Abstract
The behavioral and biochemical impact of active immunization against human β-amyloid (Aβ) was assessed using male transgenic (Tg) mice overexpressing a human mutant amyloid precursor protein (heterozygous PDAPP mice) and littermate controls. Administration of aggregated Aβ42 occurred at monthly intervals from 7 months (“prevention”) or 11 months (“reversal”), followed by double-blind behavioral training at 16 months on a cued task, then serial spatial learning in a water maze. Using a 2 × 2 design, with Aβ42 adjuvanted with MPL-AF (adjuvant formulation of monophosphoryl lipid A) or MPL-AF alone, PDAPP mice were impaired compared with non-Tg littermates on two separate measures of serial spatial learning. Immunization caused no overall rescue of learning but limited the accumulation of total Aβ and Aβ42 levels in cortex and hippocampus by up to 60%. In immunized PDAPP mice, significant negative correlations were observed between hippocampal and cortical Aβ levels and learning capacity, particularly in the prevention study, and correlations between learning capacity and antibody titer. Moreover, a subset of PDAPP mice with very low Aβ levels (hippocampal Aβ levels of <6000 ng/g or cortical Aβ levels of <1000 ng/g) was indistinguishable from non-Tg controls. Mice in the prevention study were also rescued from cognitive impairment more effectively than those in the reversal study. The combination of variability in antibody response and differential levels of Aβ accumulation across the population of immunized PDAPP mice may be responsible for success in cognitive protection with only a subset of these animals, but the similarity to the findings of certain human vaccination trials is noteworthy.
Introduction
Several animal models of Alzheimer's disease (AD) have been developed to help understand the neural mechanisms of the disease process and to provide platforms for testing novel therapeutics (Bartus et al., 1982; Price et al., 1992; Sisodia, 1992; Games et al., 1995; Duff et al., 1996; Hsiao et al., 1996; Bartus, 2000; Sommer et al., 2000). Because abnormal accumulation of β-amyloid (Aβ), Aβ oligomers, and/or amyloid plaque deposition are each held to play distinctive roles in the neurodegeneration that characterizes various stages of AD, Aβ has become a particularly important therapeutic target (Hardy and Allsop, 1991; Selkoe, 1991, 2002).
Schenk et al. (1999) made the novel suggestion of immunizing against the accumulation of Aβ. They observed that amyloid plaque deposition in old PDAPP mice could be prevented when Aβ42 was administered monthly from 2 to 13 months of age and significantly reduced when administered from 11 months of age for 7 months. Aβ antibodies apparently cross the blood–brain barrier, bind to plaques, and activate microglial cells that clear amyloid plaques (Bard et al., 2000). This process has been visualized in the living mouse brain (Bacskai et al., 2001). Alternatively, binding to Aβ antibodies might generate a peripheral sink (DeMattos et al., 2001), changing the equilibrium between the neurons and plasma to aid the removal of Aβ. Active and passive Aβ immunotherapy have since received widespread attention for possible use in humans (Mattson and Chan, 2003; Nicoll et al., 2003). A “double-blind” clinical trial that was suspended prematurely because of meningoencephalitis in 6% of the patients (Schenk, 2002; Orgogozo et al., 2003) revealed reduced plaque deposition in brain tissue (Nicoll et al., 2003) and a positive relationship between antibody titer and a lower rate of decline of cognitive function (Gilman et al., 2005).
Several preclinical studies of the impact of immunization on learning and memory in amyloid precursor protein (APP) transgenic mice have shown positive effects. TgCRND8 mice (expressing a mutant human APP695 gene) and non-Tg controls were immunized with Aβ42 from 6 weeks of age for 14 weeks (Janus et al., 2000). A longitudinal design was used with a standard water maze spatial reference memory task. Although still impaired relative to non-Tg controls, Aβ-immunized Tg mice escaped significantly faster than controls during acquisition and showed reduced Aβ plaque deposition. However, neither genotype nor immunization affected posttraining probe tests that measure spatial memory. Morgan et al. (2000) also used a longitudinal design with a radial-arm water maze with untreated non-Tg, Aβ-immunized, or keyhole limpet hemocyanin (KLH)-immunized Tg mice. Before treatment, all three groups displayed good learning at 11.5 months. The animals were retested at 15.5 months after five inoculations at monthly intervals of either Aβ or KLH. The Aβ-immunized Tg mice showed successful learning almost but not quite as good as the non-Tg mice. The improved rate of learning was explained in terms of reduced levels of soluble Aβ in the brain, but the number of subjects per group was small. In studies using passive immunization with Aβ antibodies, positive effects have been seen in object recognition memory in 24-month-old PDAPP mice (Dodart et al., 2002) and in water-maze learning using other transgenic mouse models of AD (Kotilinek et al., 2002; Billings et al., 2005; Hartman et al., 2005; Maier et al., 2006; Wilcock et al., 2006).
The aim of this study was twofold. First, using a large cohort of mice to ensure adequate statistical power, we tested whether active Aβ immunization could prevent and/or rescue the age-related learning deficit that is observed in PDAPP mice using a serial spatial learning task (Chen et al., 2000). This is a useful new behavioral protocol because, like the radial water-maze task, it engages both encoding and retrieval of memory each day, analogous to the episodic memory tasks that are susceptible in early AD. Second, like the clinical study of Gilman et al. (2005), we also examined whether any cognitive rescue would be related to the impact of immunization on Aβ levels and/or Aβ titer responses. We anticipated that there may be such a relationship because variation in Aβ levels in PDAPP mice that massively overexpress mutant APP might not always result in immunization-associated rescue in every animal. In experiment 1 (“prevention”), Aβ immunization of both PDAPP and non-Tg mice was started at 7 months and given at monthly intervals for 9 months. In experiment 2 (“reversal”), PDAPP mice were immunized from 11 months of age for 5 months. After these treatments with Aβ42 plus the adjuvant MPL-AF or adjuvant only, serial spatial learning was examined at 15–16 months together with later measurements of Aβ levels and Aβ titer responses.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
All mice (n = 150) were age-matched littermates. The founder of PDAPP mice was generated on a Swiss Webster × B6D2F1 (C57BL/6 × DBA/2) background (Games et al., 1995). They were bred with mice of the same background for three generations. Generation 3 was bred with B6D2F1. Generation 4 was then bred with Swiss Webster to generate heterozygous animals used for the present study. All PDAPP mice were non-white heterozygous males. In the prevention study (experiment 1), 67 mice were tested (PDAPP immunized, n = 17; PDAPP adjuvant only, n = 17; non-Tg immunized, n = 17; non-Tg adjuvant only, n = 16). In the reversal study (experiment 2), 83 mice were tested (PDAPP immunized, n = 24; PDAPP adjuvant only, n = 18; non-Tg immunized, n = 22; non-Tg adjuvant only, n = 19). The mice were group housed (two to four per cage). All phases of the study were conducted blind: the experimenter (G.C.) was blind to both genotype and treatment status, the preliminary data analyses was conducted by someone unconnected with the daily training of the animals (S.J.M.), the posttraining immunological analyses conducted at Elan Pharmaceuticals (South San Francisco, CA) was blind to the behavioral data, and the project leader (R.G.M.M.) was blind to the outcome until all testing was complete.
The water maze.
The open-field water maze is 2 m in diameter and set into a large laboratory room with prominent two- and three-dimensional extramaze cues. The water is filled and drained daily, maintained at 25°C, and made opaque by the addition of 250 ml of latex liquid (Cementone-Beaver, Buckingham, UK). Escape from the water was provided by a single moveable platform of 20 cm diameter (ratio of pool to platform area of 100:1) that was normally hidden 1.5 cm beneath the water surface but, during the initial cue training, rendered visible by the addition of a prominent local cue. The mice were gently placed into the water facing the side walls and allowed to swim for up to 90 s to find and climb onto the platform. The starting position was randomized and counterbalanced. Once found, they stayed there for 30 s and were then removed by allowing them to jump onto a small paint roller for return to their “transport cage” in which they were placed under a heat lamp. Several mice were trained successively in a group enabling intertrial intervals to be 10 min. For each location trained, the escape platform was in any one of 20 different locations (two series of 10 counterbalanced platform locations). The swimming movements of the mice were tracked using in-house software (Actimetrics Watermaze; Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA), which provides objective, automated video tracking of the animals and a suite of relevant analysis options, including swim path, time spent in target quadrant, thigmotaxis, etc.
Overview of experimental protocol.
The two experiments were procedurally identical and consisted of the following phases: (1) immunization; (2) shipping of the animals to Edinburgh; (3) visible platform training (5 d); (4) serial spatial learning with a hidden platform (10–20 d); (5) ELISAs for total Aβ and Aβ42 quantification; and (6) Aβ antibody titer response measurement.
Phase 1: immunization of the mice.
Aβ peptide was freshly prepared from lyophilized powder for each set of injections. For Aβ immunizations, 2 mg of human Aβ42 (US Peptides, Rancho Cucamonga, CA) was added to 0.9 ml of deionized water, and the mixture was vortexed to generate a relatively uniform suspension. A 100 ml of aliquot of 10× PBS (in which 1× PBS is 0.15 m NaCl and 0.01 m sodium phosphate, pH 7.5) was added. The suspension was vortexed again and incubated overnight at 37°C for use the next day. Aβ42 at 50 mg and 25 mg of the adjuvant MPL-AF in 200 ml of PBS were used for each subcutaneous injection. Injections of Aβ42 and MPL-AF or of MPL-AF alone were made on days 0, 14, and 28 and then once every 4 weeks.
Phase 2: shipping.
The mice were shipped by air from San Francisco to Edinburgh with a journey time of <24 h, followed by 1 week (minimum) of acclimatization to the new housing conditions and diet, as required by United Kingdom Project License requirements to minimize stress.
Phase 3: visible cue task training.
There were 5 d of training, four trials per day, with the platform location changed across trials. Curtains were drawn around the pool to exclude the extramaze cues. The platform was made visible by putting a bronze cylinder on it (15 cm high and 3 cm in diameter). If a mouse failed to get to the platform after 90 s had elapsed, it was guided to the visible platform by hand.
Phase 4: serial spatial reversal learning with a hidden escape platform.
The animals were trained for eight trials per day on a series of spatial tasks (“problems”) each consisting of finding the hidden escape platform in the water maze. Training continued until three successive trials occurred with a mean escape latency of <20 s, a criterion that might be reached within 1 d or might take >1 d to be reached (Fig. 1). Once criterion was reached, training for that day stopped and the animals were switched to a new problem (i.e., new platform location) starting on the next day. Training continued until a minimum of five separate problems had been learned or 10 d of training, whichever took longer. Two performance measures were derived. “Trials-to-criterion” is a quantitative assessment of the number of trials, inclusive of the criterion run, taken to learn each of the first five spatial locations (as in Fig. 1). “Learning capacity” is a measure of the number of separate locations that an individual mouse learned in 10 d of training. Both measures show comparable sensitivity to age-related changes in PDAPP mice in cross-sectional and longitudinal experimental protocols (Chen et al., 2000).
Serial spatial learning task. The main task is training on a series of five separate spatial locations (shown), followed by additional training for a total of 10 d (data not shown). For each problem, the hidden platform is in a different location of the pool. The trials-to-criterion data are based on this set of five problems. Swim paths are shown for the first trial of training and for the three criterion trials of each problem only.
Phase 5: ELISA analysis of total Aβ and Aβ42.
All 150 mice tested in the water maze were killed by cervical dislocation. The brain was quickly removed and put into ice-cold artificial CSF. Two brain areas, hippocampus and cortex, were dissected, weighed, and then put into Eppendorf tubes for homogenization. Guanidine buffer at 5 m was added to each tube, and the tissue was completely homogenized with a hand-held homogenizer. The brain homogenates were further diluted 1:10 with ice-cold casein buffer (0.25% casein, 0.05% sodium azide, 20 μg/ml aprotinin, 5 mm EDTA, pH 8.0, and 10 μg/ml leupeptin in PBS) before centrifugation (16,000 × g for 20 min at 4°C).
The “total” Aβ sandwich ELISA consists of the capture antibody 266, which is specific to amino acids 13–28 of Aβ, and the biotinylated reporter antibody 3D6, which is specific to amino acids 1–5 of Aβ. The Aβ42 sandwich ELISA uses the capture monoclonal antibody 21F12 (Aβ33–42). Biotinylated 3D6 is also the reporter antibody in this assay. The 266 and 21F12 monoclonal antibodies were coated at 10 μg/ml into 96-well immunoassay plates (Costar, Cambridge, MA) overnight at room temperature (RT). The plates were then aspirated and blocked with 0.25% human serum albumin in PBS buffer for at least 1 h at RT and then stored desiccated at 4°C until use. The plates were rehydrated with wash buffer (0.05% Tween 20 in Tris-buffered saline) before use. The samples and standards were added to the plates and incubated at RT for 1 h. The plates were washed three or more times with wash buffer between each step of the assay. The biotinylated 3D6, diluted to 0.5 μg/ml in casein assay buffer (0.25% casein and 0.05% Tween 20, pH 7.4, in PBS), was incubated in the wells for 1 h at RT. Avidin–horseradish peroxidase (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA), diluted 1:4000 in casein assay buffer, was added to the wells for 1 h at RT. The colorimetric substrate slow 3,30,5,59-tetramethyl benzidine (TMB) ELISA (Pierce, Rockford, IL) was added and allowed to react for 15 min, after which the enzymatic reaction was stopped with addition of 1 m H2SO4. Reaction product was quantified using a Molecular Devices (Palo Alto, CA) Vmax spectrophotometer measuring the difference in absorbance at 450 and 650 nm.
Phase 6: ELISA analysis for serum antibodies.
Blood samples were collected from all of the 150 mice behaviorally tested. Titers were determined by serial dilutions of sera against aggregated Aβ42 that had been coated onto microtiter wells. Detection used goat antimouse Ig conjugated to horseradish peroxidase and slow TMB (Pierce) substrate. Titers were defined as the dilution yielding 50% of the maximal signal. All samples and standards were assayed in duplicates.
Data analysis.
All behavioral and ELISA data were analyzed using ANOVAs and analyses of covariation. An important feature was an attempt to relate behavioral performance to levels of Aβ and antibody titer in individual animals.
Results
The animals arrived after the shipment in good health and were required to have a minimum period of 1 week acclimatization to the laboratory in Scotland before testing commenced. All mice quickly adapted to the new housing conditions without difficulty and were housed in groups consisting of three to four littermate males. Animals (n = 150) that swam well and completed the experiment were included in this report. A small number of animals, one to two per group and in all groups, were identified as poor swimmers and removed from the study.
Cue task performance
Immunization did not affect cue task learning in PDAPP or non-Tg mice. Figure 2 shows the decline in mean escape latency from between 23 and 45 s on day 1 to mean latencies of 5–7 s by day 5 in all groups of both the prevention (F = 96.2; df 1.41/89.0; p < 0.001, Greenhouse–Geisser correction) and reversal (F = 105.9; df 1.76/138.9; p < 0.001) experiments. Statistical analysis revealed no significant immunization effect in either study (F = 1.58; df = 1/63; p > 0.2 for prevention; F < 1 for reversal), but the PDAPP groups were significantly slower to escape overall (p values <0.01). Highly significant groups × genotype interactions for both the prevention and reversal studies (p values <0.005) reflected poorer cue task performance of the PDAPP mice on day 1 (p values <0.01) but little difference thereafter. All mice were escaping rapidly and consistently by days 4 and 5. Thus, although there was a transitory impairment, PDAPP mice displayed no lasting deficit in sensorimotor function or motivation to learn.
Visible platform learning. A, There was no difference in escape latency between the PDAPP and non-Tg groups in the prevention study. B, There was a deficit in escape latency in the PDAPP groups on day 1 but not thereafter in the reversal study.
Serial spatial learning
When transferred to the serial spatial reversal task, PDAPP mice showed a consistent learning deficit in both the trials-to-criterion and the learning capacity measures of performance. Figure 3, A and B (performance on five individual spatial locations) and C and D (mean trials to criterion), shows the performance of all mice on the first five platform locations that were used to compute the trials-to-criterion measure. For the prevention study, an ANOVA revealed a main transgene effect (F = 13.4; df = 1/63; p < 0.001) but no overall effect of immunization (F < 1). Similarly, in the reversal study, the ANOVA again revealed a main transgene effect (F = 29.7; df = 1/79; p < 0.001) but again no overall immunization effect (F < 1).
Serial spatial learning: trials to criterion measure. Unlike conventional measures of water-maze spatial learning (which use escape latency), the measure of performance is the number of trials taken to learn each spatial problem across five successive spatial problems. A, B, Trials to criterion for the prevention study (experiment 1). Plot of performance for each individual platform (A) and of mean performance across the five locations (B). Overall, the PDAPP groups were impaired, and there was apparently no effect of immunization. C, D, Trials to criterion for the reversal study. Plot shows performance for each individual platform and of mean performance across the five locations. Again, the PDAPP groups were impaired, and there was no effect of immunization.
The effects of Aβ immunization on spatial learning were also examined using the learning capacity measure, the maximum number of locations learned in 10 d (Fig. 4). In terms of total number of platform locations learned in 10 d, PDAPP mice were significantly impaired relative to non-Tg mice (prevention, F = 16.9, df = 1/63, p < 0.001; reversal, F = 46.5, df = 1/79, p < 0.001). However, no overall effect of immunization was found (F values <1). These results confirm and extend the findings obtained with the trials-to-criterion measure of performance over the first five problems trained. Thus, at this stage of the analysis, the data appeared to indicate that Aβ immunization of PDAPP mice for periods of either 5 or 9 months did not rescue the age-related impairment in serial spatial learning relative to non-Tg controls. However, comparison of the performance of Aβ42-immunized animals in the prevention and reversal studies revealed better behavioral performance of the PDAPP mice that had received the longer period of Aβ42 treatment (F = 5.97; df = 1/39; p < 0.025), our first indication that this overall picture was deceptive.
Serial spatial reversal learning: learning capacity measure. Additional training continues for a full 10 d. The learning capacity measure is the total number of spatial locations (problems) learned by each animal over this time. A, Prevention study. B, Reversal study. The PDAPP mice were impaired, and there was no apparent impact of immunization.
Aβ levels
Immunization was successful in lowering Aβ levels. After completion of behavioral testing, Aβ levels in the brain were measured using ELISA. Table 1 shows the total Aβ and Aβ42 levels in the cortex (top) and hippocampus (bottom) of PDAPP mice that were subject to Aβ immunization or MPL-AF only. The pattern across groups is similar for both total Aβ and Aβ42. Analysis of the two experiments together, with immunization and treatment period as separate factors, revealed that Aβ immunization caused a striking reduction in total Aβ levels in both the cortex (F = 17.9; df = 1/72; p < 0.001) and hippocampus (F = 9.2; df = 1/72; p < 0.005) and in Aβ42 levels in both the cortex (F = 24.2; df = 1/75; p < 0.001) and hippocampus (F = 11.1; df = 1/75; p < 0.005) compared with that in age-matched MPL-AF-treated PDAPP mice. The longer period of immunization in the prevention study (9 months) resulted in a lower total Aβ level than that in the reversal study (5 months) in both cortex (F = 10.1; df = 1/72; p < 0.005; 65.2 vs 37.8%) and hippocampus (F = 7.1; df = 1/72; p < 0.05; 45.9 vs 19.5%) and a lower level of Aβ42 in both the cortex (F = 6.6; df = 1/72; p < 0.05; 54.6 vs 45.2%) and hippocampus (F = 7.1; df = 1/72; p < 0.05; 47.0 vs 22.9%). The absolute Aβ levels for the hippocampus are higher than for the cortex. These overall reductions in Aβ levels are substantial and highly significant. Thus, the results from this large-scale study indicate that active Aβ immunization effectively suppresses Aβ accumulation in the brain of PDAPP mice subjected to behavioral training.
Total Aβ and Aβ42 levels in the cortex and hippocampus of PDAPP mice and amyloid titer response
Correlations between Aβ levels and behavioral performance
Despite immunization having no overall effect on the rate at which PDAPP mice could learn, we examined whether there was any correlation between Aβ levels and behavioral performance. Because the serial spatial learning task is hippocampal dependent but may involve information storage in cortex and because high mutant Aβ levels are likely to be correlated with neuronal dysfunction, it was expected that correlations would be seen between the behavioral performance of PDAPP mice and both hippocampal and cortical Aβ. In practice, a more complex pattern emerged. In non-immunized PDAPP mice, there was no relationship between performance and Aβ levels (Fig. 5). However, in treated PDAPP animals in which the overall levels of Aβ failed to accumulate, a relationship began to emerge. In the prevention study (Fig. 5, top row; Table 2), significant correlations were observed between total Aβ levels in both hippocampus and cortex with both trials to criterion (data shown only in Table 2) and learning capacity, the highest value being r = −0.624 between learning capacity and total Aβ in hippocampus (p < 0.01). Because a single data point in Figure 5, A and B, may have been disproportionately contributing to these significant correlations, Spearman's rank correlations were also calculated, and both were still significant (r = −0.552, p < 0.025 for cortical Aβ; and r = −0.563, p < 0.025 for hippocampal Aβ). In the reversal study, a similar trend was apparent (Fig. 5C,D; Table 2A). There was, however, no correlation between performance and Aβ levels in MPL-AF-treated mice (Fig. 5, bottom row; Table 2A).
Correlation between Aβ levels and learning capacity. A, B, Prevention study. Analysis of the relationship between cortical (A) and hippocampal (B) total Aβ levels and learning capacity (LC) revealed a significant negative correlation. That is, animals with very low Aβ levels learned a higher number of platform locations in 10 d. C, D, Reversal study. A similar analysis revealed a trend toward negative correlations between cortical and hippocampal Aβ levels and learning capacity, but these were not significant. E, F, Combined MPL-AF-treated PDAPP animals. No relationships were observed between Aβ levels and performance in adjuvant-treated animals.
Correlation coefficients between Aβ and antibody titer levels and serial spatial learning performance
Effective learning in animals with low Aβ levels
Across the full set of 76 PDAPP mice, there were a number of untreated animals with high Aβ levels who nonetheless displayed a high learning capacity score. This was not observed in the immunized PDAPP group. To the contrary, in this group, the animals that displayed particularly effective learning all had very low total Aβ and Aβ42 levels (Fig. 5). Paradoxically, despite no overall behavioral effect of immunization, there seemed to be (1) a change in the distribution of animals as a function of levels of Aβ levels that (2) might be associated with a subtle but detectable alteration in behavioral phenotype.
Accordingly, in the next step of the analysis, we examined all immunized (n = 41) and untreated (n = 35) PDAPP animals by stratifying them into subgroups reflecting their varied levels of cortical and hippocampal Aβ. A Pearson's χ2 test of the distribution of mice by category as shown in Table 3 was significant (for cortical total Aβ, χ2 = 10.96, p < 0.025; for hippocampal total Aβ, χ2 = 9.82, p < 0.025). More critical is whether the number of mice in the subgroups with lowest Aβ levels differed as a function of treatment. Pearson's χ2 tests for total Aβ levels indicate that immunization produced a significantly larger number of mice with very low levels of Aβ (for the subgroup of <1000 ng/g total Aβ in the cortex: χ2 = 4.02, p < 0.05; for the subgroup of <6000 ng/g total Aβ in the hippocampus: χ2 = 8.05, p < 0.01). Although this merely confirms the effectiveness of immunization in reducing Aβ, this subgrouping by category enabled us to replot the learning capacity data by subgroup, together with the same data for the non-Tg animals (Fig. 6). The nine PDAPP animals that showed cortical total Aβ levels of <1000 ng/g formed a subgroup that learned the series of platform locations at a rate (8.78 ± 0.43 locations in 10 d) that was not significantly different from non-Tg controls (8.99 ± 0.13 locations; NS) (Fig. 6A). A set of orthogonal comparisons for all of the PDAPP animals also revealed that the medium, high, and very high Aβ subgroups did not differ with respect to learning capacity (F < 1) and performed significantly worse than the very low Aβ subgroup (F = 10.9; df = 1/39; p < 0.005). A similar analysis of the 13 PDAPP animals showing hippocampal total Aβ levels of <6000 ng/g also indicated that they were not significantly different from non-Tg controls (Fig. 6B). Orthogonal comparisons again revealed that the medium, high, and very high subgroups did not differ (F < 1), with the very low subgroup (8.31 ± 0.50 locations; n = 13) performing significantly better than these subgroups (F = 9.90; df = 1/39; p < 0.005) and not differing from the non-Tg controls (NS). Thus, immunization unmasks a relationship between Aβ levels and performance that, intriguingly, is not present in the untreated animals.
Distribution of numbers of PDAPP mice into categories as a function of total Aβ levels
Learning capacity as a function of Aβ levels and Aβ titers. A, The PDAPP mice (immunized and untreated) were subdivided with respect to cortical Aβ levels. These data reveal that the subgroup with the very lowest Aβ levels were indistinguishable from non-Tg controls and better than PDAPP mice with higher Aβ levels. B, The PDAPP mice (immunized and untreated) were subdivided with respect to hippocampal Aβ levels. The subgroup with the very lowest Aβ levels was indistinguishable from non-Tg controls. C, The PDAPP mice (immunized) were subdivided with respect to Aβ titers. A relationship between antibody titer and performance was observed, but the subgroup with the highest antibody response was still impaired relative to non-Tg controls. Note an apparently exacerbated impairment in immunized mice with a low antibody titer.
Aβ antibody titer responses and spatial learning
An additional immunological analysis was also conducted on the serum anti-Aβ titer responses in all animals tested in the water maze. Comparison of the immunized animals in the prevention and reversal experiments indicated that the longer period of Aβ treatment produced more anti-Aβ activity than the shorter treatment (F = 29.64; df = 1/75; p < 0.001) (Table 1). Immunized PDAPP mice did not produce more anti-Aβ activity than immunized non-Tg mice (F = 2.87; df = 1/75; p > 0.05). In parallel with their higher antibody titer, the entire cohort of immunized PDAPP animals exhibited a highly significant correlation between antibody titer and learning capacity (Spearman's r = 0.443; p = 0.005) (Table 2). No correlation between titer and learning capacity was observed in non-Tg mice (Spearman's r = 0.116; p > 0.4). Despite the overall relationship, the correlation between antibody titer and learning capacity did not reach significance when immunized PDAPP mice from the prevention and reversal groups were considered separately (Table 2). Although immunized PDAPP mice in the prevention study exhibited lower Aβ levels and higher antibody titers compared with those in the PDAPP reversal study (see above), no relationship was observed between antibody titer and Aβ levels (Table 2).
It has been reported recently that human patients with high Aβ antibody titer responses displayed less cognitive decline over time than non-antibody responders (Gilman et al., 2005). We therefore analyzed spatial learning stratified by Aβ antibody titer responses in all immunized PDAPP mice. We sorted these animals into four categories: high responders (titers > 5000), midrange responders (1000 < titers < 5000), low responders (100 < titers < 1000), and nonresponders (<100). After either 5 or 9 month treatment with AN1792, most mice showed a high or midrange level of anti-Aβ titer response. Only two mice (one in prevention study and one in reversal) displayed <1000 titer responses (low responders), and there were no nonresponders. An ANOVA of learning capacity revealed a significant difference between these three different subgroups of immunized PDAPP animals (F = 5.1; df = 2/38; p < 0.05) (Fig. 6C), indicating that Aβ titer high responders displayed better learning than midrange or low responders. However, the measure does not appear to be that sensitive because the performance of the Aβ titer high-responders group did not differ significantly from that of non-immunized PDAPP mice (p = 0.072) and, additionally, was still slightly poorer than non-Tg controls (p < 0.05, two-tailed t test). These data parallel those obtained using Aβ levels but indicate that a high antibody titer is no guarantee that learning will be fully rescued in PDAPP mice.
Discussion
The main new finding is that a therapeutic rescue of spatial learning can occur in a subset of PDAPP mice in proportion to the apparent effectiveness of immunization in suppressing Aβ accumulation in the brain. This positive finding emerges, however, from a set of intriguingly paradoxical results. Neither short- nor long-term immunization against human Aβ had any overall effect on the rate of learning of a series of spatial tasks. However, we anticipated at the outset that active immunization with Aβ42 was likely to have a varied effect across animals, achieving very low levels of Aβ only in a subset of our transgenic mice. This was observed, notably in animals given long-term treatment beginning at an age before overt amyloid plaque deposition. A significant correlation was then unmasked between both total Aβ and Aβ42 levels and performance. Moreover, subsets of immunized PDAPP mice with very low Aβ levels (n = 9 for cortex, and n = 13 for hippocampus) were behaviorally indistinguishable from wild-type controls. There was no evidence of a relationship between learning ability and Aβ levels in untreated PDAPP mice. These findings are consistent with the notion that an active immunization therapy can be effective in an animal model of AD that involves five to seven times overexpression of human mutant Aβ.
Impact of immunization
The apparent “null result” in the overall analysis of the learning data were not because immunization was ineffective. First, ELISA measurements revealed a significantly lower level of total Aβ and Aβ42 in Aβ-immunized PDAPP compared with MPL-AF-treated PDAPP animals, consistent with Schenk et al. (1999). Second, immunized PDAPP mice showed an antibody titer response. Third, there was significant redistribution of PDAPP mice between the Aβ-treated and MPL-AF-treated groups when sorted into subcategories defined according to both cortical and hippocampal total Aβ levels.
However, although a subset of PDAPP mice with low Aβ levels behaved like wild-type controls in the learning task, there was no overall effect of immunization on cognitive function. This is puzzling because it would be expected that the overall group mean for the treated PDAPP group should show at least a trend toward being different from that of the untreated PDAPP group. The absence of an overall effect of immunization might be explained by the small number of immunized PDAPP mice with moderate to high Aβ levels that performed poorly in the water maze, raising the specter of potential risks to function when using active immunization.
Variation in the effectiveness of active immunization
A separate issue is the lack of a significant correlation between Aβ levels and performance in the control-treated PDAPP mice. However, this is not surprising, and there has been recent interest in distinguishing the potentially deleterious effects of distinct forms on cognitive function. A significant correlation between Aβ levels and cognitive decline is not always found (Arriagada et al., 1992; Hyman and Tanzi, 1992; Gomez-Isla et al., 1997; Giannakopoulos et al., 2003; Guillozet et al., 2003). A possible limitation in interpreting findings from mice with transgenic overexpression of human mutant Aβ is that variation in levels that are massively overexpressed may not be physiologically very meaningful. However, if a treatment such as immunization is successful in limiting the accumulation of Aβ considerably, physiologically meaningful correlations may then be unmasked that could be mechanistically insightful. If Aβ is a causative factor in dysfunction of AD, it is reasonable to expect that “rescue” of cognitive function could only occur if the putative therapy succeeded in reducing Aβ levels massively. This is because any lesser reduction would still leave individual mice with an overexpression of the human mutant gene. On this view, finding rescue in only a subset of PDAPP mice is arguably more consistent with the amyloid cascade hypothesis than findings in which functional rescue is observed without such a large decrease (Hardy and Allsop, 1991; Selkoe, 1991, 2002). That we only observed significant rescue in the prevention study is also consistent with the notion that maintaining very low Aβ levels over a long period is better for maintaining effective cognitive function. Thus, although the number of animals (n = 9 for cortical total Aβ; n = 13 for hippocampal total Aβ) in which these effects were observed is small relative to the total number of immunized PDAPP animals tested (n = 41), there is a consistency about the behavioral and biochemical phenotype that they displayed.
There is a second possibility for our failure to find an overall positive immunization effect. A recent study has shown that a soluble form of Aβ assembly, termed Aβ*56, contributes to cognitive deficits in Tg2576 mice (Lesne et al., 2006). Because our ELISA detected total Aβ, both soluble and insoluble, we do not know whether active Aβ immunization effectively reduces the soluble Aβ oligomers as well. If so, a statistically detectable relationship may become apparent when the levels of this species are reduced to near zero in a subset of mice, i.e., only in those animals that in our ELISAs would have shown very low levels of total Aβ. Our findings are therefore consistent with the emerging idea that different species of Aβ promote neuronal dysfunction in a differential manner. It would be valuable in future “proof-of-concept” treatment studies to include analyses of different oligomeric species of Aβ.
Duration and timing of immunization
Significant rescue of learning capacity occurred in the prevention study with administration of AN1792 at 7 months of age. It is possible that even earlier Aβ immunization, i.e., ∼2 months as by Schenk et al. (1999), might help prevent the functional damage to synapses that has been linked to abnormal accumulation of Aβ (Selkoe, 2002; Kamenetz et al., 2003), whereas later Aβ immunization would be expected to be less effective. Consistent with this idea, active Aβ immunization using 16-month-old presenilin 1/APP Tg mice with four biweekly immunizations revealed no rescue of impaired learning when retesting in a radial-arm water maze was performed at 18 months (Austin et al., 2003).
Comparison with other studies
The statistical power of animal studies requires careful consideration, and we feel ethically justified in having used so many animals to complete this study. Only by running a large study (n = 150) were we able to unmask a subtle impact that Aβ immunization. Recent criticism of some animal therapeutic studies (e.g., models of stroke) has focused on the use of overly small group sizes (Sandercock and Roberts, 2002; Pound et al., 2004). We share this concern about “under-powered” animal studies. The recent report by Billings et al. (2005) using >250 mice is consistent with this perspective.
The variability of Aβ levels in treated PDAPP animals is also relevant. Genetic background can significantly affect Aβ metabolism and Aβ deposition in different mouse strains expressing similar levels of APP holoprotein (Lehman et al., 2003). Thus, the more complex genetic makeup of the triple-cross background strain used with the PDAPP mice may contribute to their variability in Aβ metabolism and deposition and reaction to immunization.
Our findings may seem inconsistent with those of previously published reports for which positive overall effects of immunization were seen with water maze reversal learning, a radial water maze task, and novel object recognition (Janus et al., 2000; Morgan et al., 2000; Dodart et al., 2002; Kotilinek et al., 2002; Billings et al., 2005; Comery et al., 2005; Hartman et al., 2005; Maier et al., 2006; Wilcock et al., 2006). Passive Aβ immunotherapy may be more effective in rescuing cognitive deficits in APP Tg mice (Dodart et al., 2002; Kotilinek et al., 2002; Billings et al., 2005; Comery et al., 2005; Hartman et al., 2005; Wilcock et al., 2006). The discrepancies between our and the other studies may reflect group sizes, which were typically smaller in earlier studies, the presence of an age-independent deficit in PDAPP mice that may be unrelated to Aβ levels, the effectiveness of active versus passive Aβ immunization, and/or the differential sensitivity of various behavioral tasks. The serial spatial learning task (Chen et al., 2000) is, for example, more sensitive in revealing age-related learning deficits than a standard reference memory training protocol (Justice and Motter, 1997). It is for this reason that we have argued for the use of episodic-like tasks such as the radial-arm water maze and serial spatial learning task in phase 1 studies (Morgan et al., 2000; Morris, 2001).
Implications with respect to human studies
Because of the unexpected meningoencephalitis caused by AN1792, the first clinical immunization study of AD patients was halted (Schenk, 2002). Neuropathological observations indicate that immunization is associated with reduced plaques in the human brain (Nicoll et al., 2003). A subset of 20 AN1792-immunized patients that were identified as Aβ titer responders in the Zurich cohort of the AN1792 clinical trial showed a reduced rate of decline of cognitive function (Hock et al., 2003). It has been reported that, although analysis of the full 300 patients of the AN1792 study failed to reveal overall differences between placebo and immunized patients across a range of cognitive tests, a subset of 59 patients that were antibody responders showed a lesser decline, relative to placebo controls, in one neuropsychological test battery (Gilman et al., 2005). In our animal study, we find that a subset of immunized PDAPP mice with very high Aβ titer responses displayed better spatial learning than low Aβ titer responders. Furthermore, our findings suggest that active Aβ immunization can successfully restore spatial learning in a mouse model of AD if the brain Aβ levels fail to accumulate. Together, human and animal studies collectively provide additional grounds for confidence that immunization is a viable approach to novel therapeutics for AD.
Footnotes
-
The animals for this study were supplied by Elan Pharmaceuticals; the remainder of the work was supported by the United Kingdom Medical Research Council.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Richard G. M. Morris, Centre and Division of Neuroscience, University of Edinburgh, 1 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, UK. r.g.m.morris{at}ed.ac.uk