Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

Human Peripersonal Space: Evidence from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

Tobias Schicke
Journal of Neuroscience 4 April 2007, 27 (14) 3616-3617; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0733-07.2007
Tobias Schicke
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Numerous studies have investigated how information about the position of a target object perceived through the senses is converted into motor commands, so that an effector can act toward this target. One of the challenges the brain faces in solving this task is the conversion of spatial coordinates initially perceived with reference to the sensory organs (e.g., with respect to the retina in vision or with respect to the head in audition) into coordinates that guide the chosen effector, usually the hand, toward those coordinates. In the early 1990s, neurons were discovered in the macaque monkey brain that have a tactile receptive field (RF) centered on the hand or face and a visual RF around the location of the tactile RF (Colby et al., 1993; Graziano et al., 1994). Importantly, these neurons often respond to a visual stimulus only if it is no more than ∼30 cm away from the hand. These neurons therefore seem to represent only the space close around the body, the so-called “peripersonal” or “near” space. Furthermore, the visual RF of these neurons follows the hand when it is moved; in other words, visual stimuli near the hand are coded by these neurons with respect to the hand, not the eyes. The firing rate of these neurons is influenced both by vision of a fake monkey arm and by displacement of the unseen real arm, indicating that both visual and proprioceptive signals are used to determine arm position (and, thus, stimulus location with respect to the arm).

Evidence that such a representation of peripersonal space also exists in the human brain has mainly involved purely behavioral testing in healthy participants (Maravita et al., 2003) or neuropsychological studies of patients suffering from hemispatial neglect. These patients sometimes show either an amelioration or aggravation of their neglect syndrome when a stimulus is presented in their peripersonal compared with their extrapersonal space (Ladavas, 2002).

In a recent study published in The Journal of Neuroscience, Makin et al. (2007) provide compelling evidence for a representation of peripersonal space in humans using functional magnetic resonance imaging in healthy participants. In a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design, the authors presented a small ball near and far from the left hand with respect to both visual and proprioceptive information [Makin et al. (2007), their Fig. 1 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/27/4/731/F1)]. The stimulus was presented near the left thigh. In one condition, the hand was placed visibly on the left thigh, putting the hand near the stimulus both visually and proprioceptively. In a second condition, the hand was placed on the shoulder, such that the stimulus at the thigh was now far from the hand both visually and proprioceptively. In a third condition, the hand placed on the thigh was occluded by a cardboard shield (near was signaled only by proprioception, because visual information about the hand was absent). Finally, in a fourth condition, the participant's hand was placed on his or her shoulder, while a prosthetic (dummy) hand was placed on her thigh (vision near, proprioception far). All four of these conditions were also run with a far stimulus presented 70 cm toward the feet from the thigh stimulus, allowing for a comparison of near versus far stimuli in all conditions in which at least one sense signaled the thigh stimulus to be near. Because peripersonal neurons have both visual and tactile RFs, in a separate, purely tactile part of the experiment, Makin et al. (2007) touched the participant's hand with the stimulus while the participant closed his or her eyes.

Increased activity to the near over the far stimulus, when both vision and proprioception signaled stimulus proximity relative to the hand, was found in the ventral promoter cortex, the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and the lateral occipital complex (LOC) [Makin et al. (2007), their Fig. 3 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/27/4/731/F3)]. However, posterior areas (LOC and posterior IPS) were activated more by the near than the far stimulus when the hand was visually close to the stimulus (i.e., “hand on thigh” and “rubber hand on thigh” conditions) [Makin et al. (2007), their Fig. 5 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/27/4/731/F5)]. These areas were not (although just barely nonsignificant; p = 0.07) modulated by proprioceptive information (i.e., their activation to a near stimulus was similar to that of a far one in the “hand on thigh but covered” and “hand on shoulder” conditions).

In contrast, the anterior IPS was significantly more activated by a near stimulus in the hand on thigh but covered condition, in which proximity to the stimulus was only signaled proprioceptively, albeit at a significance level of only 0.03 (compare with the nonsignificance of 0.07 of the same condition in the posterior IPS) [Makin et al. (2007), their Fig. 6 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/27/4/731/F6)]. At the same time, the rubber arm condition (vision near only) did not show a significant far–near difference, so that the relative influence of vision and proprioception differed between posterior and anterior IPS. In addition, only the anterior IPS responded also to the purely tactile stimulation.

The authors interpret their results to indicate a caudorostral gradient with respect to the kind of information used to determine the spatial relation of the stimulus to the hand. In LOC and the posterior IPS, perihand space is defined primarily through vision, whereas in more anterior parts of the parietal lobe and in the frontal lobe, information about perihand space is formed from vision, proprioception, and somatosensation. However, the small statistical difference for a proprioceptive influence in posterior and anterior IPS (p = 0.07 vs 0.03) suggests that this interpretation should be affirmed by a replication of these results. Furthermore, at least neck-proprioceptive information reaches the part of IPS subsumed here as posterior in macaques (Snyder et al., 1998).

The parietal and frontal areas reported by Makin et al. (2007) to be involved in the representation of peripersonal space are well in line with neurophysiological data from macaques and with imaging studies concerned with action planning and hand–object manipulation in humans. Therefore, the question of which sense, vision or proprioception, dominates the determination of hand position not withstanding, this study provides compelling evidence for a human homolog of the monkey network representing peripersonal, or at least perihand, space. It remains to be determined whether the areas reported here are primarily responsible for the hand alone and subserve mainly hand action or whether they mediate the peripersonal space around the rest of the body as well. This question has not been exhaustively answered even in the monkey brain.

Maybe the most intriguing result is the involvement of visual area LOC, which is located in the ventral (“what”) rather than the dorsal (“where”) stream; the ventral stream has so far not been linked to the representation of peripersonal space, let alone shown to be modulated by hand position with respect to a stimulus. This study is therefore a good example of how human neuroimaging can base a hypothesis on neurophysiological work in monkeys, as well as provide data to create new hypotheses and research questions, which may be elucidated by neurophysiological recording or functional imaging.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short reviews of a recent paper in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to mimic the journal clubs that exist in your own departments or institutions. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Tobias Schicke, Biological Psychology and Neuropsychology, University of Hamburg, Von-Melle-Park 11, 20146 Hamburg, Germany. tobias.schicke{at}uni-hamburg.de

References

  1. ↵
    1. Colby CL,
    2. Duhamel JR,
    3. Goldberg ME
    (1993) Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: anatomic location and visual response properties. J Neurophysiol 69:902–914.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Graziano MS,
    2. Yap GS,
    3. Gross CG
    (1994) Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science 266:1054–1057.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Ladavas E
    (2002) Functional and dynamic properties of visual peripersonal space. Trends Cogn Sci 6:17–22.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Makin TR,
    2. Holmes NP,
    3. Zohary E
    (2007) Is that near my hand? Multisensory representation of peripersonal space in human intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci 27:731–740.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Maravita A,
    2. Spence C,
    3. Driver J
    (2003) Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach. Curr Biol 13:R531–R539.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Snyder LH,
    2. Grieve KL,
    3. Brotchie P,
    4. Andersen RA
    (1998) Separate body- and world-referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex. Nature 394:887–891.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 27 (14)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 27, Issue 14
4 Apr 2007
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Human Peripersonal Space: Evidence from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Human Peripersonal Space: Evidence from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Tobias Schicke
Journal of Neuroscience 4 April 2007, 27 (14) 3616-3617; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0733-07.2007

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Human Peripersonal Space: Evidence from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Tobias Schicke
Journal of Neuroscience 4 April 2007, 27 (14) 3616-3617; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0733-07.2007
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Early Neural Development of Social Interaction Perception in the Superior Temporal Sulcus
  • Universal Coding for Uncertainty?
  • Beyond Motor Control: Diffusion MRI Reveals Associations between the Cerebello-VTA Pathway and Socio-affective Behaviors in Humans
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.