Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Unconscious Activation of the Cognitive Control System in the Human Prefrontal Cortex

Hakwan C. Lau and Richard E. Passingham
Journal of Neuroscience 23 May 2007, 27 (21) 5805-5811; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4335-06.2007
Hakwan C. Lau
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Richard E. Passingham
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Psychological task. In each trial, the instruction figure (i.e., the bigger figure that is hollow in the middle) determined which task the volunteers had to perform. If the instruction figure was a square (right), the volunteers had to decide whether the word was bisyllabic (i.e., has two syllables). If the instruction figure was a diamond (left), the volunteers had to decide whether the upcoming word referred to concrete objects. Before the presentation of the instruction, the prime was presented. The prime was a small figure occupying the central space left out in the instruction figure and could also be either square-shaped or diamond-shaped. The instruction figure did not overlap spatially with the prime figure, but their contours did, such that the instruction figure acted as a metacontrast mask for the prime and reduced the visibility of the prime. This was particularly true when the SOA was at 83 ms, as in the Low-Visibility conditions, compared with High-Visibility conditions when the SOA was at 16 ms.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Behavioral demonstration of the priming effect. HiVis and LoVis, respectively, refer to the high and low visibility of the primes. Con and InCon refer to the congruency and incongruency between the primes and the instructions. When the primes were incongruent with the instructions, volunteers were primed to perform the alternative (i.e., wrong) task, and their accuracies were lower (left) and response times bigger (right). Interestingly, this was particularly true when the visibility of the primes was low, as reflected by interactions between the factors of Congruency and Visibility in both the accuracies (p = 0.001) and the response times (p < 0.0005). This justifies the claim that the priming effect operates fundamentally at an unconscious level.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Neural activity associated with priming. Data were extracted from the brain areas that were previously found to be associated with the Phonological task (left ventral premotor area) and the Semantic task (left inferior frontal cortex and middle temporal gyrus). The location of these is schematically illustrated on the brain above. Here, task relevant means activity from the Semantic areas when the volunteers were instructed to perform the Semantic task, and activity from the Phonological areas when they were instructed to perform the Phonological task. Task irrelevant means the activity was extracted from the alternative areas, which was more important for the primed task than the instructed task. When the visibility of the primes was low (LoVis), which means the priming effect was strongest (see Fig. 2), activity in task-relevant areas was significantly reduced (p = 0.019), and activity in task-irrelevant areas was significantly increased (p = 0.045). This suggests that the volunteers were actually engaged in exercising the wrong neural circuits when they were primed to perform the wrong task. This effect is not present when the visibility of the primes was high (HiVis), suggesting that this effect could not be attributable to the degree of conscious perception of the prime. This is reflected by a three-way interaction between Task Relevance (i.e., activity in task relevant areas vs activity in task irrelevant areas), Congruency (Con; between prime and instruction), and Visibility (of the prime) (p = 0.040). InCon, Incongruency; n.s., not significant.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Mid-DLPFC and unconscious priming. We looked for activity in the brain that was associated with the unconscious priming effect in general, regardless of which task was explicitly cued, by testing for the interaction between Congruency (Con; between prime and instruction) and Visibility (of the prime). This test revealed activity in the mid-DLPFC (right; x = −38). This effect was specific to the Low-Visibility condition, in which volunteers did not consciously perceive the primes. InCon, Incongruency; n.s., not significant; HiVis, high visibility; LoVis, low visibility.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Task design

    Task
    CongruencyPhonologicalSemantic
    CongruentCongruent
    PhonologicalSemantic
    IncongruentIncongruent
    • Volunteers could be instructed to perform either the Phonological task or the Semantic task, and the prime in each trial could either be Congruent or Incongruent with the instruction. These were repeated for the conditions in which the primes were of either High Visibility or Low Visibility. Together, these constitute a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design, with Task, Congruency, and Prime Visibility as experimental factors.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 27 (21)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 27, Issue 21
23 May 2007
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Unconscious Activation of the Cognitive Control System in the Human Prefrontal Cortex
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Unconscious Activation of the Cognitive Control System in the Human Prefrontal Cortex
Hakwan C. Lau, Richard E. Passingham
Journal of Neuroscience 23 May 2007, 27 (21) 5805-5811; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4335-06.2007

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Unconscious Activation of the Cognitive Control System in the Human Prefrontal Cortex
Hakwan C. Lau, Richard E. Passingham
Journal of Neuroscience 23 May 2007, 27 (21) 5805-5811; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4335-06.2007
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Influence of Reward on Corticospinal Excitability during Movement Preparation
  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.