Abstract
The development of olfactory bulb projections that form the lateral olfactory tract (LOT) is still poorly understood. It is known that the septum secretes Slit1 and Slit2 which repel olfactory axons in vitro and that in Slit1−/−;Slit2−/− mutant mice, the LOT is profoundly disrupted. However, the involvement of Slit receptors, the roundabout (Robo) proteins, in guiding LOT axons has not been demonstrated. We show here that both Robo1 and Robo2 receptors are expressed on early developing LOT axons, but that only Robo2 is present at later developmental stages. Olfactory bulb axons from Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− double-mutant mice are not repelled by Slit in vitro. The LOT develops normally in Robo1−/− mice, but is completely disorganized in Robo2−/− and Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− double-mutant embryos, with many LOT axons spreading along the ventral surface of the telencephalon. Finally, the position of lot1-expressing cells, which have been proposed to be the LOT guidepost cells, appears unaffected in Slit1−/−;Slit2−/− mice and in Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mice. Together, our results indicate that Robo1 and Robo2 directly mediate the repulsive activity of Slit receptors on LOT axons, and are required for normal guidance of these axons in vivo.
Introduction
The development of axonal projections has been studied extensively in the olfactory system. Olfactory neurons from the olfactory epithelium send their axons to specific glomeruli in the main olfactory bulb (OB), where they synapse on the dendrites of mitral and tufted cells. These OB neurons then project to the primary olfactory cortex (Schwob and Price, 1984; Shipley and Ennis, 1996; Zou et al., 2001). OB axons are positioned laterally, under the pial surface, and constitute the lateral olfactory tract (LOT). The first mitral cell axons leave the OB at embryonic day 12.5 (E12.5) in mice and the LOT is discernible the following day (Pini, 1993; Sugisaki et al., 1996). Organotypic cocultures of OB and telencephalic vesicles or cell membranes have suggested that LOT axons are guided by short-range cues distributed along their pathway on guidepost cells called “lot” cells (Sugisaki et al., 1996; Hirata and Fujisawa, 1997). Moreover, LOT axons were also shown to be repelled in vitro by diffusible factors secreted by the olfactory epithelium, septum, and olfactory cortex (Pini, 1993; Hu and Rutishauser, 1996; de Castro et al., 1999). Thus, the pathfinding of LOT axons in the telencephalon involves a combination of short- and long-range cues. We have shown previously that the septum-derived repellents for LOT axons are two Slit proteins, Slit1 and Slit2 (Nguyen Ba-Charvet et al., 1999, 2002), but the receptor(s) mediating Slit function in this system have not been fully defined.
The major functional receptors for Slits are members of the roundabout (Robo) receptors. The first roundabout gene, Robo, was identified in Drosophila in a screen for genes regulating midline crossing in the CNS (Seeger et al., 1993). Three Robo genes have been found in Drosophila (Kidd et al., 1998; Rajagopalan et al., 2000a; Simpson et al., 2000b) and mammals (Brose et al., 1999; Li et al., 1999; Yuan et al., 1999; Jen et al., 2004), and one in Caenorhabditis elegans (Zallen et al., 1998). Mammalian Slits can bind to all Robo receptors with comparable affinity (Brose et al., 1999; Li et al., 1999; Sabatier et al., 2004). Robo proteins belong to the Ig superfamily and have five Ig-like domains followed by three fibronectin type III repeats, a transmembrane portion and an intracellular tail containing conserved cytoplasmic motifs. Another protein, Robo4, containing the first two Ig domains has been found only expressed by endothelial cells and plays a role in angiogenesis (Huminiecki et al., 2002; Bedell et al., 2005), but its ability to bind Slit is debated (Suchting et al., 2005). The involvement of Robo receptors in Slit signaling in vivo has been well demonstrated in invertebrates (Kidd et al., 1998; Rajagopalan et al., 2000a,b; Simpson et al., 2000a), in the spinal cord of vertebrates (Sabatier et al., 2004), and in C. elegans (Hao et al., 2001). We show here, using in vitro assays, binding studies, and the phenotypic analysis of Robo1 and Robo2 mouse knock-outs, that Robo1 and Robo2 are the receptors mediating Slit repulsive action on LOT axons.
Materials and Methods
Swiss mice (Janvier, Le Genest Saint Isle, France) were used for binding studies. Slit-deficient mice and Robo-deficient mice were described previously and genotyped by PCR (Plump et al., 2002; Grieshammer et al., 2004; Long et al., 2004) (L. Ma and M. Tessier-Lavigne, unpublished observation). To better visualize LOT axons, Slit1;Slit2 mutant mice were crossed to the ERE mouse line, a trap line in which the lacZ reporter gene was randomly inserted into the genome (a gift from Y. Saga, National Institute of Genetics, Mishima, Japan). The insertion site is still unknown. The day of the vaginal plug was counted as E0 and the day of the birth as postnatal day 0 (P0). Mice were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg). All animal procedures were performed in accordance with institutional guidelines.
Generation of LRR2-hSlit1-AP and LRR2-hSlit2-AP.
To generate the human leucine-rich repeat (LRR) Slit1/2-alkaline phosphatase (AP) fusion proteins (LRR2-hSlit1-AP or LRR2-hSlit2-AP), the second LRR of Slit1 (amino acids 282–512) or Slit2 (amino acids 341–505) was amplified by PCR and cloned between the XhoI and XbaI sites of the AP-Tag5 vector (GenHunter, Nashville, TN).
Binding studies.
Human embryonic kidney 293 cells were transfected with LRR2-hSlit1/2-AP using Lipofectamine 2000 reagent (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, Ca) and grown for 48 h. The supernatant was used directly without additional purification. AP activity was measured as described previously (Flanagan and Leder, 1990) and the presence of the fusion protein in the supernatant confirmed on Western blot with an anti-AP antibody (1:6000; GenHunter). A single band at the expected molecular weight 100 KDa was detected (data not shown).
Binding was performed as described by Kolodkin et al. (1997). Briefly, 20 μm cryosections (from fresh-frozen brains) were fixed for 8 min in cooled 100% methanol. The sections were then washed three times in PBS, 4 mm MgCl2, and incubated in a blocking solution (PBS, 4 mm MgCl2, 10% fetal bovine serum; Invitrogen) for 1 h at room temperature (RT). Four-hundred microliters of LRR2-hSlit1/2-AP supernatant, diluted 1/5 in PBS, were added to the sections and incubated for 2 h. After five washes in 4 mm MgCl2 in PBS at RT, sections were fixed for 2 min with 60% acetone, 4% paraformaldehyde, and 20 mm HEPES, pH 7. After five additional washes in PBS, the sections were incubated at 65°C for 2 h in PBS to inactivate endogenous phosphatases. Sections were washed twice in PBS and incubated in 100 mm Tris, pH 9.5, 100 mm NaCl, and 5 mm MgCl2 for 5 min. Last, the slides were incubated for 2 h at RT in 100 mm Tris, pH 9.5, 100 mm NaCl, and 5 mm MgCl2, containing 300 μg/ml nitroblue tetrazolium (NTB; Roche, Meylan, France) and 250 μg/ml 5-bromo-4-chloro-3-indolyl phosphate (BCIP; Roche).
Immunocytochemistry.
Until E16, embryos were fixed by immersion in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.12 m phosphate buffer, pH 7.4. Older mice were perfused transcardially with 4% PFA, postfixed for 3 h, and then cryoprotected in 10% sucrose. Cryostat sections were blocked in 0.2% gelatin in PBS containing 0.25% Triton X-100 and incubated overnight at RT with goat anti-neuropilin 1 (NP1; R & D Systems, Minneapolis, MN), rat anti-L1 (Millipore, Temecula, CA), goat anti-Robo1 (R & D Systems), goat anti-Robo2 (R & D Systems), rabbit anti-Robo2 (a gift from Dr. Murakami, Osaka, Japan) (Sabatier et al., 2004), goat anti-Robo3 (R & D Systems), rabbit anti-β-galactosidase (MP Biomedicals, Irvine, CA), and rat mAb Lot1 (Sato et al., 1998), followed by species-specific secondary antibodies directly conjugated to fluorophores [cy-3 (Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA) or Alexa Fluor (Invitrogen)]. Sections were examined under a fluorescent microscope (DMR; Leica, Nussloch, Germany).
PLAP staining.
Cryostat section, pretreated just as for immunocytochemistry, were incubated 30 min in PBS at 65°C, buffered 10 min in B3 buffer (0.1 m Tris, pH 9.5, 0.1 m NaCl, 50 mm MgCl2). We then detected human placental alkaline phosphatase (PLAP) activity with 0.1 mg/ml BCIP and 1 mg/ml NBT in B3 buffer. PLAP-stained tissue was then washed in PBS and mounted in Mowiol (Calbiochem, La Jolla, CA).
X-Gal staining.
ERE mouse is a gene-trap line isolated from the random insertion series of the β-galactosidase gene into the mouse genome performed by Dr. Yumiko Saga at the National Institute of Genetics in Japan. The brains of the embryos were dissected and stained using X-Gal by the method of Saga et al. (1992). In the line, most, if not all, of the main olfactory projection neurons were found expressing β-galactosidase (T. Kawasaki and T. Hirata, unpublished observation).
Repulsion assay.
OB explants from E14.5 Robo-deficient, heterozygous, and wild-type littermate embryos were cocultured in collagen gels with aggregates of Slit2-transfected COS-7 cells [transformed African green monkey kidney fibroblast cells (COS cells)] or floor-plate cells from E13 rat embryos as described previously (Nguyen Ba-Charvet et al., 1999). Explants were cultured in neurobasal medium supplemented with B27 for 48 h. At the end of this period, explants were fixed in ice-cold 4% PFA and immunostained with a neuron-specific anti-class III β-tubulin monoclonal antibody (TuJ-1; Covance, Berkeley, CA) for visualization of the axons.
DiI tracing.
The olfactory bulb of E16 to E18 embryos was injected with the lipophilic tracer 1,1′-dioctadecyl-3,3,3′,3′-tetramethylindocarbo-cyanine perchlorate (DiI; Invitrogen) as described previously (de Castro et al., 1999).
Results
Robo1 and Robo2 are expressed in the lateral olfactory tract
To determine whether LOT axons expressed Robo receptors, we first performed an immunohistochemical study. At E13, LOT axons were strongly immunoreactive for Robo2, consistent with our previous in situ hybridization data (Nguyen Ba-Charvet et al., 1999; Marillat et al., 2002) (Fig. 1A). Interestingly, E13 LOT axons were also immunoreactive for Robo1 (Fig. 1B). Neither Robo1 nor Robo2 were expressed on so-called lot cells (see below). Double immunolabeling revealed that Robo1 is expressed by a subset of Robo2-positive LOT axons (Fig. 1C). This expression pattern was unchanged until E15 (data not shown). From E16, we were unable to detect Robo1 protein on LOT axons (Fig. 1D), visualized with an anti-L1 antibody (Fig. 1E,F,H,I) (Inaki et al., 2004), whereas Robo2 was still highly expressed (Fig. 1G). In Robo1-deficient mice, a cassette encoding β-galactosidase (β-gal) and PLAP was inserted in the Robo1 locus (Long et al., 2004). Similarly, β-galactosidase-fused with tau was inserted into the Robo2 locus (Grieshammer et al., 2004). At E16, neither β-galactosidase, nor PLAP could be detected in the LOT of Robo1+/− mice, whereas β-gal was expressed in the LOT of Robo2+/− mice (data not shown). In contrast, β-gal was not expressed by lot cells. Robo3/Rig1 (retinoblastoma inhibiting gene 1) expression was never detected on LOT axons (data not shown). Overall, these results show that early developing LOT axons coexpress Robo2 and Robo1, whereas only Robo2 is expressed on older LOT axons.
LRR2-hSlit bind to LOT axons
A previous structure–function analysis of the LRR domains of Drosophila Slit (Howitt et al., 2004) revealed that the active site of Slit is located on the second of the four LRR (LRR2), which is highly conserved between Slits (Howitt et al., 2004). To attempt to visualize Slit binding sites in the developing mouse brain we generated two fusion proteins consisting of LRR2-hSlit1 or LRR2-hSlit2 (see Materials and Methods) fused to AP and performed binding studies. We found that at E13–E18, both LRR2-hSlit1-AP and LRR2-hSlit2-AP strongly bound to LOT axons (Fig. 2A–C,E). To determine if Slit-AP constructs selectively bound to Robo receptors, we next performed binding on OB and forebrain sections from E13 and E18 Robo1 and Robo2 single and double mutants (see Materials and Methods). We used LRR2-hSlit2-AP, because it is more highly expressed by transfected cells than LRR2-hSlit1-AP. At E13, LRR2-hSlit2-AP binding to LOT axons was observed in Robo1 and Robo2 single mutants, but not in Robo1;Robo2 double mutants (Fig. 2D) (data not shown). At E18, LRR2-hSlit2-AP binding was still intense in wild-type and Robo1−/− LOT axons (Fig. 2E,F), but was absent in Robo2−/− or Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− LOT axons (Fig. 2G) (data not shown). At no time did LRR2-hSlit2-AP bind to LOT cells. These results confirm the immunocytochemical data, and also show that Robo1 and Robo2 are most likely the only major Slit receptors on LOT axons (see Discussion).
Slits do not repel OB axons from Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mutant mice
We next examined whether OB axons from mice deficient for both Robo1 and Robo2 could still respond to Slit chemorepulsive activity. We used E14 OB explants from wild-type, Robo1+/−;Robo2+/−, or Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mutant mice, and cultured them for 24–36 h in collagen gels adjacent to explants of spinal cord floor plate, which is a source of Slit proteins (Hu and Rutishauser, 1996; Hu, 1999) or adjacent to COS cell aggregates expressing Slit2. OB axons were visualized after immunostaining with anti-class III β-tubulin monoclonal antibody TuJ-1, a pan-neuronal marker. As described previously (Nguyen Ba-Charvet et al., 1999), very robust repulsion was observed, with OB axons growing away from the Slit2-secreting COS cells [with a proximal/distal ratio (see Materials and Methods) of 0.196 ± 0.08 (SEM), from six explants] (Fig. 3E) or floor plate explants (0.229 ± 0.09, from eight explants) (Fig. 3B). The behavior of OB axons from Robo1+/−;Robo2+/− mice was similar to that of controls, whether the chemorepulsive activity was from floor plate (0.245 ± 0.12, from four explants) (Fig. 3G) or from Slit2-secreting COS cells (0.313 ± 0.16, from four explants) (Fig. 3H). In contrast, the repulsion was virtually abolished with OB explants from Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mice adjacent to floor-plate explants (0.993 ± 0.31, 10 explants from three distinct double mutants) or Slit2-secreting aggregates (1.12 ± 0.1, nine explants from three distinct double mutants) (Fig. 3C,F). Together, these results indicate that the repulsive responses of OB axons to Slit proteins in vitro requires Robo1 and Robo2 receptors.
LOT axon defects in Slit1:Slit2 and Robo1;Robo2 mutant mice
We observed previously that the LOT is very disorganized in Slit1;Slit2 double-mutant mice, consistent with a role in mediating septal repulsion in vivo (Nguyen-Ba-Charvet et al., 2002). To confirm the involvement of Robo receptors in LOT guidance in vivo, we next analyzed LOT development in Robo1 and Robo2 single- and double-mutant mice. DiI injections in the olfactory bulb of E15 to E18 embryos in wild-type mice (n = 5) (Fig. 4A) always lead to the anterograde tracing of a single axon tract that runs rostrocaudally just under the pial surface, which corresponds to the LOT. We observed the same phenotype using different LOT markers, such as the anti-L1 antibody, which has been shown to label all LOT axons and an antibody against neuropilin-1, which is expressed by all LOT axons, except the deepest ones (Inaki et al., 2004). Both antibodies reveal a thick LOT axon bundle in the lateral telencephalon of wild-type animals (Fig. 5A–C). In Robo1−/− mutant mice (seven of seven cases) or Robo1+/−;Robo2+/− mutant mice (nine of nine cases), the LOT ran normally as a thick axon bundle similar to wild-type (Figs. 4B,C, 5D–F). Nevertheless, in most Robo1−/− mice, the LOT was slightly enlarged (six of seven cases). In contrast, in Robo2−/− mice (two of two cases), the LOT was thinner and spread along the ventrolateral surface of the telencephalon (Figs. 4D, 5G–I), although they still respect their normal dorsal boundary. These defects were even more striking in Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mice (seven of seven cases) at E15.5 (data not shown) as well as at E18: the LOT was very defasciculated into small axonal bundles fanned all over the ventral side of the telencephalon (Fig. 4E,F). On coronal forebrain sections from Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mice, L1-positive axons as well as NP1-positive axons were divided into multiple small bundles scattered from the initial LOT lateral position to the ventral midline (Fig. 5J,K). This phenotype was the same as described previously in Slit1−/−;Slit2−/− mice (Nguyen-Ba-Charvet et al., 2002). Interestingly, in Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mice, a subset of LOT axons, originating from the lateral OB, were still present at their normal position (Fig. 5J–L). This heterogeneous behavior of medial/lateral LOT axons was even more striking in Slit1−/−;Slit2−/− mice injected with DiI in the OB or that expressed β-galactosidase in LOT axons (see Materials and Methods) (Figs. 6, 7A–D) (n = 2). Again, medial OB axons were more severely affected than lateral ones. This also confirmed that LOT axons were specifically misrouted ventrally, but still respect their dorsal boundary. Together, these results show that the pathfinding of the LOT axons, in particular those originating from the medial OB, is profoundly disrupted in Robo2−/− and Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mutant mice.
Lot cell pattern is not perturbed in Slit- or Robo-deficient mice
It has been shown previously that LOT axons may be guided through short-range interaction with so-called “lot cells,” which may act as guidepost cells (Hirata et al., 2001). Lot cells can be visualized with the monoclonal antibody mAb lot1 and migrate ventrally from the dorsal telencephalic vesicle to their final position (Sato et al., 1998; Tomioka et al., 2000). To determine whether the observed LOT defects observed in Slit and Robo mutants results from an abnormal development of lot cells, we studied lot1 expression in Slit1;Slit2-, Robo1-, Robo2-, and Robo1;Robo2-deficient mice. Lot cells confined to the vicinity of the LOT are the most numerous between E14.5 and E16.5 (Sato et al., 1998). At these ages, LOT axons could be detected by anti-neuropilin-1 immunohistochemistry (Sugisaki et al., 1996) (Fig. 7G). In Slit1+/−, as in wild-type mice, lot1-expressing cells were observed surrounding and/or enriched around neuropilin-1-labeled LOT axons (n = 2) (Fig. 8A–C). The same distribution of lot cells was observed in Slit1+/−;Slit2+/− mice (n = 2) (Fig. 8D–F). In Slit1−/−;Slit2+/− mice, although the LOT was obviously thinner (Fig. 8G) (n = 2) and started to divide into bundles (data not shown), the distribution of lot cells was not modified (Fig. 8H). Last, in Slit1−/−;Slit2−/− mice where the LOT was totally disrupted, with few axons still present at their normal position, lot cells were still concentrated at their proper location (n = 5) and were not widespread in the cortex (Figs. 7E–H, 8J–L). However, the density of lot1-positive cells was slightly decreased rostrally, near the ventral part of the OB, where many misrouted LOT axons are found (n = 5/5) (Fig. 7E–H). In E16 Robo1−/− mice (Fig. 9D–F) (n = 1), the same pattern was observed as in wild-type mice (Fig. 9A–C). In Robo2−/− mice, the distribution of lot cells appeared unchanged although the LOT was slightly perturbed, (Fig. 9G,H) (n = 5). In Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mutant mice (n = 2), which exhibit the most striking defects, lot cells were not seen surrounding LOT axons displaced ventromedially, but were still concentrated at their normal lateral position (Fig. 9J–L). Thus, lot cells appear to migrate normally in Slit and Robo mutants, and the action of Slit and Robo on LOT axon pathfinding is likely to be direct.
Discussion
The first diffusible chemorepulsive activity in the vertebrate brain was identified in the rat septum and proposed to guide mitral cell axons (Pini, 1993). Later, we and others showed that this septum-derived repellent activity was caused by a combination of Slit1 and Slit2 that are both highly expressed in the septum from the beginning of LOT development (Li et al., 1999; Nguyen Ba-Charvet et al., 1999, 2002). Nevertheless, some in vitro data using Robo-Fc proteins raised doubt on the role Robo receptors in the guidance of LOT axons (Patel et al., 2001) and direct evidence using Robo-function-blocking reagents was still lacking. Our current results show that Robo1 and Robo2 are the major receptors, present on LOT axons that mediate Slit chemorepulsive activity in vitro and also in vivo. This represents the first direct evidence in vertebrates for a role for Robo receptors in slit-mediated axonal repulsion. This also confirms that Robo receptors play a major role in guiding forebrain axons in vertebrates (Miyasaka et al., 2005; Andrews et al., 2006).
Other studies proposed that the septum repellent and Slit proteins are not essential to guiding LOT axons, but only function in parallel with lot cells. Thus, the removal of the septum or the addition of a Robo1 ectodomain in whole-telencephalon organotypic cultures did not affect the projection of mitral cell axons (Hirata et al., 2001). Our results demonstrate that the distribution of lot1-expressing cells is not significantly perturbed in Slit1-, Slit2-, Slit1;Slit2-, or Robo1-, Robo2- and Robo1;Robo2-deficient mice. Importantly, misrouted LOT axons do not appear surrounded or in contact with lot1-positive cells. This suggests that long-range guidance by Slit molecules and their receptors Robos is preponderant over short-range cues for guiding LOT axons. However, we cannot entirely rule out the possibility that lot cells contribute to the guidance of some LOT axons. First, in Slit1;Slit2 double-deficient mice, the number of lot-1 expressing cells is slightly reduced near the exit point of medial OB axons, suggesting that this subtle disruption of the distribution of lot cells might play a part in the ectopic projection of OB axons. Second, it has been shown previously that in deleted in colorectal cancer (DCC) mutant embryos, the perturbation of lot cell migration leads to defects of LOT axons pathfinding (Kawasaki et al., 2006). It is unlikely in this system that a direct effect of the DCC on the guidance of olfactory axons contributes to the misguidance, as netrin-1 does not exert any effect on OB axons in vitro (de Castro et al., 1999; Li et al., 1999). Third, we show here that a subset of LOT axons originating from the lateral OB still project along the normal LOT pathway along lot cells, and that medial axons are more affected. Non-cell-autonomous factors such as lot cells might be involved in this differential behavior of the lateral and medial OB axons. Together, these results suggest that the LOT contains different type of OB axons that may be guided by distinct molecules.
In addition to Slits and their Robos receptors, developing LOT axons have been proposed to be influenced by other long-range cues of the Semaphorin family. Mitral cell axons express both neuropilin-1 and neuropilin-2. In organotypic cultures, the olfactory epithelium secretes a repellent cue for OB axons (de Castro et al., 1999). This repulsive effect can be mimicked by cells secreting Sema3F, a secreted semaphorin expressed in the olfactory epithelium at the time of LOT development. Furthermore, Sema3B secreted by aggregates of COS cells is able to attract OB axons. Nevertheless, the analysis of the mice deficient for the class 3 semaphorin receptors neuropilin-1 (Kitsukawa et al., 1997) or neuropilin-2 (Chen et al., 2000) showed a totally normal LOT, which does not support a critical role for these semaphorins in LOT axon guidance in vivo. Last, in Robo- and Slit-deficient mice, the preferential growth of misrouted LOT axons toward the septum may be in part caused by the presence of still unidentified attractants released by the ventral telencephalon, whose activity could be unmasked in absence of slit/robo-mediated repulsion.
We and others have shown previously that the N-terminal leucine-rich regions in Slit are sufficient to repel OB axons (Chen et al., 2001; Nguyen Ba-Charvet et al., 2001). More recently, in Drosophila, it has been found that the second leucine-rich domain of Slit is necessary to trigger Robo signaling. We show that in mice, LRR2 is actually sufficient for binding to LOT axons and requires Robo1 and Robo2; it is also sufficient for binding to other Robo expressing forebrain axons (Lopez-Bendito et al., 2007). The finding that LRR2-hSlit1-AP or LRR2-hSlit2-AP binding on forebrain sections is apparently abolished in Robo1−/−;Robo2−/− mice strongly suggests that Robo1 and Robo2 are the only receptors for Slit1 and Slit2 on these axons. Previous studies have shown that other extracellular proteins such as the glycosylphosphatidylinositol-anchored heparan sulfate proteoglycan glypican-1 also bind Slits (Liang et al., 1999; Ronca et al., 2001). However, glypican-1 binds to the C-terminal fragment of Slit, which is dispensable for its chemorepulsive activity (Brose et al., 1999). Cell-surface heparan sulfate is also involved in vitro, in the repulsive activity of Slit on the postnatal migrating subventricular zone (Hu, 2001). Our results suggest that, in vivo, Slit directly binds to Robo receptors on embryonic forebrain axons, but do not exclude that heparan sulfate proteoglycans could still play a role in modulating Slit/Robo signaling in postnatal or adult forebrain neurons.
It has been shown previously that, as in Drosophila, there may be a Robo code for lateral positioning of commissural axons in mice (Long et al., 2004). Robo1-expressing commissural axons are concentrated both in the ventral and lateral funiculi, whereas Robo2-expressing axons are mainly in the lateral funiculus. LOT axons are also organized according to a mediolateral pattern: early born mature mitral cells project their axons into the middle lamina of the LOT, whereas late-born immature mitral cells send their axons into the superficial lamina (Inaki et al., 2004). We observed that Robo1 is only expressed in the LOT until E15, whereas Robo2 is expressed in present in the all LOT axons, all along the development. Moreover, Robo1 is more highly expressed in a superficial subpopulation of Robo2-positive LOT axons (Fig. 1C). This suggests that Robo1 and Robo2 are part of a complex molecular system required to build the laminar organization of the LOT.
In conclusion, the detailed molecular mechanism of LOT formation is very complex and still needs to be thoroughly analyzed. For example, the factors controlling the growth of LOT axons along the rostrocaudal axis still have to be identified. This behavior is not affected in Slit1;Slit2-deficient mice, nor in Robo1;Robo2-deficient mice. At later ages, LOT axons send collaterals branches to the olfactory cortex (Hirata and Fujisawa, 1999). These collateral branches are the only connections of mitral and tufted cell axons with the olfactory cortex. So far, only Anosmin-1, a secreted protein, defective in the X chromosome-linked form of Kallmann syndrome has been shown to promote the branching of OB axons (Soussi-Yanicostas et al., 2002). However, Slit proteins are also possible candidates because Slit2 regulates the branching of sensory axons from the dorsal root ganglia (Wang et al., 1999) and Slit1 the branching of pyramidal cell dendrites in the neocortex (Whitford et al., 2002). Unfortunately the possible branching activity of Slits on OB axons is difficult to assess in the Slit1;Slit2- or Robo1;Robo2-deficient mice, because the growth of LOT axons is very disturbed and these double mutants die at birth. Our results also indicate that some guidance cues act on all LOT axons, but a previous study (Zou et al., 2001) has clearly shown that the projection from the OB to the olfactory cortex is topographically organized and that mitral cells innervated by olfactory receptor neurons from distinct zones of the olfactory epithelium (expressing distinct olfactory receptors) project to distinct regions in the olfactory cortex. The factors that regulate the formation of this complex projection map remain unknown.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by a grant from the Association pour la Recherche sur le Cancer to A.C. We thank Dr. Yumiko Saga (National Institute of Genetics, Mishima Japan) for providing the gene-trap mouse line.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Alain Chédotal, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique Unité Mixte de Recherche 7102, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Equipe Développement Neuronal, Case 12, 9 quai Saint-Bernard, 75005 Paris, France. alain.chedotal{at}snv.jussieu.fr