Abstract
Intracellular recordings of cortical neurons in awake cat and monkey show a depolarized state, sustained firing, and intense subthreshold synaptic activity. It is not known what conductance dynamics underlie such activity and how neurons process information in such highly stochastic states. Here, we combine intracellular recordings in awake and naturally sleeping cats with computational models to investigate subthreshold dynamics of conductances and how conductance dynamics determine spiking activity. We show that during both wakefulness and the “up-states” of natural slow-wave sleep, membrane-potential activity stems from a diversity of combinations of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances, with dominant inhibition in most of the cases. Inhibition also provides the largest contribution to membrane potential fluctuations. Computational models predict that in such inhibition-dominant states, spikes are preferentially evoked by a drop of inhibitory conductance, and that its signature is a transient drop of membrane conductance before the spike. This pattern of conductance change is indeed observed in estimates of spike-triggered averages of synaptic conductances during wakefulness and slow-wave sleep up states. These results show that activated states are defined by diverse combinations of excitatory and inhibitory conductances with pronounced inhibition, and that the dynamics of inhibition is particularly effective on spiking, suggesting an important role for inhibitory processes in both conscious and unconscious cortical states.
- intracellular recordings
- conductance analysis
- cerebral cortex
- computational models
- GABAergic conductances
- spike-triggered average
Introduction
Intracellular recordings of cortical neurons in awake, conscious cats and monkeys show a depolarized membrane potential (Vm), sustained firing, and intense subthreshold synaptic activity (Matsumura et al., 1988; Baranyi et al., 1993; Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001). It is presently unclear how neurons process information in such active and irregular states. An important step to investigate this problem is to obtain a precise characterization of the conductance variations during activated electroencephalogram (EEG) states. Input-resistance measurements indicate that during such activated states, cortical neurons can experience periods of high conductance, which may have significant consequences for their integrative properties (Destexhe and Paré, 1999; Kuhn et al., 2004) (for review, see Destexhe et al., 2003). In anesthetized animals, several studies have provided measurements of the excitatory and inhibitory conductance contributions to the state of the membrane, using various paradigms (Borg-Graham et al., 1998; Hirsch et al., 1998; Paré et al., 1998; Anderson et al., 2000; Wehr and Zador, 2003; Priebe and Ferster, 2005; Haider et al., 2006). However, no such conductance measurements have been made so far in awake and naturally sleeping animals.
In the present study, we provide such estimates using a combination of intracellular recordings and computational models. Intracellular recordings were performed in the association cortex of nonanesthetized cats across wake or sleep states, according to a technique described previously (Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001). Intracellularly recorded neurons display a highly complex and irregular subthreshold synaptic activity from which we estimate the conductances (and their variances) by matching stochastic models to the intracellular data. We also use the same approach to estimate the time course of relative conductance changes during the transition between depolarized and hyperpolarized phases (up and down states), as seen intracellularly during slow-wave sleep (SWS). We next introduce computational models with stochastic conductances reproducing the above measurements to infer the type of conductance variations that underlie spiking activity in such states. Parts of this paper have been published previously in abstract form (Destexhe et al., 2005; Pospischil et al., 2005).
Materials and Methods
Intracellular recordings in awake and naturally sleeping animals.
Intracellular recordings were performed in cat association cortex (areas 5, 7, and 21 of the parietal cortex) using a technique described in detail previously (Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001). Briefly, in a chamber specially designed for chronic intracellular recordings, local field potential (LFP), electromyogram (EMG), and electrooculogram (EOG) electrodes as well as head fixation screws were implanted under anesthesia. Electrodes for LFP recordings were inserted in cortical depth and their tips were located at a depth of 0.8–1.0 mm. The animals were gradually habituated to the recording setup and the head-fixed position. After 3–4 d of training, the cats started to display normal sleep-waking cycles and could move their limbs.
Intracellular recordings were performed under current clamp using standard borosilicate glass micropipettes (outer diameter, 1.5 mm; inner diameter, 0.84 mm) filled with 2.5 mm potassium acetate. Initially, pipettes were pulled to a resistance of 120–150 MΩ and under visual guidance their tips were broken to obtain a resistance of 50–70 MΩ. The pipettes descended in the recording chamber. After insertion in the brain, the resistance of some of the pipettes further decreased. The intracellular electrodes with resistance lower than 30 MΩ were discarded because intracellular recordings obtained with these electrodes often revealed signs of deterioration appearing as unstable membrane potential, which becomes progressively depolarized, and broadening of action potential duration. Typically, the recording sessions lasted up to 3 h. Only recordings showing stable Vm (no drift) and overshooting action potentials are considered here. To correctly estimate the membrane potential, we recorded the potential drop during penetration of the recorded neuron and after pulling out of the neuron. If the difference in the estimation of recorded membrane potential at the beginning and at the end of recording exceeded 2 mV, these data were discarded. A total of 118 neurons were recorded during the wake/sleep cycle, including 96 presumed excitatory cells and 22 presumed inhibitory interneurons (see Results).
To accumulate sufficient statistics for the analysis (see below), recordings needed to last typically >30 min and be conducted using several levels of injected different holding currents (DCs). A total of 15 neurons satisfying these criteria were obtained and were used for conductance analysis. In some cases, the recording was long enough to span across several states of vigilance [quiet wakefulness, slow-wave sleep, and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep]. Experiments were conducted in agreement with ethics guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care and were approved by the committee for animal care of Laval University.
Analysis and computational models.
The basis of the analysis was to match the Vm of the intracellularly recorded neurons to a stochastic model of synaptic background activity. This model was used for estimating synaptic conductances and to perform spike-triggered averages of synaptic conductance variations related to spikes, as well as for simulating membrane dynamics based on those estimates. The methods related to these different approaches are described below.
The basis of the analysis was a “fluctuating conductance model,” which approximates the highly complex synaptic activity as a random process, using global synaptic conductances described by the following stochastic equation (Destexhe et al., 2001): where ge(t) is the global excitatory conductance, fluctuating around the mean value ge0 and with variance ςe2, τe is the excitatory time constant, and ξe(t) is a Gaussian white noise source with zero mean and unit SD. The global inhibitory conductance gi(t) is described by an equivalent equation with parameters gi0, ςi2, τi and noise source ξi(t). These conductances determine the subthreshold variations of the membrane potential V (t), according to the following membrane equation: where C denotes the membrane capacitance, Iext a stimulation current, GL the leak conductance, and EL the leak reversal potential, whereas Ee and Ei are the reversal potentials of ge(t) and gi(t), respectively.
The advantage of this stochastic model is that different analytic approximations are available for the steady-state Vm distribution (VmD). The VmD method consists of matching this analytic expression to Vm distributions obtained experimentally, leading to estimates of excitatory and inhibitory conductances, as well as their variance, solely from current-clamp recordings (for a detailed discussion and testing of this method, see Rudolph et al., 2004). The method is based on the assumption of Gaussian distribution for synaptic conductances. The validity of this approximation was tested in numerical simulations (Destexhe et al., 2001; Rudolph and Destexhe, 2003, 2005) and theoretical investigations (Rudolph and Destexhe, 2006), as well as experimental studies (Rudolph et al., 2004, 2005). Vm distributions must be obtained at least at two DC levels, and using multiple current levels allows one to identify the linear range of the voltage–current (V–I) relationships (see Fig. 3A). This is an important check to limit the possible contamination of subthreshold voltage-dependent conductances. To insure that only subthreshold activity is used for the analysis, action potentials, if present, were removed by excluding all Vm activity within a window of 5 ms before and 10 ms after the spike. To avoid the possible bias caused by removing compound EPSPs before spikes, the VmD estimates were performed in hyperpolarized segments with little spiking activity; the testing of this method using dynamic-clamp experiments indicates that such a bias is negligible (Rudolph et al., 2004).
Because two DC levels are necessary to estimate conductances, n > 2 levels provides multiple estimates corresponding to different ranges of Vm. Up to n = 8 current levels were used here, which leads up to 28 possible pairings. For each pairing, the fitting of the Vm distributions with the analytic expression [in particular its Gaussian approximation (Rudolph et al., 2004)] yields estimates of the synaptic conductances and their variances, provided a number of other parameters are kept fixed. These parameters are the synaptic reversal potentials (Ee = 0 mV and Ei = −75 mV) and the decay time constants (τe = 3 ms and τi = 10 ms) of synaptic conductances, as well as the leak conductance (GL) (see Results) and reversal potential (EL = −80 mV). The values of τe = 3 ms and τi = 10 ms were obtained from the power spectra of Vm activity (Rudolph et al., 2005). The leak parameters were estimated based on previous intracellular experiments realized on neurons of the same area of cat cortex (area 5–7) after application of tetrodotoxin (Paré et al., 1998): after suppression of network activity, the membrane conductance was reduced by approximately five times, and the resting Vm was of −80 mV on average. The “optimal values” of conductance presented here (see Fig. 3C) were based on the assumption that the up states of these experiments (ketamine–xylazine anesthesia) have the same relative impact on the membrane as the up states of natural slow-wave sleep analyzed here. This assumption is supported by the fact that the absolute values of input resistance during up states are similar in the two preparations (Paré et a., 1998; Steriade et al., 2001). We performed variations around this estimate (see Results) to check that the results obtained do not qualitatively depend on this assumption. Note that the values of the conductance variances are not affected by the particular choice of the leak conductances (Rudolph et al., 2004) (see Results).
We also performed a spike-triggered average (STA) conductance analysis from Vm activity. The average conductance time course related to spikes was estimated using a method outlined previously, and which was tested in real neurons with dynamic clamp (Pospischil et al., 2007). The STA of the voltage is calculated first, and the method searches for the “most likely” spike-related conductance time courses [ge(t), gi(t)] that are compatible with the observed voltage STA. This estimation is made by discretizing the time axis in Equations 1 and 2, which leads to a discretized system from which one can evaluate the probability distribution for the spike-related conductance time courses. Because this distribution is symmetric, the STAs of the synaptic conductances can be determined by maximizing its probability distribution, which amounts to differentiating the distribution and subsequently solving a system of algebraic equations (Pospischil et al., 2007). The system of algebraic equations is solved using standard numerical methods (Press et al., 1986).
To quantify the conductance STA, we fitted the conductance time course using the exponential template for excitation, and an equivalent equation for inhibition. Here, t0 stands for the time of the spike, ke quantifies the maximal increase/decrease of conductance before the spike, with time constant Te (and similarly for inhibition). We also calculated the “relative conductance change” before the spike: Here, the terms ge0ke and gi0ki quantify the absolute excitatory and inhibitory conductance change before the spike, respectively. The difference between these two contributions is normalized to the total synaptic conductance. A negative value indicates an overall drop of total membrane conductance before the spike (see Fig. 9A), whereas a positive value indicates an increase of total conductance (supplemental Fig. 4, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).
To relate the STA analysis to the VmD analysis, we quantified the “relative excess conductance” by calculating the quantity: Here, a negative value indicates a membrane dominated by inhibitory conductance, whereas a positive value indicates dominant excitatory conductance.
Similarly, we calculated the “relative excess conductance fluctuations” by evaluating the quantity: Finally, the fluctuating conductance model (Eq. 2) was simulated numerically together with voltage-dependent Na+ and K+ conductances responsible for generating action potentials [Hodgkin–Huxley type models with parameters identical as those described by Destexhe et al., (2001)]. The conductance values obtained from intracellular data were integrated in this model to verify that the Vm activity and firing behavior obtained match the recordings of the corresponding cell. Simulations were performed on Linux workstations using the Neuron simulation environment (Hines and Carnevale, 1997).
Results
Intracellular recordings in awake and naturally sleeping animals
Intracellular recordings of cortical neurons were performed in parietal cortex of awake and naturally sleeping cats (see Materials and Methods). Intracellular recordings were done simultaneously with the LFP, EMG, and EOG to identify behavioral states. With pipettes filled with K+-acetate, we recorded and electrophysiologically identified activities of 96 presumed excitatory neurons during the waking state. Of them, 47 neurons revealed a regular-spiking (RS) firing pattern, with significant spike-frequency adaptation in response to depolarizing current pulses, and spike width of 0.69 ± 0.20 ms (range, 0.4–1.5 ms). The Vm of RS neurons varied in a range between −56 and −76 mV (mean, −64.0 ± 5.9 mV). Twenty-six of these RS neurons were “wake-active” cells in which the firing was sustained all through the wake state, as described previously (Matsumura et al., 1988; Baranyi et al., 1993; Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001). In these wake-active neurons (Fig. 1A), the Vm was depolarized (around −65 mV) and showed high-amplitude fluctuations and sustained irregular firing (3.1 Hz on average; range, 1–11 Hz) during wakefulness. During slow-wave sleep, all neurons always showed up and down states in the Vm activity in phase with the slow-waves (Fig. 1A, SWS), as described previously (Steriade et al., 2001).
Almost half of the RS neurons recorded (21 of 47), were “wake-silent” cells, which systematically ceased firing during periods of quiet wakefulness (Fig. 1B). Note that during the transition from slow-wave sleep to waking, these wake-silent neurons continued to fire for 10–60 s, and after that period, their Vm hyperpolarized by several millivolts and they stopped to fire action potentials as long as the animal remained in the state of quiet wakefulness. Figure 1B illustrates one example of a wake-silent cell that, after awakening, had a Vm of −53.0 ± 4.9 mV and fired with frequency 10.1 ± 7.9 Hz for ∼30 s. Thereafter, the Vm hyperpolarized to −62.5 ± 2.6 mV and the same neuron stopped firing. The hyperpolarization during waking state was not caused by K+ load because on two occasions we were able to obtain intracellular recordings from wake-silent neurons during waking state that was preceded and followed by other states of vigilance (supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). In this case, the recorded neuron was relatively depolarized and fired action potentials during REM sleep. After awakening, this neuron was hyperpolarized by ∼10 mV and stopped firing. After 3 min of waking state, the animal went to slow-wave sleep state and the same neuron was depolarized and started to fire action potentials. On one occasion (data not shown), we recorded extracellularly spikes from two units. One of the units stopped firing during waking state lasting ∼10 min whereas another unit continued to emit action potentials. This observation suggests that it is a particular set of neurons and not local networks that stop firing during quiet wakefulness. The mean firing rates for RS neurons were 6.1 ± 6.7 Hz (silent neurons included; 10.1 ± 5.6 Hz with silent neurons excluded). No wake-silent cells were observed for other neuronal classes than RS cells and, all together, wake-silent neurons represented ∼25% of the total number of recorded cells in the wake state. This large proportion of wake-silent neurons constitutes a first hint for an important role for inhibitory conductances during waking.
In contrast, no silent neuron was found for presumed interneurons. During quiet wakefulness, 22 neurons were electrophysiologically identified as fast-spiking (FS). They displayed virtually no adaptation and had action potential width of 0.27 ± 0.08 ms (range, 0.15–0.45 ms). After awakening, FS neurons tended to increase firing (Fig. 2A,B), and none of them was found to cease firing (n = 9). Interestingly, the increase of firing of FS neurons seems to follow the steady hyperpolarization of RS wake-silent neurons (Fig. 2A). The mean firing frequency of FS neurons was 28.8 ± 20.4 Hz (range, 1–88 Hz; only two neurons fired with frequency <2 Hz), which was significantly higher than that of RS neurons (p ≤ 0.001) (Fig. 2C). The mean Vm of FS neurons was −61.3 ± 4.5 mV, which is not significantly different from the Vm of RS neurons (p = 0.059).
To check for the contribution of K+ conductances during quiet wakefulness, we recorded activities of three RS neurons with Cs+-filled pipettes (data not shown). The presence of cesium greatly affected the repolarizing phase of action potentials, demonstrating that Cs+ was effective in blocking K+ conductances, but the Vm distribution was marginally affected by the presence of cesium. The action of intracellular Cs+ may overlap with the blocking action of neuromodulators on other K+ conductances (McCormick, 1992; Metherate and Ashe, 1993), which may explain the absence of effect of Cs+ on the Vm. This preliminary evidence for a limited effect of cesium during wakefulness indicates that leak and K+ conductances have no major effect on the Vm distribution, suggesting that it is mainly determined by synaptic conductances.
Synaptic conductances in wakefulness and natural sleep
To determine the relative contribution of excitatory and inhibitory conductances, we have analyzed the intracellular recordings described above using the previously proposed VmD method (Rudolph et al., 2004). This method assumes that the Vm fluctuations are caused by the combined action of two synaptic conductances, excitatory (ge) and inhibitory (gi) (see Materials and Methods). Instead of modeling explicitly the activity of thousands of synapses, one considers the Vm and synaptic conductances as stochastic variables, which are described by probability densities. Using this formalism, it is possible to obtain an analytic expression for the steady-state probability distribution of the Vm, which is expressed as a function of the synaptic conductance parameters (Rudolph and Destexhe, 2003, 2005). Because the steady-state Vm distribution is observable experimentally, fitting the analytic expression to experimental Vm distributions provides estimates of the underlying synaptic conductances. This method was tested using dynamic-clamp methods and was shown to provide consistent estimates of synaptic conductances and their fluctuations (Rudolph et al., 2004).
We computed Vm distributions for periods of stationary activity during wakefulness and slow-wave sleep up states (SWS-up). Figure 3B (awake) shows Vm distributions of two different but representative cells obtained from periods of wakefulness in which the cat and the LFPs did not show any sign of drowsiness. The Vm distribution was approximately Gaussian, centered around V̄ = −63.1 mV, and the SD of the Vm (ςV) was ∼3.6 mV. During slow-wave sleep, we calculated the Vm distribution specifically during up states (Fig. 3B, SWS). It had an approximately similar shape as during wakefulness (V̄ = −62.7 mV; ςV = 3.3 mV). Similar distributions were also observed during REM sleep (data not shown). The Vm distributions were computed using several pairs of DC levels, which were selected in the linear portion of the V–I relationship (Fig. 3A). The conductance estimates obtained for several of such pairings (see Materials and Methods) are represented in Figure 3C. In this example, during both wakefulness and slow-wave sleep up states, the inhibitory conductances were several-fold larger than excitatory conductances. Variations of different parameters, such as the leak conductances (Fig. 3D), or the parameters of synaptic conductances (see Materials and Methods), affected the absolute values of conductance estimates, but always pointed to the same qualitative effect of dominant inhibition. The sole exception was when considering high leak conductances, larger than the synaptic activity itself, in which case the excitatory and inhibitory conductance were of comparable magnitude (Fig. 3D, asterisk).
The VmD method also provides estimates of the variance of synaptic conductances. Similar to absolute conductance estimates, conductance variances were generally larger for inhibition (Fig. 3C). However, in contrast to absolute conductance estimates, the estimates of conductance variance do not depend on the particular choice of leak conductances (Fig. 3D, bottom). These results suggest that inhibition provides a major contribution to the Vm fluctuations.
This pattern was observed in the majority of cells analyzed, although a diversity of conductance combinations was present when considering the different states of vigilance, including periods of REM sleep. In cells for which synaptic conductances were estimated (n = 11 for awake, wake-active cells only; n = 7 for SWS up states; n = 2 for REM), the average Vm and fluctuation amplitude were comparable in all states [awake, V̄ = −54.2 ± 7.5 mV, ςV = 2.4 ± 0.7 mV; SWS-up, V̄ = −58.3 ± 4.9 mV, ςV = 2.7 ± 0.5 mV; SWS down states (SWS-down), V̄ = −67.0 ± 6.9 mV, ςV = 1.9 ± 0.6 mV; REM, V̄ = −58.5 ± 5.2 mV, ςV = 2.1 ± 0.9 mV] (Fig. 4A). However, the total input resistance showed important variations (awake, 16.1 ± 14.5 MΩ; SWS-up, 12.3 ± 19.6 MΩ; SWS-down, 22.4 ± 31.7 MΩ; REM, 8.5 ± 12.1 MΩ), possibly caused by differences in the passive properties and cellular morphologies. The estimated synaptic conductances spread over a large range of values for both mean (ranging from 5 to 70 nS and 5 to 170 nS for excitation and inhibition, respectively; medians: 21 and 55 nS for excitation and inhibition during SWS-up, 13 and 21 nS for excitation and inhibition for awake, respectively) (Fig. 4B,C) and SD (ranging from 1.5 to 22 nS and 3.5 to 83 nS for excitation and inhibition, respectively; medians: 7.6 and 9.3 nS for excitation and inhibition for SWS-up, 4.3 and 7.7 nS for excitation and inhibition for awake, respectively) (Fig. 5A,B). In all states and for reasonable assumptions for the leak conductance (Fig. 4D, gray), dominant inhibition was found in more than half of the cells analyzed (n = 6 for awake and n = 7 for SWS-up had >40% larger mean inhibitory conductance; n = 6 for awake and n = 4 for SWS-up had > 40% larger inhibitory SD). In the remaining cells, inhibitory and excitatory conductance values were of comparable magnitude, with a tendency for a slight dominance of inhibition (except for n = 2 cells in awake). Moreover, in all cells analyzed, inhibition was more pronounced during the up states of SWS (estimated ratios between inhibition and excitation were 2.7 ± 1.4 and 3.0 ± 2.2 for conductance mean and SD; medians were 1.9 and 1.4, respectively) compared with wakefulness (ratios of 1.8 ± 1.1 and 1.9 ± 0.9 for conductance mean and SD; medians were 1.4 and 1.4, respectively) (Figs. 4C, 5B). Renormalizing the conductance values to the leak conductance for each cell in the wake state led to values that were more homogeneous (Fig. 4E). In this case, the excitatory conductance was of the order of the leak conductance (Fig. 4E, left) (0.81 ± 0.26), whereas inhibition was ∼1.5 times larger (Fig. 4E, right) (1.26 ± 0.31).
These results were also checked using the classic “ohmic” conductance analysis (see supplemental Methods, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). By integrating the Vm measurements in the various active states into the membrane equation (Eq. 8, supplemental Materials and Methods, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), we obtained estimates for the ratio between mean inhibitory (excitatory) conductances and the leak conductance for each cell (supplemental Fig. 2A, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). This and the pooled results for all available cells (supplemental Fig. 2B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) also indicate that the relative contribution of inhibition is several-fold larger than that of excitation for both wakefulness and SWS up states. Average values are ḡi/ḡe = 3.2 ± 1.3 for SWS-up and ḡi/ḡe = 1.7 ± 1.1 during wakefulness. Here also, these values were relatively robust against the choice of the leak conductance (supplemental Fig. 2C, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).
In a small subset of cells (n = 3), the recording was long enough to span across several wake and sleep states so that SWS and wakefulness could be directly compared. In agreement with the reduction of the average firing rate of RS neurons during the transition from SWS to wakefulness, we observed a reduction of the mean excitatory conductance (values during wakefulness were between 40 and 93% of those during SWS-up) and its fluctuation amplitude (between 45 and 85% of those observed during SWS-up). In contrast to the observed increase of the firing rate of interneurons during sleep–wake transitions, the inhibitory conductances also decreased markedly (values during wakefulness were between 35 and 60% for the mean conductance, and between 10 and 71% for the SD compared with corresponding values during SWS-up) (see Discussion).
Conductance time course during up- and down-state transitions
We next estimated the time evolution of conductances during up and down states of slow-wave sleep. We performed a similar analysis as above, with the difference that Vm distributions were not calculated by accumulating statistics only over time, but also over repeated trials. Several up states (n = 1, between 6 and 36 slow-wave oscillation cycles at eight DC levels) were selected and aligned with respect to the “down–up” transition as determined by the sharp LFP negativity (Fig. 6, left). The Vm distributions were then calculated within small (10 ms) windows before and after the transition. This procedure led to estimates of the time course of the conductances and their variances, as a function of time during “down–up” state transitions, and similarly for “up–down” transitions (Fig. 6, right). It is important to note that conductance changes were estimated here relative to the down state, and not with respect to rest, as above. This analysis showed that, for this particular cell, the onset of the up state is driven by excitation, whereas inhibitory conductances activate with a delay of ∼20 ms, after which they tend to dominate over excitation. Note that in this case, inhibition is only slightly larger than excitation, presumably because the reference state is here the down state, which does not represent the true resting state. In this cell, the end of the up state was preceded by a drop of inhibition (Fig. 6B, asterisk). The variance of inhibitory conductances was always larger than that of excitatory conductances (Fig. 6B, bottom).
Dynamics of spike initiation during activated states
To investigate how excitatory and inhibitory conductance dynamics affect spike initiation, we first simulated the above results using computational models. We used a spiking model with stochastic conductances (Eq. 2) (see Materials and Methods) whose parameters are given by the above estimates. Integrating the particular values of conductances shown in Figure 3C led the model to generate Vm activity in excellent agreement with the intracellular recordings (Fig. 7A,C, awake). All of the present conductance measurements during the waking state were simulated in a similar way and yielded Vm activity consistent with the recordings (two more examples with clearly dominant excitation or inhibition are shown in supplemental Fig. 3, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Similarly, integrating the conductance variations, given in Figure 6B, generated Vm activity consistent with the up–down state transitions seen experimentally (Fig. 7B,C, SWS, SWS-up). These results show that the conductance estimates obtained above are consistent with the Vm activity recorded experimentally.
We next evaluated the optimal conductance changes related to spike initiation in the simulated wake state. Figure 8A shows that the STA shows opposite variations for excitatory and inhibitory conductances preceding the spike. As expected, spikes were correlated to an increase of excitation (Fig. 8, excitatory). Less expected was that spikes were also correlated with a decrease of inhibitory conductance (Fig. 8, inhibitory), so that the total synaptic conductance decreases before the spike (Fig. 8, total). Such a drop of total conductance was not present in simulated states when inhibition was not dominant (Fig. 8B). These results were checked using different combinations of parameters, and a drop of total conductance was always associated with inhibition-dominant states, except when the variance of inhibition was very small (data not shown). Such a drop of total conductance before the spike therefore constitutes a good predictor for inhibition-dominant states, given that conductance fluctuations are roughly proportional to their means.
This prediction was tested using intracellular recordings of electrophysiologically identified RS cells. We performed STAs of the Vm during wakefulness and the up states of SWS (Fig. 9A, Avg Vm). The corresponding STA conductances were estimated by discretizing the time axis and solving the membrane equation (see Materials and Methods). The STA conductances demonstrated a drop of total membrane conductance preceding the spike (Fig. 9A, total), which occurred on a similar time scale compared with the model (compare Fig. 8A). The decomposition of this conductance into excitatory and inhibitory components shows that the inhibitory conductance drops before the spike, whereas the excitatory conductance shows a steeper increase just before the spike (Fig. 9A) (note that the latter increase is probably contaminated by voltage-dependent currents associated with spike generation). Such a pattern was observed in most of the cells tested (7 of 10 cells in awake, six of six cells in SWS-up, and two of two cells in REM) (Fig. 9B,C). An example of a neuron that did not show such a drop of total conductance is given in supplemental Figure 4 (available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Most of the cells, however, yielded STAs qualitatively equivalent to that of the model when inhibition is dominant (Fig. 8A).
Finally, we investigated whether the dominance of inhibition (as deduced from conductance analysis) and the drop of conductance (from STA analysis) are related by including all cells for which both analyses could be done (Fig. 10). The total conductance change before the spike was clearly related to the difference of excitatory and inhibitory conductance deduced from VmD analysis (Fig. 10A, gray area), indicating that cells dominated by inhibition generally gave rise to a drop of total conductance before the spike. However, there was no quantitative relationship between the amplitude of those changes. Such a quantitative relationship was obtained for conductance fluctuations (Fig. 10B), which indicates that the magnitude and sign of the conductance change before the spike is strongly related to the relative amount of excitatory and inhibitory conductance fluctuations. The clear correlation between the results of these two independent analyses therefore confirms that most neurons have strong and highly fluctuating inhibitory conductances during wake and sleep states.
Discussion
We presented here a combination of intracellular recordings and computational models of cortical neurons that points to the following conclusions: (1) Vm activity during wake and sleep states results from diverse combinations of excitatory and inhibitory conductances, with dominant inhibition in most of the cases; (2) inhibitory conductance fluctuations are generally larger than for excitation; (3) spike initiation is in most cases correlated with a decrease of inhibition, which appears as a transient drop of membrane conductance before the spike. We discuss below the significance of these findings.
It is important to note that the excitatory and inhibitory conductances were estimated here from somatic recordings. The values obtained therefore reflect the overall conductances as seen from the soma, after dendritic integration, and are necessarily different from the “total” conductance present in the soma and dendrites of the neuron. However, these somatic estimates are close to the conductance interplay underlying spike generation because the spike initiation zone (presumably in the axon) (Stuart et al., 1997) is electrotonically close to the soma.
Using such somatic estimates, dominant inhibition was seen in all cells during the up-states of slow-wave sleep (n = 7 cells had >40% larger inhibitory conductance), and in the majority of neurons recorded in the wake state (n = 6 cells showed clear dominant inhibitory conductances). This conclusion is robust to a number of parameter variations, including different hypotheses about the leak conductance. A notable exception is when high leak conductances (larger than synaptic activity) are assumed, in which case excitatory and inhibitory conductances are of comparable magnitude. However, this possibility is very unlikely in cats because high leak conductances would predict dramatic effects of cesium on the Vm distribution, which is not what we observed (I. Timofeev, unpublished observations). Moreover, intracellular measurements in cats show that the total conductance resulting from intense synaptic activity is several-fold larger than the leak conductance (Borg-Graham et al., 1998; Paré et al., 1998). Nevertheless, some neurons were found to have roughly equal excitatory and inhibitory conductances during waking for reasonable values of the leak conductance (Fig. 4D). These results suggest that there exists a diversity of combinations of excitation and inhibition, which varies from cell to cell in the same network, as also shown previously for visual responses in anesthetized cats (Monier et al., 2003).
However, with the exception of one cell, the variance of inhibition was always larger than that of excitation, for all parameters tested. In contrast to conductance estimates, the VmD estimate of conductance variance does not depend on any hypothesis about the leak conductance. This high level of inhibitory fluctuations is consistent with previous measurements in cortical neurons under anesthesia (Hasenstaub et al., 2005; Rudolph et al., 2005). It suggests that a major part of Vm fluctuations is caused by variations of inhibitory conductances. Because variations of Vm are most effective on evoking spikes, rather than the absolute Vm level (Mainen and Sejnowski, 1995), these results suggest that inhibition is particularly effective in evoking spikes (see below).
Interestingly, we also found that a significant proportion of excitatory neurons cease firing during the wake state. Such wake-silent cells were also observed in motor cortex of young rats using whole-cell recordings (Brecht et al., 2004). The high proportion of these wake-silent cells (44.6%) suggests that there should be less excitatory conductance during wakefulness compared with slow-wave sleep, which is indeed what we observed. Interneurons also show diverse behaviors: the majority of FS cells recorded show an increase of firing in the wake state, whereas a minority shows the opposite (data not shown). In contrast, all excitatory cells analyzed in the sleep-wake transition (n = 3) show a decrease of inhibition at the transition to waking. This discrepancy can be explained by bias in our sampling of different populations of interneurons, which are known to display opposite sensitivity to neuromodulators (Xiang et al., 1998), or by a different state of depression/potentiation at inhibitory synapses, depending on behavioral state. In agreement with a previous study under anesthesia (Rudolph et al., 2005), these differences also suggest that up states and EEG-activated states stem from similar, but nonidentical network states.
In contrast with the present results, previous conductance measurements showed that, paradoxically, up and down states have a comparable input resistance in rat barrel cortex in vivo (Zou et al., 2005; Waters and Helmchen, 2006). The difference is not caused by contamination by action-potential related Na+ currents, as suggested by Waters and Helmchen (2006), because the spikes are removed in our VmD analysis and the V–I relationships were linear. Indeed, applying the VmD method to barrel cortex in vivo also gave the same paradoxical results (Zou et al., 2005), which were radically different from those in the association cortex of cats using the same method (Rudolph et al., 2005) (present study). High-conductance states have also been observed in vivo in cat visual cortex (Borg-Graham et al., 1998; Hirsch et al., 1998; Anderson et al., 2000), in cat association cortex (Paré et al., 1998; Rudolph et al., 2005), and in ferret visual cortex (Haider et al., 2006), which suggests fundamental differences of network dynamics in different cortical regions.
The present evidence for dominant inhibition also contrasts with the roughly equal conductances measured in voltage-clamp during spontaneous up-states in ferret cortical slices (Shu et al., 2003) or in vivo (Haider et al., 2006). Although we also observed neurons with roughly equal conductances (n = 5 for wake, none for SWS-up), this does not explain the differences. A possible explanation is that those voltage-clamp measurements were performed in the presence of Na+ and K+ channel blockers (QX314 and cesium), and these drugs affect somatodendritic attenuation by reducing the resting conductance. Consequently, excitatory events located in dendrites have a more powerful impact on the soma compared with the intact neuron, which may explain the discrepancy. Another possible explanation is that, in voltage-clamp experiments, when the voltage clamp is applied from the soma, the more distal regions of the cell are unlikely to be clamped, which may result in errors in estimating conductances and reversal potentials. Moreover, in this case, the presence of uncompensated electrode series resistance may worsen the estimates or affect the ratio between excitation and inhibition (for simulations, see supplemental Table 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Additional conductance measurements should be performed in nonanesthetized animals to address these issues. However, our results are in agreement with conductance measurements performed in cortical neurons in vivo under anesthesia, which also show evidence for dominant inhibitory conductances (Borg-Graham et al., 1998; Hirsch et al., 1998; Destexhe et al., 2003; Rudolph et al., 2005).
Not only inhibition seems to provide a major contribution to the conductance state of the membrane, but also the conductance variations, which are larger for inhibition compared with excitation. This suggests that inhibition widely contributes to setting the Vm fluctuations and, therefore, it presumably has a strong influence on action-potential firing. This hypothesis was tested in computational models, which predicted that when inhibition is dominant, spikes are correlated with a previous decrease of inhibition, rather than an increase of excitation. This decrease of inhibition should be visible as a membrane conductance decrease before the spike, which is indeed what we observed in most neurons analyzed in wake and sleep states (Fig. 9). A prominent role for inhibition is also supported by previous intracellular recordings demonstrating a time locking of inhibitory events with action potentials in awake animals (Timofeev et al., 2001), and the powerful role of inhibitory fluctuations on spiking in anesthetized states (Hasenstaub et al., 2005). Together, these results suggest that strong inhibition is not a consequence of anesthesia, but rather represents a property generally seen in awake and natural sleep states, pleading for a powerful role for interneurons in determining neuronal selectivity and information processing.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Agence Nationale de la Recherche, the Human Frontier Science Program, and the European Community (A.D.), and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (I.T.). I.T. is a scholar of the CIHR. We dedicate this paper to the memory of Mircea Steriade, and we are very grateful to him for many stimulating discussions and continuous support.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Alain Destexhe, Integrative and Computational Neuroscience Unit, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, 91198 Gif-sur-Yvette, France. destexhe{at}iaf.cnrs-gif.fr