Abstract
Neural development is especially vulnerable to environmental influences during periods of neurogenesis and rapid maturation. In fact, short periods of environmental manipulations confined to embryonic development lead to significant changes in morphology and function. A guiding principal emerging from studies of sensory systems is that experimentally induced effects are most dramatic in higher neural levels (e.g., cortex) and primarily involve postnatal synaptic refinements. In contrast to other sensory systems, the gustatory system is particularly susceptible to the effects of deprivation much earlier and with profound changes evident in the brainstem. Here we show that feeding pregnant rats a custom diet featuring a low-sodium content for 9 d before the tongue appears in the fetus produces extensive restructuring of the gustatory brainstem. Rats born to mothers fed the custom diet from embryonic day 3 (E3) to E12 have terminal field volumes of the greater superficial petrosal, chorda tympani, and glossopharyngeal nerves at adulthood that are expanded as much as 10 times beyond that found in rats fed a standard rat chow. The widespread alterations are not attributable to increased numbers of nerve cells, increased target size, or obvious changes in peripheral taste function. Moreover, we show that the limited period of feeding the custom diet has much larger effects than if rats were fed the diet to postweaning ages. Our results suggest that early periods of altered experience, especially during nucleus of the solitary tract neurogenesis, leads to a restructuring of the gustatory brainstem, which in turn may impact the control of sensory and homeostatic processes.
Introduction
Abnormal events occurring during postnatal critical periods of sensory development lead to central morphological and functional changes (Hubel and Wiesel, 1970; Moore, 1985; Lasiter and Kachele, 1990; Cummings and Brunjes, 1997; Knudsen, 1999; Maier et al., 2003) that can often be recovered after reintroduction of the appropriate sensory stimulus at the appropriate time (Cummings et al., 1997; Dupont et al., 2001; Liao et al., 2002). Early prenatal critical periods also exist in which environmental alterations have extensive effects on the fetal brain that persist into postnatal development and adulthood. In fact, early limited periods of acute ethanol exposure and protein restriction can result in more pronounced effects than extended periods of deprivation (Gressens et al., 1997, 2001). The degree of susceptibility to abnormal environmental events most likely relates to specific developmental processes occurring during each critical period.
The gustatory system is also susceptible to experimental manipulation during discrete periods of development. However, unlike other sensory systems where neural alterations are most profound in cortex, large-scale changes occur in the gustatory brainstem (Hill et al., 1983; King and Hill, 1991, 1993; Lasiter, 1991; Vogt and Hill, 1993; May and Hill, 2006). In fact, introducing a custom diet featuring a low-sodium content from embryonic day 3 (E3) to E12 is sufficient to induce ∼50% enlargement of the chorda tympani (CT) nerve terminal field in the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) compared with rats fed a nutritionally balanced commercial diet (Krimm and Hill, 1997). This period of E3–E12 is before formation of the tongue (Mistretta, 1972) and before the extension of processes from ganglion cells toward their peripheral and central targets (Mbiene and Mistretta, 1997; Zhang and Ashwell, 2001a) but overlaps with the period of generation of presumptive cells destined for the NTS (E11–E14 in rat) (Altman and Bayer, 1982). Thus, substantial alterations in the diet during early development may play a role in NTS development that is exclusive of peripheral gustatory influences such as neural activity.
To test the hypothesis that dietary manipulations occurring during an early, limited period results in a greater restructuring of the terminal field organization in the gustatory NTS compared with feeding the diet long term, we used a triple anterograde labeling technique to examine the morphological characteristics of three gustatory nerve terminal fields and their overlap. We compared adult rat terminal fields in three groups: rats fed the custom diet featuring a low-sodium content from E3 to E12 or from E3 to postnatal day 28 (P28) and rats fed a nutritionally balanced commercial diet throughout development. Our data show that 9 d of feeding the custom diet produces extensive restructuring of the gustatory brainstem, and the effects are more global than in E3–P28 fed rats. This suggests that early periods of abnormal experience, especially when limited to NTS neurogenesis, leads to morphological and possible functional restructuring of the gustatory brainstem, which in turn may have serious consequences for the control of sensory and homeostatic processes.
Materials and Methods
Terminal field experiments
Animals.
All experiments were endorsed by the Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Virginia and followed guidelines set by the National Institutes of Health and the Society for Neuroscience. Control and breeder rats were purchased from Harlan Sprague Dawley (Indianapolis, IN). Experimental rats were bred on site in the University of Virginia vivarium (see below).
Dietary conditions.
Control animals (n = 7) received a standard rat chow diet containing 0.3% NaCl (rodent diet, catalog #8604; Harlan Teklad, Madison, WI) and tap water ad libitum. One group of experimental rats received a custom diet featuring a low-sodium content (0.03% NaCl; MP Biomedicals, Solon, OH) and distilled water ad libitum via their mothers from E3 to E12 (n = 7).
The sodium-restricted diet is custom made to approximate the macronutrient composition found in commercial diets (i.e., ∼24% protein, 4% fat, 4.5% fiber, 65% carbohydrate) while closely regulating the NaCl concentration at 0.03%. Although the only dietary deficiency in the experimental diet is the low sodium, there are differences between the two diets in their specific components. In particular, the custom low-sodium diet consists of 65% sucrose, whereas the carbohydrate content of the standard, sodium-replete diet consists primarily of soybean, wheat, and corn. Similarly, the protein in the sodium-restricted diet is provided by casein, whereas crude protein makes up a similar concentration in the commercial diet; the fat content in the sodium-restricted diet is provided by corn oil, whereas crude fat makes up a similar concentration in the commercial diet.
Although there are differences between diets (i.e., they are not matched), the goal of this and previous studies (King and Hill, 1991; Krimm and Hill, 1997; May and Hill, 2006; Sollars et al., 2006) is to compare neurobiological differences between a custom sodium-restricted diet and a commercial stock diet and not between specific dietary components. This choice was made primarily because the effects of a low-sodium diet in our studies are not attributable to altered taste stimulation but likely result in early undernutrition and/or malnutrition. Moreover, we chose here and previously to use a standard rodent diet as our control diet because of the rich literature that exists in studies of rodent development with standard rodent diets. Therefore, we are examining the effects on terminal field organization between a diet proven to alter peripheral (Hill et al., 1983; Hill, 1987; Hill and Przekop, 1988) and central (Vogt and Hill, 1993) gustatory function and structure (King and Hill, 1991; Krimm and Hill, 1997; May and Hill, 2006; Sollars et al., 2006) with a nutritionally balanced commercial diet. We note, however, that our previous work used a sodium-replete diet matched to the sodium-restricted diet (Hill et al., 1983; Hill, 1987; Hill and Przekop, 1988), and the neurophysiological effects on the taste system were no different between the custom-made “control” diet and the sodium-replete diet used here. Hereafter, we will refer to the custom diet as the “sodium-restricted diet” and to the commercial diet as the “control diet,” realizing that other key dietary components differ also.
To determine the date of conception, female rats were checked for sperm every morning during breeding. When females were sperm positive, they were moved to a separate cage and placed on the sodium-restricted diet 3 d later (E3). Nine days after being placed on the sodium-restricted diet (E12), the females were placed on the control diet for the remainder of gestation, and the pups were maintained on the control diet from birth until the time of the taste nerve labeling procedure (≥40 d postnatal). A second experimental group was given the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 (n = 5). At P28–P30, these rats were fed the control diet and maintained on it for at least 3 weeks before the taste nerve labeling procedure.
Triple fluorescent anterograde nerve labeling.
Procedures used to label three nerves with fluorescent tracers were similar to that described by May and Hill (2006) (Fig. 1). Briefly, the CT, greater superficial petrosal (GSP), and glossopharyngeal (IX) nerves were labeled with anterograde tracers to determine the volume and spatial interaction among the afferent terminal fields in the NTS. Animals were sedated with an intramuscular injection of 0.32 mg/kg Domitor (medetomidine hydrochloride; Pfizer Animal Health, Exton, PA) and anesthetized intramuscularly with 40 mg/kg Ketaset (ketamine hydrochloride; Fort Dodge Animal Health, Fort Dodge, IA). A water-circulating heating pad was used to maintain body temperature. The rats were positioned in a nontraumatic head holder, and a ventral approach was taken to expose the GSP and CT nerves within the right tympanic bulla. The CT and GSP nerves were cut near the geniculate ganglion in the tympanic bulla, and dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO; Fisher Scientific, Fair Lawn, NJ) was briefly applied to the cut nerves. Crystals of 3 kDa biotinylated dextran amine were then applied to the proximal cut end of the GSP, and 3 kDa of tetramethylrhodamine dextran amine was applied to the proximal cut end of the CT nerve (Fig. 1). A mixture of Vaseline and mineral oil was applied to prevent migration of dye. The IX nerve was isolated just medial to the tympanic bulla and cut and placed on a small piece of Parafilm. Again, DMSO was applied briefly, and crystals of 3 kDa Cascade Blue dextran amine were applied to the proximal cut end of the IX nerve. All dextran amine conjugates were purchased from Invitrogen (Eugene, OR). A Vaseline and mineral oil mixture and a layer of Parafilm were placed on top of the IX nerve to keep the dye in place. After all three nerves were labeled, the incision was closed with sutures. Animals were then given intramuscular injections of 5 mg/ml Antisedan (atipamezole hydrochloride; Pfizer Animal Health) to promote reversal of anesthesia. After an 18–24 h survival, animals were deeply anesthetized intraperitoneally with 2.4 g/kg urethane (ethyl carbamate; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) to maintain adequate oxygenation and perfused transcardially with Krebs-Henseleit buffer, pH 7.3, followed by 8% paraformaldehyde, pH 7.2.
We established previously (May and Hill, 2006) that (1) tracers placed on a nerve did not inadvertently label other nerves, (2) the full complement of fibers were labeled as revealed by examinations of the respective ganglia, and (3) the period of survival after surgery was optimal for transport of each anterograde tracer. It should be noted that the mere presence of labeled terminal fields from GSP, CT, or IX axons does not necessarily mean that it conveys taste information. For example, the IX nerve carries a significant tactile component to the NTS (Frank, 1968). Thus, the NTS is a highly integrative center for multiple sensory inputs that originate from the oral, pharyngeal, and laryngeal epithelia as well as the viscera (Bradley, 2007).
Tissue preparation.
Brains were removed and postfixed in 8% paraformaldehyde overnight. The medulla was blocked and sectioned horizontally on a vibratome at 50 μm to allow visualization of the entire rostrocaudal and mediolateral extent of the gustatory afferent terminal fields in the NTS. Tissue sections were collected in 0.1 m PBS, pH 7.2, at room temperature. Sections were then incubated for 1 h in PBS containing 0.2% Triton with 1:500 streptavidin Alexa Fluor 488 (Invitrogen) and 1:500 rabbit anti-Cascade Blue (Invitrogen) at room temperature. Streptavidin Alexa Fluor 488 was used to visualize the biotinylated dextran amine-labeled, GSP-positive terminals. The rabbit anti-Cascade Blue was used as a primary antibody directed at Cascade Blue-labeled, IX nerve-positive terminals (Fig. 1). Next, sections were rinsed in PBS (three times for 5 min) and incubated for 30 min in PBS containing 0.2% Triton and 1:500 goat anti-rabbit Cy5 (Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA). Goat anti-rabbit Cy5 was used as a secondary antibody to visualize IX nerve terminals. Finally, sections were rinsed in PBS and imaged on a confocal laser-scanning microscope (Fluoview 300; Olympus America, Melville, NY).
Confocal microscopy and data collection.
Terminal fields were imaged with an Olympus IX70 microscope fitted with a Fluoview 3.3 laser-scanning system (Olympus America) equipped with a 488 nm blue argon laser, a 543 nm green HE/NE laser, and a 633 nm red HE/NE laser. Each 50 μm section containing fluorescent label was wet mounted between two coverslips, and optical sections of each terminal field were captured sequentially every 3 μm. All three terminal field images were digitally merged, and terminal field area and areas of overlap among terminal fields were quantified using Neurolucida software (version 4.34; MicroBrightField, Colchester, VT) (Fig. 2). The volume of terminal fields and field overlap in each 50 μm section were calculated by summing the areas in each optical section and multiplying by the optical section thickness (3 μm). Total terminal field volumes for each nerve and the volumes in which two or more fields overlapped were obtained by summing the volumes from each 50 μm section. The density of the label was not quantified. Therefore, the terminal field volumes were calculated when any label, regardless of density, was present. This provides data of the topographical organization of the terminal fields but does not provide information concerning the absolute amount of afferent input into the NTS.
Although volume measurements were not done “blind” to the experimental group, the reliability of the measures was checked and confirmed through a subset of measurements within each group. More importantly, the volumes obtained for control rats closely match previously reported values for the CT (King and Hill, 1991; Krimm and Hill, 1997) and GSP and IX (May and Hill, 2006) volumes. Therefore, multiple investigators, using different experimental protocols, report reliably consistent terminal field volumes.
For figure plates, Fireworks (Macromedia, San Francisco, CA), Volocity (Improvision, Lexington, MA), and Photoshop (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA) were used to compose images from digital files in which all optical sections for each physical section were flattened into one plane. Images were enhanced in contrast and brightness for presentation purposes. No other digital image manipulations were used.
Statistical analysis.
The mean ± SEM was determined for total nerve terminal field volume, total overlap among terminal fields, and volumes of terminals contained within dorsal, intermediate, and ventral zones. These variables were compared between rats fed the control diet, rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, and rats fed the sodium-restricted from E3 to P28 using a one-way ANOVA and post hoc independent-samples t tests. In the post hoc analysis, the α level of 0.05 was partitioned among the three groups to avoid an inflated type I error rate. Statistical results from post hoc analysis with an α level of p < 0.05 are reported as significant.
IX nerve terminal field in the spinal trigeminal nucleus
Animals.
Six additional adult (three rats fed the control diet, three rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12) Sprague Dawley rats (Harlan Sprague Dawley) were used to measure the volume of the IX nerve terminal field in the oral division of the spinal trigeminal nucleus. This was done as a follow-up experiment to determine whether the enlarged IX terminal field in the NTS of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (see Results) also occurred in a projection area that does not receive gustatory inputs (i.e., receives tactile input).
Nerve labeling, tissue preparation, and scanning confocal image collection.
Labeling the IX nerve and tissue preparation procedures were the same as described above. IX terminations in the spinal trigeminal nucleus were visualized, and the volumes were measured using scanning confocal microscopy as described above for the nerve terminal fields in the NTS.
Statistical analysis.
Mean total IX terminal fields in the spinal trigeminal nucleus were compared between adult rats fed the control diet and adult rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and analyzed using independent-samples t tests. Statistical results with an α level of p < 0.05 are reported as significant.
NTS volume
Animals.
Six additional adult (three rats fed the control diet, three rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12) Sprague Dawley rats (Harlan) were used to measure the volume of the NTS. Rats were deeply anesthetized intraperitoneally with 2.4 g/kg urethane (ethyl carbamate; Sigma-Aldrich) and perfused transcardially at ≥40 d of age with Krebs-Henseleit buffer, pH 7.3, followed by 8% paraformaldehyde, pH 7.2.
Tissue preparation and confocal microscope-transmitted light imaging.
Brains were removed and postfixed in 8% paraformaldehyde overnight. The medulla was blocked and horizontally sectioned on a vibratome at 50 μm, collected, and prepared for imaging as described above. Sections were imaged on the scanning confocal microscope using the transmitted light function with the 488 nm argon laser. The NTS is relatively translucent within the brainstem and can be easily visualized within the medulla without staining. The boundaries of the nucleus were clearly determined in all images.
Data collection.
The NTS volume was measured using Neurolucida computer software (version 4.34; MicroBrightField). To calculate volume, the measurements from all of the sections were summed and multiplied by 50 μm.
Statistical analysis.
Mean total NTS volumes were compared between adult rats fed the control diet and adult rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and analyzed using independent-samples t tests. Statistical results with an α level of p < 0.05 are reported as significant.
Geniculate ganglion and petrosal ganglion cell number
Six additional adult rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 had either the CT (n = 4) or the GSP (n = 2) nerve labeled as described previously, with the exception that Micro-Ruby dextran (Invitrogen) was used as the tracer. Three additional adult rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 had the petrosal ganglia labeled with Micro-Ruby by way of the IX nerve. Micro-Ruby was used over other tracers because of its superior quality in labeling cells and imaging of whole ganglia. Each rat was perfused as described previously. The ganglia (geniculate for CT and GSP, petrosal for IX) were removed and postfixed in 8% paraformaldehyde overnight. Each ganglion was then dissected from either the VII or IX nerve, and the intact ganglion was imaged on the confocal microscope at 2 μm intervals using the 543 nm green HE/NE laser, generating an average of 50 optical sections through each ganglion (Shuler et al., 2004). All labeled neurons were physically counted with the aid of Neurolucida software (Shuler et al., 2004). We chose to label ganglia at adulthood because the ganglion cell number becomes stable at P20 with no further cell death or addition thereafter (Carr et al., 2005).
The ganglion cell number was compared between adult rats fed the control diet (Shuler et al., 2004; May and Hill, 2006) and adult rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and analyzed using independent-samples t tests. Statistical results with an α level of p < 0.05 were reported as significant.
CT nerve neurophysiology
Seven additional rats (four rats fed the control diet, three rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12) were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg Nembutal, i.p.). The animals were tracheotomized and placed on a circulating water heating pad to maintain body temperature. Hypoglossal nerves were transected bilaterally to prevent tongue movement, and the animal was placed in a nontraumatic head holder. The left CT nerve was isolated using a mandibular approach. The nerve was exposed near the tympanic bulla, cut, desheathed, and positioned on a platinum electrode. A second electrode was placed in nearby muscle to serve as ground. A mixture of petroleum jelly and mineral oil was placed in the cavity around the nerve. Whole CT activity was fed to a Grass Instruments (Quincy, MA) low-impedance input stage amplifier. The filtered signal was monitored with an audio loudspeaker and oscilloscope. The signal was integrated (time constant, 1.0 s) and was led to and analyzed with PowerLab Scope software (ADInstruments, Mountain View, CA).
Stimulation procedure.
All chemicals were reagent grade and prepared in distilled water. Neural responses to a concentration series of 0.05, 0.1, 0.25, and 0.5 m NaCl, sodium acetate, and KCl were recorded. Each concentration series was bracketed by application of 0.5 m NH4Cl. In addition, responses were recorded to the nonsalt stimuli of 0.01N HCl, 0.01 m quinine hydrochloride, and 1.0 m sucrose. Solutions were applied to the tongue in 5 ml aliquots with a syringe and allowed to remain to the tongue for ∼40 s. After each solution application, the tongue was rinsed with distilled water for ≥1 min. CT responses were calculated as follows: the height of the tonic response was measured 20 s after application, and response magnitudes were expressed as ratios relative to the mean of 0.5 m NH4Cl responses before and after stimulation. Response data were retained for analysis only when 0.5 m NH4Cl responses that bracketed a concentration series varied by <10%.
Statistical analysis.
Neurophysiological responses were compared between adult rats fed the control diet and adult rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and analyzed using independent-samples t tests. The α level was set at p < 0.05 and adjusted to compensate for the number of statistical tests conducted within a concentrations series.
Results
Organization of gustatory nerve terminal fields in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 compared with rats fed the control diet
Total terminal field volumes
As found previously in rats fed the control diet, the sequence of terminal fields in the NTS from the most dorsal horizontal to the most ventral horizontal section containing labeling was IX, GSP, and then CT (May and Hill, 2006). The same general sequence was seen in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, although the terminal fields were dramatically enlarged, resulting in a blurring of their distinct topographies. For example, the CT terminal field in rats fed the control diet was contained within an average of 270 μm (i.e., 5.5 horizontal sections), whereas rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 had a CT terminal field that averaged 410 μm (8.2 sections). The increased number of labeled sections combined with a general increase in terminal field volume in each section yielded a mean total CT terminal field volume in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 that was four times larger than found in rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 a–h, 4) (F (2,18) = 24.2; post hoc p = 0.0001). The CT terminal field was not the only terminal field affected: the IX terminal field volume was approximately three times the volume of that found in rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 a–c,e–g, 4) (F (2,15) = 10.9; post hoc p = 0.002), and the GSP terminal field volume was double the size of rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 a–h, 4) (F (2,18) = 5.2; post hoc p = 0.01).
Because there were dramatic increases in all terminal fields that invaded territory not normally occupied in rats fed the control diet, we explored whether there was more overlap in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12. In rats fed the control diet, the CT terminal field overlapped considerably with the GSP terminal field but relatively little with the IX terminal field (Figs. 3 c, 4). The overlap between the GSP and IX terminal fields was intermediate to that with the CT, respectively (Figs. 3 b, 4) (May and Hill, 2006). In stark contrast, the CT–GSP terminal field overlap in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 was triple that of rats fed the control diet, because of the more dorsal extension of the CT field and the enlarged GSP field (Figs. 3 e–h, 4) (F (2,18) = 17.4; post hoc p = 0.0001). As a result of the increased volumes of both IX and GSP terminal fields, the IX–GSP overlap was three times larger in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 compared with rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 a–c,e–g, 4) (F (2,15) = 5.8; post hoc p = 0.02). In addition, the ventral extension of the IX field into the intermediate zone and the more dorsal projection of the CT field yielded a IX–CT overlap in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 that was >10 times greater than seen in rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 a–c,e–g, 4) (F (2,15) = 16.8; post hoc p = 0.001). Accordingly, CT–GSP–IX overlap was also larger in these rats (Figs. 3 a–c,e–g, 4) (F (2,15) = 14.0; post hoc p = 0.001).
Zonal distribution of terminal fields
To localize regions especially susceptible to the dietary manipulation and to be consistent with our previous studies of the CT terminal field in the NTS (King and Hill, 1991; Krimm and Hill, 1997; May and Hill, 2006; Sollars et al., 2006), the NTS was divided into dorsal, intermediate, and ventral zones. The dorsal zone contained sections in which the solitary tract was most visible and included sections in which the fourth ventricle occupied the largest mediolateral extent (Fig. 5 a,b). The dorsal zone was further characterized by the spinal trigeminal tract extending to approximately the rostralmost extent of the NTS and by the lack of the hypoglossal nucleus and the facial nucleus (Fig. 5 a,b, facial nucleus not shown). This was the largest of the three zones, encompassing almost the entire IX terminal field in rats fed the control diet (Fig. 3 a,b). In rats fed the control diet, this zone typically consisted of four to five 50 μm sections and was distinguished by the rounded shape of the CT terminal field (Fig. 3 a,b,e,f,i,j). The intermediate zone (usually two 50 μm sections) was characterized by the decrease in the fourth ventricle volume compared with the dorsal zone, by the dorsal extent of the hypoglossal nucleus, by the extension of the spinal trigeminal tract rostrally beyond the inferior cerebellar peduncle, and by the presence of the dorsal extent of the facial nucleus (Fig. 5 c, facial nucleus not shown). In all animals, regardless of group, the terminal fields of the CT and GSP nerves in the intermediate zone was most easily identified and most consistent across animals (Fig. 3 c,g,k). To provide another referent, this zone is ∼1.5 mm ventral to the surface of the medulla, 1.5 mm lateral from the midline, and 2.5 mm rostral from obex. The ventral zone of the gustatory terminal fields (usually three to four 50 μm sections) had an expanded hypoglossal nucleus and facial nucleus compared with the intermediate sections (Fig. 5 d). Dividing the fields in this manner highlights the vastly different distribution of terminations along the dorsoventral aspect of the NTS and more narrowly defines specific regions of anatomical change. Compartmentalizing terminal fields into zones also demonstrates the similar characteristics and contiguous and overlapping arrangement of the three afferent terminal zones.
It must be pointed out that we use the terms “dorsal,” “intermediate,” and “ventral” zones here to be consistent with previous reports (King and Hill, 1991; Krimm and Hill, 1997; May and Hill, 2006; Sollars et al., 2006). However, the orientation of the NTS within the brainstem is such that the caudalmost portion of the NTS is dorsal to the rostralmost portion (i.e., the NTS expands ventrally and rostrally from the dorsalmost extent of the NTS) (Fig. 6). Therefore, the dorsal zone more accurately represents the dorsocaudal portion of the field in the NTS, and the intermediate and ventral sections represent a more ventrorostral portion of the terminal field in the NTS (Fig. 6). Similar to previous reports that instituted a low-NaCl diet early in development, most of the reorganization induced by feeding the sodium-restricted diet at E3–E12 occurred in the more dorsal horizontal sections of the NTS. The effects, however, were more widespread in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 than ever seen before. These rats had irregularly shaped CT terminal fields that were about six times larger than rats fed the control diet in the dorsal zone (Figs. 3 a,b,e,f, 7) (F (2,18) = 21.3; post hoc p = 0.002). Moreover, the CT terminal field volume in the intermediate zone was also larger than rats fed the control diet and was characterized by a more caudal expansion of the terminal field (Figs. 3 c,g, 7) (F (2,18) = 10.4; post hoc p = 0.003). An elaboration of IX and GSP terminal fields was also evident. Because of the mediolateral and rostrocaudal expansion of the field, the IX terminal field of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 was larger in the dorsal zone (Figs. 3 a,b,e,f, 7) (F (2,15) = 11.3; post hoc p = 0.002) compared with rats fed the control diet, and the dorsal (Figs. 3 a,b,e,f, 7) (F (2,18) = 5.5; post hoc p = 0.01) and intermediate (Figs. 3 c,g, 7) (F (2,18) = 3.5; post hoc p = 0.04) zones of the GSP terminal field were also significantly greater than rats fed the control diet.
As expected from the volumes of each terminal field, most of the alterations related to zones of overlap were in the dorsal zone. For example, the overlap among terminal fields in the dorsal zone was significantly larger than in rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 a,b,e,f, 7): GSP–CT overlap (F (2,18) = 17.2; post hoc p = 0.0001), IX–CT overlap (F (2,15) = 16.6; post hoc p = 0.001), IX–GSP overlap (F (2,15) = 5.9; post hoc p = 0.02), CT–GSP–IX overlap (Figs. 3 a,b,e,f, 7) (F (2,15) = 14.5; post hoc p = 0.001). In addition, the GSP–CT overlap was also significantly larger in the intermediate zone in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 compared with rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 c,g, 7) (F (2,18) = 7.3; post hoc p = 0.005).
Comparison with recovery from long-term sodium restriction: less is more
As a comparison with rats that were sodium restricted during a 9 d early prenatal period, we examined the effects of feeding the sodium-restricted diet to include postnatal periods. Contrary to the widespread differences evident in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, rats that received the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 and were then fed the control diet for at least 3 weeks displayed selective differences in total terminal field volume and exclusively in the dorsal zone. The average dorsal to ventral sections containing the terminal field label was 370 μm (i.e., 7.4 sections) compared with 270 and 410 μm for rats fed the control diet and rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, respectively.
Similar to the terminal fields in rats fed the control diet and rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, the IX terminal field projected to the dorsalmost horizontal section, followed ventrally by the GSP and the CT projections. However, the topography of the CT terminal field in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 was different from both control and rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12: CT terminal field projections extended 100 μm farther than rats fed the control diet in the dorsal to ventral axis, but 50 μm less than rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12. This yielded a mean total CT terminal field volume in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 that was double the volume of rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 b–d,f–h, 4) (F (2,18) = 24.2; post hoc p = 0.04) but only half the volume of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (Figs. 3 e–h,j–l, 4) (p = 0.003). A similar pattern emerged for IX total terminal field volume: rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 had IX terminal fields double the size of rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 a,b,i,j, 4) (F (2,15) = 10.9; post hoc p = 0.04) but smaller than rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (Figs. 3 e–h,i–k, 4) (p = 0.04).
In contrast to rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, those fed the diet from E3 to P28 displayed differences in terminal field volume only in the dorsal zone. Specifically, the mean dorsal volume of CT terminal field was triple that of rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 b,j, 7) (F (2,18) = 21.3; post hoc p = 0.02), and the IX terminal field was double that of rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 a,b,i,j, 7) (F (2,15) = 11.3; post hoc p = 0.04); both means were significantly smaller than dorsal CT and IX terminal field volumes in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12.
Because of the significantly larger CT and IX terminal fields in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28, we also examined the overlap among terminal fields. As a result of the enlarged IX and CT terminal fields, the IX–CT overlap was approximately five times greater in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 than in rats fed the control diet (Figs. 3 k, 4) (F (2,15) = 16.8; post hoc p = 0.01) but half the size of the IX–CT overlap found in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (Figs. 3 e–g, 4) (p = 0.006). CT–GSP–IX overlap was also significantly larger in rats fed the restricted diet from E3 to P28 (Figs. 3 j,k, 4) (F (2,15) = 14.0; post hoc p = 0.01) but, again, half the volume of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (Figs. 3 e–g,j,k, 4) (p = 0.02).
Because of the enlarged CT and IX terminal field in the dorsal zone of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28, dorsal zone CT–GSP overlap (F (2,18) = 17.2; post hoc p = 0.03) and IX–CT overlap (Figs. 3 j, 7) (F (2,18) = 16.6; post hoc p = 0.01) were significantly larger than that of rats fed the control diet. Consequently, CT–GSP–IX overlap was also larger, again exclusively in the dorsal zone (Figs. 3 i,j, 7) (F (2,15) = 14.5; post hoc p = 0.01). Not surprisingly, the GSP–CT overlap (p = 0.01), IX–CT overlap (p = 0.006), and CT–GSP–IX overlap (Figs. 3 e–h,i–l, 4) were smaller than that of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12.
An analysis of enlarged terminal fields as they relate to NTS subnuclei: coronal sections
To further illustrate the overlapping terminal fields and the group-related differences, we labeled the three nerves as described previously in a rat fed the control diet, in a rat fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, and in a rat fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 and viewed the fields in coronally sectioned tissue. Terminal field labeling and imaging of the GSP, CT, and IX nerves were done as described for the horizontally sectioned tissue in the rat fed the control diet (Fig. 8 a–d), in the rat fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (Fig. 8 g–j), and in the rat fed the restricted diet from E3 to P28 (Fig. 8 m–p). One optical section using transmitted light was also imaged to show the respective terminal fields overlayed with the approximate location of the subnuclei (Fig. 8 e, control, k, E3–E12, q, E3–P28). Finally, each section was mounted on slides and subsequently stained with cresyl violet to show the NTS and other dorsal medullary structures (Fig. 8 f, control, l, E3–E12, r, E3–P28). No measurements were taken from coronal sections; they are used to examine terminal field organization in the coronal plane only.
The photomicrographs shown in Figure 8 were ∼400 μm caudal from the anteriormost extent of the NTS (also see Fig. 3). Thus, sections used here to illustrate the diet-related effects were from the caudal “rostral” pole, which also contains the intermediate zone of the NTS (Halsell et al., 1996). Examination of sections rostral and caudal to the ones shown in Figure 8 emphasize the group-related differences best appreciated through horizontal sections (Fig. 3). For example, more rostral sections would better show the differences in the GSP field, and more caudal sections would best illustrate differences in the IX field. Sections shown in Figure 8 best illustrate the unusual arrangement of the CT. Specifically, the CT terminal field in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (Fig. 8 h) was much larger than seen in rats fed the control diet (Fig. 8 b) and in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 (Fig. 8 n). Moreover, the distinct overlap of the three fields can be readily seen in Figure 8, d, j, and p, corresponding to rats fed the control diet and to rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and from E3 to P28, respectively. Finally, what was particularly surprising was how the CT field extended beyond the medial and rostral central subnuclei in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (Fig. 8 k) and in rats fed the diet from E3 to P28 (Fig. 8 q). This is particularly noticeable in the rat fed the restricted diet from E3 to E12, where the CT terminal field extends into the ventral subdivision of the NTS (Fig. 8 j,k). Although the match is not exact, perhaps because of different animals, the “displaced” area of the CT field would likely extend beyond that shown in Figure 3 h. (Not all labeled sections are shown in Fig. 3). It should be noted that the boundaries for the subdivision of NTS subnuclei shown in Figure 8 are similar to those shown by Halsell et al. (1996); the exact boundaries may differ slightly from the boundary shown.
IX nerve terminal field in the spinal trigeminal nucleus
The IX nerve carries a significant tactile component (Frank, 1968) and sends terminations to both the NTS and spinal trigeminal nucleus in the brainstem (Contreras et al., 1982; Bradley et al., 1985; Sweazey and Bradley, 1986). Because the IX terminal field is greatly enlarged in the NTS of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, we examined the IX terminal field in the spinal trigeminal nucleus and found that there was no difference in the total spinal trigeminal IX terminal field volume between rats fed the control diet and rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 (31.3 ± 6.4 and 35.2 ± 5.9, respectively; p > 0.05) (Fig. 9). Figure 9 shows a representative horizontal section in a rat fed the control diet in which the IX terminal field (shown in blue) is superimposed over an image of the lateral medulla. The section is ∼100 μm ventral to where the largest IX projection into the NTS is seen.
Body weights and NTS volume
Birth weights and body weights of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and rats fed the control diet were similar (P3, 9.4 ± 0.6 and 8.8 ± 0.6 g; P35, 116.7 ± 6.7 and 112.4 ± 10.6 g; p > 0.05), as were the mean total NTS volumes (9.5 × 108 ± 0.03 and 9.8 × 108 ± 0.06 μm3; p > 0.05). In contrast, pups that were fed the sodium-restricted diet from E8 through adulthood (i.e., like the current rats fed the restricted diet from E3 to P28) weighed less at birth and at adulthood and had smaller NTS volumes (May and Hill, 2006). When rats fed the sodium-restricted diet were fed the control diet at P28, they underwent remarkable weight gain and somatic growth. As a result of the rapid growth after repletion, rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 had NTS sizes that caught up to rats fed the control diet (King and Hill, 1991). Therefore, for both rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and from E3 to P28, NTS volumes were similar to that in rats fed the control diet when afferent nerve labeling was done at adulthood.
Ganglion cell number
Because sodium repletion occurs when ganglion cells are being born (Altman and Bayer, 1982), the enlarged CT, GSP, and IX terminal fields may be caused by an increase in the number of ganglion cells projecting to the NTS (i.e., normal developmental cell death may have been affected by the dietary manipulation). This, however, did not occur for any of the cell populations. The geniculate ganglion cell numbers in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 were not different from rats fed the control diet for the CT nerve (451.5 ± 14.2 and 487.9 ± 37.9; p ≥ 0.05) (Shuler et al., 2004) or the GSP nerve (414.5 ± 13.4 and 430 ± 102.3; p ≥ 0.05) (May and Hill, 2006). Similarly, the petrosal ganglion cell number was also unaffected for the IX nerve (restricted diet from E3 to E12, 1003.5 ± 159.1; control diet, 1192.0 ± 156.4; p ≥ 0.05) (May and Hill, 2006).
The increased ganglion cell number is not an issue in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28, because rats that are sodium restricted throughout development and adulthood do not have CT, GSP, or IX ganglion cells that are different in number from rats fed the control diet (May and Hill, 2006). Therefore, because geniculate ganglion cell addition and death is complete before P28 (i.e., P20) (Carr et al., 2005), rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28 would have cell numbers comparable to lifetime sodium-restricted rats.
CT nerve physiology
To test the hypothesis that the enlarged terminal fields in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 were attributable to altered afferent signals, as proposed to occur in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 through adulthood (Sollars et al., 2006), we recorded taste responses from the CT nerve in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and in rats fed the control diet. In response to a concentration series of NaCl, sodium acetate, NH4Cl, and KCl, there were no group-related differences in response magnitudes (p < 0.05). Figure 10 illustrates the similar response–concentration curves for NaCl between the rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and rats fed the control diet obtained from this study. For comparison, Figure 10 also shows the attenuated relative responses to NaCl in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 through adulthood [E3 to adulthood restricted data are taken from Hill (1987)]. We conclude from these recordings that neurophysiological taste responses, at least carried by the CT nerve, are not affected at early adulthood by an early period of dietary sodium restriction.
Discussion
The current study shows that the terminal fields of three nerves that carry gustatory information are radically restructured when a diet deficient in sodium is instituted during an early prenatal period. Nine days of feeding this diet leads to as much as a 10 times enlargement of afferent terminal fields of the CT, GSP, and IX nerves in the adult rat NTS, with the greatest effects in the dorsalmost zones (Fig. 11). The enlarged terminal fields not only encompass a larger portion of the NTS normally occupied in rats fed the control diet, they also appear to invade subnuclei that typically do not receive gustatory nerve inputs. This extensive brainstem reorganization is independent of changes in the number of projection neurons, the size of the target, and of afferent activity; none of these measures in adult rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 were different from rats fed the control diet. Furthermore, the effects of the early dietary manipulation on the IX terminal field are specific to the gustatory brainstem, because IX terminal field in the spinal trigeminal nucleus is unaffected.
In contrast to the pervasive effects observed in rats that were fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12, rats fed the diet from E3 to P28 displayed a selective and less extensive reorganization of terminal fields in the NTS. Specifically, the terminal field volumes of the CT and IX nerves were two times greater than rats fed the control diet and the GSP terminal field was unaffected. These findings are similar to results from rats that are fed the sodium-restricted diet throughout their development (May and Hill, 2006). Consequently, the briefer period of feeding the sodium-restricted diet leads to more dramatic effects on these measures than the longer period of dietary manipulation.
We must emphasize here that we are comparing the effects of two different diets: a custom diet that is low in sodium compared with a commercially available sodium-replete diet (see Materials and Methods). The diets are not matched for all dietary components. However, we believe that the effects are secondary to low sodium and may be more likely attributable to the effects of malnourishment during key periods of brainstem development (see below).
The period of greatest terminal field changes: an indicator of activity-independent mechanisms
These effects are remarkable, because the 9 d period in which the sodium-restricted diet was fed occurs before many milestones in peripheral and central gustatory development. Some of these milestones include formation of tongue (Mistretta, 1972), projection of ganglion cells to their peripheral and central targets (Zhang and Ashwell, 2001b), and significant brainstem development (Zhang and Ashwell, 2001a). This period does, however, occur during a crucial time in gustatory system development, when geniculate ganglion cells are at their peak proliferation rate (E12) and during the differentiation of brainstem nuclei (E11–E16) (Altman and Bayer, 1982). One explanation for the enlarged terminal fields is that feeding the low-sodium diet during this developmental period causes a change in the number of ganglion cells that are born, or in the number of cells that survive and subsequently project to their targets. As noted previously, however, this is not the case; geniculate and petrosal ganglion cell numbers do not differ from rats fed the control diet.
Because the changes cannot be explained by peripheral taste function or anatomy, the effects seen in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 may be attributable primarily to activity-independent mechanisms, contrasting with that proposed for the CT nerve of rats fed the sodium-restricted diet throughout their lifetime (Sollars et al., 2006). Enlarged CT terminal fields found in rats fed the restricted diet on or before E8 throughout development and adulthood may result from the lack of pruning of exuberant synapses as a consequence of highly attenuated taste responses of the CT nerve (Hill, 1987; Sollars et al., 2006). This is consistent with similar observations in other sensory systems (Schlaggar et al., 1993; O'Leary et al., 1995; Catalano and Shatz, 1998). Once past the period in which afferent activity prunes terminal fields (i.e., past the critical period), the system remains “frozen” in the immature state. This would most likely occur for rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to P28. In comparison, rats fed the diet from E3 to E12 were never exposed to the sodium-restricted diet, except via their mothers (McClellan and Novak, 2001), and appear to have a normal physiological response to sodium salt stimuli. Thus, mechanisms directly related to afferent activity seem unlikely because the control diet was fed long before the system is functional.
Moreover, our preliminary data show dramatic developmental differences between rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and long-term restricted or control rats; rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 progressively expand terminal fields after birth, whereas the other two groups begin with enlarged fields at early postnatal ages that then decrease in size (Mangold and Hill, 2006). This developmental difference highlights the dramatic cellular/molecular differences between rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 and from E3 to P28. Although we think it is unlikely, the distinct possibility exists that possible group-related neurophysiological differences prenatally may influence central gustatory development similar to that seen in the visual system (Shatz, 1996; Penn and Shatz, 1999).
Potential hormonal/growth factor-related mechanisms
Mechanisms contributing to the effects noted here may relate to hormonal and/or growth factors acting on, or present in, the developing fetus. For example, maternal undernutrition reduces fetal levels of 11β-hydroxysteriod dehydrogenase type 2, which is present in the fetal rat medulla during our dietary manipulation (Diaz et al., 1998). A decrease in this enzyme causes an increase in maternal glucocorticoid levels (Fleming et al., 2004; Owen et al., 2005), leaving the fetus vulnerable to the effects of excess maternal glucocorticoids (Fleming et al., 2004; Fujioka et al., 2006).
Other key examples in which hormonal and/or growth factors impact brainstem development relate to insulin and insulin-like growth factors (IGFs) (Barker, 1997; Lee et al., 1999). Fetal insulin, IGF, and glucose concentrations rapidly fall during maternal undernutrition (Barker, 2001), although the link between available nutrients and resulting growth factor levels is unclear (Ikeya et al., 2002). Both IGF-1 and IGF-2 mRNA are prenatally expressed and play important roles in neural development, including proliferation, differentiation, maturation, and survival of neurons (Popken et al., 2004; Gicquel and Le Bouc, 2006). Therefore, if low levels of these hormones during the very early “critical period” dictate changes in terminal field organization of gustatory nerves, one would predict similar effects on rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 or from E3 to P28. This is not what we found. However, it may be the surge in IGFs or insulin on repletion at a critical period in NTS development (approximately E12) that produces the effects seen here. Consistent with this hypothesis, IGF-1 has been implicated in brainstem nuclei development; transgenic mice that overexpress IGF-1 show dramatic changes in NTS morphology (Dentremont et al., 1999). We propose that a surge of IGF-1 or a similar factor when diets are switched at E12 permanently shapes the molecular/cellular characteristics of the NTS, ultimately resulting in enlarged terminal fields. Such a surge at this critical developmental period would not be available in rats sustaining longer periods of dietary sodium restriction (i.e., E3–P28 restricted rats). We cannot rule out that other differences in the diet (e.g., type of carbohydrates) also contribute to the brainstem reorganization. However, peripheral taste function is not altered in rats raised on a high-sucrose/sodium-replete diet (D. L. Hill, unpublished observations).
Implications for reorganization of function
There are clear implications of how the abnormal organization of centrally projecting primary afferents would produce altered taste function. For example, three separate nerves carrying gustatory information terminate in approximately the same central target, presumably leading to coordinated processing of overlapping inputs. This central target is predominantly in the zone (dorsal) we find to be most affected by the early and limited dietary sodium restriction. Significant levels of functional convergence occur in control rats (Travers et al., 1986; Bradley and Grabauskas, 1998). Therefore, one may expect that the widespread changes in terminal field organization in rats fed the sodium-restricted diet from E3 to E12 may receive the incoming information uniquely, such that coding of the qualitative and/or quantitative aspects, including tactile and temperature information, of the stimulus are changed from that in rats fed the control diet. An alteration in the way NTS cells extract features of the sensory stimulus may, in turn, lead to altered taste-related function that would impact perception of gustatory quality and quantity. Moreover, aberrant afferent activity could influence nongustatory processes such as oral–motor functions (Travers and Rinaman, 2002) and homeostatic processes such as cephalic phase insulin release (Teff, 2000).
Conclusion and summary
Because of the timing and large extent of experimentally induced changes seen in the NTS, the developing gustatory system is an excellent model for examining the nature and the mechanisms responsible for plasticity in the mammalian brainstem. Specifically, it is especially susceptible to environmental influences during early brainstem development and provides an important experimental contrast to experimentally induced changes in synaptic modifications in sensory cortices.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant DC00407. We extend our appreciation to Dr. Peter C. Brunjes for the use of his confocal laser microscope and to William B. Goodwin for his technical assistance.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. David L. Hill, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904. dh2t{at}virginia.edu