). This could account for the reported changes in failure rate and mEPSC frequency as well as the alteration in CV observed in this and a previous study (Fitzjohn et al., 2001). The reduction in AMPAR clusters is consistent with this possibility. In addition, the increase in PPF would occur if high Pr synapses were preferentially silenced, as suggested previously (Fitzjohn et al., 2001). The finding that jasplakinolide also blocked changes in PPF and CV associated with DHPG-LTD also supports the idea that alterations in AMPAR trafficking can account for the alterations in CV and PPF. Therefore, a purely postsynaptic mechanism for the induction and expression of DHPG-LTD is the simplest explanation for our observations. Previously, we found that, whereas NMDAR-LTD is associated with a reduction in sensitivity to uncaged l-glutamate, DHPG is not associated with any change (Rammes et al., 2003). We interpreted this result to mean that DHPG is either caused by a presynaptic mechanism or to lateral diffusion of AMPARs to extrasynaptic sites that are affected by the l-glutamate application. In light of the present findings, the latter possibility is the more likely explanation for these results. Therefore, it seems likely that DHPG-LTD involves PTP-regulated lateral diffusion of AMPARs away from the synaptic site.
Footnotes
We are grateful to the Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust for financial support.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Peter Moult, Medical Research Council Centre for Synaptic Plasticity, Department of Anatomy, University of Bristol, School of Medical Sciences, University Walk, Bristol BS8 1TD, UK. Email: p.r.moult{at}bristol.ac.uk