Abstract
Much data suggests that hippocampal–medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) interactions support memory consolidation. This process is thought to involve the gradual transfer of transient hippocampal-dependent memories to distributed neocortical sites for long-term storage. However, hippocampal projections to the neocortex involve a multisynaptic pathway that sequentially progresses through the entorhinal and perirhinal regions before reaching the neocortex. Similarly, the mPFC influences the hippocampus via the rhinal cortices, suggesting that the rhinal cortices occupy a strategic position in this network. The present study thus tested the idea that the mPFC supports memory by facilitating the transfer of hippocampal activity to the neocortex via an enhancement of entorhinal to perirhinal communication. To this end, we simultaneously recorded mPFC, perirhinal, and entorhinal neurons during the acquisition of a trace-conditioning task in which a visual conditioned stimulus (CS) was followed by a delay period after which a liquid reward was administered. At learning onset, correlated perirhinal-entorhinal firing increased in relation to mPFC activity, but with no preferential directionality, and only after reward delivery. However, as learning progressed across days, mPFC activity gradually enhanced rhinal correlations in relation to the CS as well, and did so in a specific direction: from entorhinal to perirhinal neurons. This suggests that, at late stages of learning, mPFC activity facilitates entorhinal to perirhinal communication. Because this connection is a necessary step for the transfer of hippocampal activity to the neocortex, our results suggest that the mPFC is involved in the slow iterative process supporting the integration of hippocampal-dependent memories into neocortical networks.
Introduction
The hippocampus plays a time-limited role in the formation of declarative memories, with memories gradually becoming independent of the hippocampus over time (Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1986; Kim and Fanselow, 1992). It is believed that these remote memories are gradually transferred from the hippocampus to the neocortex for long-term storage (Wiltgen et al., 2004). In keeping with this, the hippocampus and neocortex show inverse gradients of activity in relation to the retrieval of recent versus remote spatial memories (Bontempi et al., 1999). In particular, remote memories produce stronger activations in temporal and frontal neocortical areas than recent ones and inversely in the hippocampus (Bontempi et al., 1999; Takashima et al., 2006).
A related line of investigation indicates that the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is critical for the consolidation of hippocampal-dependent memories. In trace-conditioning tasks for instance, hippocampal lesions cause a severe deficit when made soon after training, but not after a month, whereas mPFC lesions produce the opposite pattern of impairments (Takehara et al., 2003). Consistent with this, expression of the immediate early gene zif268 is enhanced in the mPFC by remote, but not recent, contextual memory tests (Frankland et al., 2004). Moreover, local intra-mPFC injections of NMDA antagonists within 2 weeks after training impair long-term recall, implicating the mPFC in memory consolidation (Takehara-Nishiuchi et al., 2006). Last, functional imaging studies in humans have revealed that as the retention interval increases, memory retrieval produces gradually stronger activations in the mPFC whereas the opposite is seen in the hippocampus (Takashima et al., 2006).
Overall, this data suggests that in the first few weeks after training, memories become gradually independent of the hippocampus and that, in parallel, they progressively become more dependent on mPFC activity.
Although hippocampal projections to the mPFC (Swanson, 1981; Ferino et al., 1987; Jay and Witter, 1991; Thierry et al., 2000) likely support mPFC involvement in memory, the role of mPFC activity in memory formation remains unclear. One possibility is that mPFC affects the transfer of hippocampal activity toward the neocortex. Consistent with this possibility, the mPFC projects to the rhinal cortices (Room et al., 1985; Sesack et al., 1989; Hurley et al., 1991), the main route for impulse traffic into and out of the hippocampus. Indeed, most hippocampal projections to the neocortex involve a multisynaptic pathway that sequentially progresses through the entorhinal (ER) cortex, perirhinal (PR) areas 35 and 36, and then the neocortex. Moreover, pretraining ER lesions impair acquisition on a trace-conditioning task (Ryou et al., 2001) and multiple lines of evidence implicate the rhinal cortices in memory formation (Meunier et al., 1993; Suzuki and Eichenbaum, 2000; Muir and Bilkey, 2003; Murray et al., 2005; Leutgeb et al., 2005). Thus, the present study was undertaken to test the idea that the mPFC influences memory formation by modulating interactions between the neocortex and hippocampus at the level of the rhinal cortices. To this end, we examined the relative timing of unit activity in the mPFC, PR, and ER cortices during the acquisition of a trace-conditioning task.
Materials and Methods
Surgery
Three adult cats were implanted with two arrays of high-impedance microelectrodes aimed at the mPFC (see Fig. 1 A1) and the rhinal cortices (see Fig. 1 A2). All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Rutgers University, in compliance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Department of Health and Human Services).
Cats were preanesthetized with ketamine and xylazine (15 and 2 mg/kg, i.m.) and artificially ventilated with a mixture of ambient air, oxygen, and isoflurane. Atropine (0.05 mg/kg, i.m.) was administered. The end-tidal CO2 concentration was maintained at 3.7 ± 0.2%, and the body temperature at 37–38°C with a heating pad. Bupivacaine (subcutaneous) was administered in the region to be incised 15 min before the first incision. In sterile conditions, an incision was performed on the midline of the scalp and the skull muscles were retracted. After trepanation and opening of the dura mater, an array of microelectrodes was stereotaxically lowered until the electrodes reached the deep rhinal layers. A second array was inserted in the mPFC with an oblique, lateromedial approach (Fig. 1 B).
Finally, four screws were cemented to the skull to later fix the cat's head without pain or pressure. At the end of the surgery, the animals were administered penicillin (20,000 UI/kg, i.m.) and an analgesic (Ketophen, 2 mg/kg, s.c., daily for 3 d). Recording sessions began 8 d after the surgery.
Behavior
The learning task was an appetitive trace-conditioning paradigm in which a visual CS (1.5 s) was followed by a 1.5 s delay period, after which a liquid reward was administered. Trials occurred at random intervals of 30–90 s. The animals performed 38–95 trials (median 55.5) per daily session for 9 consecutive days. The liquid reward (2 ml/trial) was a preferred food [Gerber's (Fremont, MI) pureed baby food “sweet potatoes and turkey”]. The animals were only fed during recording sessions, and their weight was monitored daily to maintain body weight within 10% of its initial value.
Behavior was monitored by means of a switch detecting when the tongue of the animals contacted the receptacle where the food reward was administered. The visual CS was a global change in the illumination (from black to white) of a 12 inch liquid crystal display (LCD) screen placed one foot in from of the animals. Detection of the visual CS did not necessitate that the animals maintain a fixed gaze at the center of the LCD screen because it encompassed most of their visual field. Because the animals were hungry, they were highly aroused and remained awake at all times (as assessed by EEG recordings) with their eyes opened.
Histology
At the end of the experiments, the animals were given an overdose of sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, i.v.) and perfused fixed. The recording sites were marked with electrolytic lesions (0.5 mA, 5 s) (see Fig. 1 B). The brains were later sectioned on a vibrating microtome (at 100 μm) and stained with cresyl violet to verify the position of recording electrodes. Microelectrode tracks were reconstructed by combining micrometer readings with the histology. This report includes only neurons that were histologically confirmed to be located in the regions of interest.
Recording and analysis methods
Neuronal activity was sampled at 100 μm intervals. Each time the electrodes were moved to a new site (once a day), 30 min elapsed before data were acquired to ensure mechanical stability. The signals picked up by the electrodes (Fig. 1 C) were observed on an oscilloscope, digitized, and stored on a hard disk. Spike sorting was performed off-line using a clustering algorithm based on principal component analysis and K-means.
Computation of spike-triggered joint histograms.
To analyze the relative timing of mPFC, PR, and ER activity, we computed spike-triggered joint histograms (STJHs), as described in Results. The STJH is an adaptation of the joint peristimulus time histogram (JPSTH) method (Aertsen et al., 1989) where, instead of external stimuli, mPFC spikes are used as a temporal reference to study correlated PR and ER firing. Thus, like JPSTHs, STJHs are “trial-based,” only instead of stimuli, as in the standard JPSTHs, our STJHs were mPFC-spike-based. The STJH is computed by taking ±150 ms segments of rhinal activity around mPFC spikes (time 0), and by plotting the spikes of the PR cell on the x-axis and of the ER cell on the y-axis. The bins of the STJH that contain a coincidence of spikes are incremented. Repeating this process for each mPFC spike gradually produces the raw STJH. For presentation purposes only, the STJHs were smoothed using a two-dimensional Gaussian with variance equal to 25 ms.
Statistical significance of STJHs.
This was assessed by performing bin by bin (10 ms) comparisons of the raw STJH with two control matrices (see Fig. 2) (see Results, A method to examine correlated rhinal activity time locked to mPFC firing). The first control matrix was used to test that peaks in the STJH are indeed locked to mPFC firing. We generated surrogate mPFC spike trains with a similar number of spikes as in the original spike trains, but with spike times selected from a random homogenous distribution. The STJH was then recalculated and this process was repeated 50 times. The random STJHs were averaged to form the control matrix. The second control matrix tests that peaks in the STJH are not merely caused by independent rhinal responses to the mPFC (a “shift predictor”). To address this, the set of segments of perirhinal activity surrounding mPFC spikes was shuffled and then matched to the set of segments of entorhinal activity surrounding mPFC spikes. In other words, each segment of perirhinal activity around an mPFC spike was now matched to an entorhinal segment surrounding a different mPFC spike. The process was repeated 50 times, each time calculating a new STJH, and the result averaged to form the control matrix. STJHs were considered significant if they differed from both control STJHs [p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected for the number of bins (900)]. When computing ratios of significant STJHs as a function of a behavioral event (CS or reward) during a trial, we used nonoverlapping windows of 0.5 s. We also tested windows of 250 ms or 1 s and this produced qualitatively identical results.
Although visual inspection of the STJHs indicated that the vast majority of them had a single dominant peak, this was quantified by calculating the DIP statistical test for unimodality (Hartigan and Hartigan, 1985). Using this approach, we found that as many as 76% of the STJHs were unimodal.
Computation of directionality index.
We computed a directionality index to assess the direction of impulse traffic in the rhinal cortices in relation to mPFC activity. This was obtained by separately adding all bins above the main diagonal (a) versus below the main diagonal (b) in the STJHs and then computing (a − b/a + b) for each STJH. The directionality index ranges from −1 to 1 and a positive value indicates that the PR cell tended to fire before the ER cell and inversely for negative values.
Statistical significance of cross-correlograms.
To determine whether cross-correlograms (CCs) were statistically significant, long epochs of spontaneous activity were segmented randomly. Then, we shuffled the segments of one neuron with respect to the other, and recomputed the CCs 100 times, thus generating 100 control CCs. Each center bin (±50 ms) from the original CC was compared against the randomized distribution for that bin. If any bin was significantly different at the p < 0.05 level, the CC was deemed significant. We did not compensate for multiple comparisons of the number of center bins, thus biasing the results toward a higher proportion of significant CCs. Despite this, a low proportion of significant PR–ER CCs was observed (see Results).
Identifying aspects of mPFC activity that correlate with significant STJHs.
We considered three factors: (1) firing rate, (2) firing pattern (spike clustering), and (3) synchrony between simultaneously recorded mPFC cells. Spike clustering (2) was defined as the number of spikes generated with interspike intervals ≤50 ms as measured from autocorrelation functions. This is equivalent to computing interspike interval histograms. mPFC synchrony (3) was defined as the number of spikes in the ±50 ms range of cross-correlation functions. Auto- and cross-correlograms were first normalized for variations in firing rates. To this end, spikes were shuffled 50 times and the auto- or cross-correlation recalculated each time. These randomized correlograms were then averaged and subtracted from the original correlograms. The excess in activity (nonshuffled minus shuffled average) in the ±50 ms center bins was then used below.
Next, we constructed a multiple logistic regression model using these three factors as independent variables and the significance of the STJHs as the dependent variable. It is important that such analyses not only include short post-CS or postreward periods, because the activity of mPFC cells might show large variations that are associated with the CS and/or rewards and therefore dominate the correlations in the model, although being unrelated to changes in the proportion of STJHs. Thus, to limit the impact of such coincidental relations, we calculated all of the required parameters during long periods of activity that surrounded and included the trace-conditioning trials: from 10 s before the CS to 15 s after the CS, analyzed in nonoverlapping 250 ms time windows. It should be pointed out that the results of the multiple regression held when the post-CS and postreward data were omitted from the analysis or when the data obtained from the early versus late stages of learning were considered separately.
For the population model, we collapsed data across sessions and cats, and calculated the proportion of significant STJHs in each time window, together with the average of each of the three independent parameters. After verifying that the complete model was significant (with a likelihood ratio test, p < 0.05), we used a backward stepwise procedure that drops one variable at a time and checks if the difference between the complete model and the reduced one is significant (likelihood ratio test).
To take into account the variability between different cats, sessions, and cells or cell couples, we next calculated individual models that related the activity of individual mPFC cells or cell couples to the STJH(s) they contributed to. In this case, the dependent variable was binary (significant STJH or not), and the independent variables (1–3) were continuous. Logistic regressions were fitted using the Matlab glmfit function (Mathworks, Natick, MA).
Results
Using two arrays of high-impedance microelectrodes (Fig. 1 A, dots), the activity of mPFC (n = 125) (Fig. 1 B1,C), ER (n = 145) (Fig. 1 B2,C), and PR (n = 224) (Fig. 1 B2,C) neurons was simultaneously recorded in behaving cats.
A method to examine correlated rhinal activity time locked to mPFC firing
Typically, coactivity patterns in two brain areas, in this case the PR and ER cortices, are assessed by computing CCs. With this method, the relation with a third factor, in this case the activity of mPFC neurons, is hidden in the result of the CCs and cannot be assessed. To circumvent this problem, we used an analysis technique that measures correlated activity in the rhinal cortices, but in relation to mPFC firing. Specifically, we adapted the JPSTH method (Aertsen et al., 1989); but instead of using an external stimulus as the triggering event as in the JPSTH, we used the spikes generated by mPFC neurons. Hereafter, this analysis will be termed spike-triggered joint histogram (STJH) (Paz et al., 2006). We first explain how this analysis is performed and interpreted and then describe the results it yielded.
STJHs are computed by taking segments (±150 ms) of rhinal activity around mPFC spikes. As shown in Figure 2 A, the abscissa and ordinate of the STJHs respectively represent when PR and ER spikes occurred (red ticks) in relation to mPFC spikes (at time 0 on both axes). Then, STJH bins that contain a coincidence of spikes (Fig. 2 A, squares) are incremented. In other words, if around a given mPFC spike, there was a spike from the PR cell at time x, and one from the ER cell at time y, one count is added to the matrix bin (x,y). Repeating this process for all mPFC spikes gradually produces the raw STJH. One example of such an STJH is presented in Figure 2 B1, in which the counts are color coded. The STJHs can reveal correlations that are often invisible in a raw CC because standard CCs include all of the spikes that rhinal cells generate, whereas STJHs only consider those that occur in relation to mPFC action potentials.
Because we are interested in mPFC-related rhinal correlations, we tested the raw STJHs against two null hypotheses: (1) that the observed correlation is similar to that expected independently of mPFC activity; and (2) that the correlation reflects independent responses of rhinal neurons to mPFC activity. Two control randomized matrices were calculated for the two hypotheses (see Materials and Methods) (Fig. 2 B2,B3) and we performed bin-to-bin comparisons of significance between the raw and the two randomized sets of STJHs, using a Poisson distribution with a threshold p value corrected for multiple comparisons (Fig. 2 B4). Finally, we tested whether most significant bins clustered around one peak (76% of STJHs, DIP statistical test). All analyses were limited to these unimodal STJHs.
The location of significant bin clusters in the STJHs contains information about the timing of correlated rhinal activity in relation to mPFC spikes (Fig. 2 C). A concentration of significant bins in quadrant 1 (Fig. 2 C) represents correlated rhinal activity that occurred after mPFC spikes whereas a concentration of bins in quadrant 3 (Fig. 2 C) implies that mPFC firing typically occurred after correlated rhinal spikes. For other possible timing sequences, see Figure 2 C. To capture the particular coding scheme revealed by the STJHs, we summed the bins in each quadrant separately, and designated the largest as the timing index. Below, when comparing mPFC-related rhinal interactions in different conditions, we compared the proportion of cell triplets with STJHs falling in the four quadrants. Because the sum of the proportions in the four quadrants is 100%, a decrease in one quadrant is necessarily accompanied by an overall increase in the others.
The location of significant bins in the STJHs also indicates the prevalent direction of impulse traffic in the rhinal cortices in relation to mPFC spikes. Bins above the main diagonal of the STJH represent correlated activity where ER firing follows PR spikes or, in other words, lateral-to-medial impulse transfer (Fig. 2 D). Conversely, bins below the main diagonal indicate ER to PR transfer (Fig. 2 D). Because the directionality of information transfer in the rhinal cortices has important implications for the mechanisms of learning and memory, we computed a directionality index (DI) that captures this dimension. To this end, we separately added all bins above the main diagonal (a) versus below the main diagonal (b) in the STJHs (Fig. 2 D). The DI was obtained by computing (a − b/a + b) and thus ranges from −1 to 1, where a positive value indicates that the PR neuron tended to fire before the ER cell and inversely for negative values.
mPFC-related modulation of rhinal interactions
We first consider mPFC-related rhinal interactions taking place during the quiet waking state, when no rewards were delivered. Previous studies that cross-correlated the activity of PR and ER neurons during the waking state reported a low incidence of significant CCs (Pelletier et al., 2004). This was also the case here, where only 14% (23 of 164) of PR-ER cell couples had significant CCs (p < 0.05, shuffling procedure). However, when we used STJHs to restrict the analysis to rhinal spikes that occurred around mPFC activity, we observed significant rhinal correlations that were not apparent in the CCs. This point is illustrated in Figure 3, which shows cross-correlograms for four couples of PR and ER neurons (Fig. 3 A), the corresponding STJHs centered on mPFC spikes (Fig. 3 B), and control STJHs produced by shuffling of the mPFC spike trains (Fig. 3 C). As shown in Figure 3 A, a majority of cross-correlograms were flat (Fig. 3 A1–A3), indicating that there is little correlated activity between rhinal neurons when mPFC activity is not taken into account. In contrast, all depicted STJHs (Fig. 3 B) show that in relation to mPFC spikes, there is a high degree of correlated PR–ER activity. Moreover, even a high raw rhinal cross-correlogram (Fig. 3 A4) can actually be related to mPFC spikes (Fig. 3 B4), because the increased correlation is evident immediately after mPFC firing in the STJH.
Using the criteria described above on all cell triplets (n = 164), 35% (or 58 of 164) of STJHs were deemed statistically significant, much more than expected by chance (p < 0.0001, Fisher's exact test). When this ratio was computed separately for couples of rhinal cells where CCs were significant or not, a similar percentage of significant STJHs was observed (42 and 34%, respectively). Thus, the STJH analysis indicates that hidden in the CCs, are periods of enhanced rhinal interactions that prevalently occur when mPFC cells are active. To gain some insight in the proportion of rhinal spikes involved in this effect, we compared the number of PR and ER spikes occurring within ±150 ms of mPFC spikes versus random times. We found that PR and ER cells were slightly more active around mPFC spikes compared with random times (an increase of 2.6 and 4.3%, respectively). Expressed in firing rates, PR and ER cells fired at 1.32 and 2.36 Hz, respectively, around random times compared with 1.35 and 2.46 Hz around mPFC spikes. These small elevations suggest that the critical factor behind the effect disclosed by the STJHs is a higher incidence of temporally contiguous PR and ER spikes, not a global increase in rhinal activity levels.
Figure 4 A illustrates the grand average of significant STJHs during the waking state. It shows that correlated PR–ER firing prevalently occurred after mPFC spikes. To examine this further, we studied the distribution of the timing index (Fig. 2 C) where significant bins in each quadrant of the STJHs are summed separately, and the quadrant with the highest total is designated as the timing index. This revealed that peaks of correlated rhinal activity were not distributed homogeneously in relation to the mPFC spikes (Fig. 4 B) (p < 0.01, χ 2 ). Rather, many STJHs had a preponderance of significant bins in the first quadrant (33% higher than expected; p < 0.05, χ 2 ), where mPFC spikes generally occurred before PR and ER firing. In contrast, 22% of significant STJHs fell in the third quadrant, where mPFC spikes usually occurred after correlated rhinal activity.
To determine whether there was a preferential directionality of rhinal interactions in relation to mPFC activity, we computed the DI for all significant STJHs (Fig. 4 C). Both positive and negative DIs were extensively represented in the population, with an average slightly, but significantly below zero (population mean, −0.01; t test, p < 0.05). Thus, in quiet waking, both directions of PR–ER interactions occur in relation to mPFC activity, but with a slight preference for ER to PR communication.
Learning a trace-conditioning task
To study how mPFC-rhinal interactions are affected during memory formation, three cats were trained on an appetitive trace-conditioning task (Fig. 5 A). In this task, a visual CS that predicted the delivery of a liquid reward 3 s later was presented at random intervals (30–90 s) (Fig. 5 A, methods). The CS (1.5 s) was a global change in the illumination of a computer screen that encompassed most of the visual field of the animals. The reward was a highly salient event for the animals because it was preferred food and the subjects were fed only during the recording sessions. The animals underwent one training session per day (38–95 trials/session) over a period of 9 d.
In addition to the data reviewed in the Introduction, much evidence indicates that this task is dependent on the hippocampus and mPFC. First, in eye blink conditioning, introduction of a delay between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli renders this task hippocampal-dependent (Shors, 2004). Second, in several appetitive operant learning tasks involving a delay between the conditioned stimuli and responses, hippocampal lesions interfere with learning (Savage et al., 2004). Third, in an appetitive trace-conditioning paradigm very similar to the one used here, disruption of hippocampal activity with intraseptal infusions of scopolamine interfered with learning (Asaka et al., 2000). Last, hippocampal and mPFC neurons exhibit learning-dependent changes in activity profile in relation to the conditioned stimulus and during the delay period (Berger et al., 1983; Solomon et al., 1986; Weiss et al., 1996; Munera et al., 2001; Baeg et al., 2001; McEchron et al., 2003; Gilmartin and McEchron, 2005).
Two behavioral measures were used to assess learning on our task: the mean response time, defined as the mean latency of the first 25 licks after reward delivery (Fig. 5 B), and the rate of anticipatory licking after CS onset but before reward delivery (Fig. 5 C). Both measures showed the same trend for all cats considered individually or as a group: a rapid initial learning phase that could be observed as early as training days 2–3, followed by a protracted period of slower improvements that extended up to training days 5–9 (ANOVA of trials over sessions, p < 0.05 for all cats and both measures). In keeping with this, the two learning curves were well fitted with an exponential function (Fig. 5 B–C, dashed lines) (p < 0.01 for both, bootstrap).
Overall, these behavioral measures suggest that although memory for the predictive value of the CS is formed as early as the second training session, it is then gradually strengthened over a period of several days.
Learning-related changes of mPFC-rhinal interactions
Although our sample of mPFC neurons included infralimbic (IL; n = 76) and prelimbic (PL; n = 49) cells, the results obtained with these two groups of cells were pooled because (1) IL and PL have overlapping projections to the rhinal cortices (Vertes, 2004), and (2) the activity profile of IL and PL neurons during the CS, delay, and reward were statistically indistinguishable (supplemental Table 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).
To determine whether mPFC-rhinal interactions fluctuated as a function of learning, we computed STJHs during the trace-conditioning trials, in nonoverlapping 0.5 s windows for all triplets of simultaneously recorded mPFC, PR, and ER neurons (n = 164). Immediately after CS presentation, the ratio of significant STJHs increased markedly (Fig. 6 A) (from 21 to 41%; p < 0.001, χ 2 ), indicating an increase in the strength of mPFC-related rhinal correlations. Reward delivery was also associated with an increase in the proportion of significant STJHs (Fig. 6 B) (from 19 to 42%; p < 0.001, χ 2 ). Additionally, the distribution of the timing index was altered, with a decrease in the third quadrant (from 30 to 13% after CS and from 25 to 11% after reward; p < 0.05 for both, χ 2 ), suggesting that rhinal correlations follow mPFC activity in a higher proportion of STJHs.
To study the time course of these changes and their relation to behavior, we examined how the proportion of significant STJHs fluctuated across training sessions, pooling the data obtained during the first 1.5 s after CS onset. This revealed a gradual increase in the proportion of significant STJHs as a function of training sessions (Fig. 6 C) (exponential fit, p < 0.01; χ 2 , p < 0.01). This was in contrast to reward-related activity where no temporal trend was observed (Fig. 6 D) (p > 0.1).
To further establish that the findings were specific to perirhinal–entorhinal interactions, we computed STJHs on rhinal neurons located in the same area, but located at different rostrocaudal levels. Thus, in this case, the STJHs were still conditioned on mPFC spikes, but instead of analyzing the activity of perirhinal and entorhinal neurons, we examined the activity of two perirhinal or two entorhinal cells. We found that although the baseline proportions of significant STJHs were similar (19% for the CS, 20% for the reward), the CS- and reward-related increases in the proportion of significant STJHs were much lower (increase of 6 and 7% for the CS and reward, respectively) compared with ∼20% for triplets including mPFC, perirhinal, and entorhinal cells (p < 0.001, Fisher's exact test).
Overall, these results indicate that reward delivery causes a consistent increase in mPFC-related rhinal correlations at all phases of learning, whereas CS-related increases in rhinal correlations develop gradually, as learning progresses and the CS acquires predictive value. In support of this, the exponential fits for the progression of behavior (Fig. 5 B,C) and the ratio of significant STJHs during the CS (Fig. 6 C) had similar time constants (p > 0.1, t test).
Learning-related changes in the directionality of mPFC-related rhinal interactions
Next, to test whether learning-related changes in the proportion of significant STJHs were associated with a shift in the directionality of rhinal interactions, we examined how the DI fluctuated in relation to the CS and reward in 0.5 s windows around the events of interest (Fig. 6 E–H). To do so, we quantified the proportion of STJHs with positive DIs. A proportion >50% indicates that the prevalent direction of impulse traffic in the rhinal cortices is from PR to ER neurons, whereas a proportion <50% signifies the opposite. After the CS, a significant reduction from the 50% baseline was observed (Fig. 6 E) (p < 0.01, χ 2 ), indicating a preferential ER to PR direction of impulse transfer around mPFC activity. In contrast, no change in DI was seen in relation to the reward (Fig. 6 F) (p > 0.1, χ 2 ).
To study the time course of DI fluctuations as a function of the learning phase, we pooled the data obtained during the first 1.5 s after CS onset (Fig. 6 G) or reward delivery (Fig. 6 H). This analysis revealed a similar trend as for the STJH ratio analysis (Fig. 6 C): as learning progressed, there was a gradual shift from no directional propensity to a preference for mediolateral interactions (Fig. 6 G) (exponential fit, p < 0.01; χ 2 , p < 0.01). This was in contrast to reward related activity where the DI did not fluctuate with the learning phase (Fig. 6 H) (p > 0.01).
To summarize, whereas there was no preferential directionality of PR–ER interactions in relation to rewards at all phases of learning, in relation to the CS, ER to PR impulse transfer gradually became prevalent as learning progressed.
What aspects of mPFC activity promote rhinal correlations?
To address this question, we asked what parameters of mPFC activity correlate with the emergence of significant STJHs. First, higher mPFC firing rates could facilitate rhinal correlations (although STJH were normalized for firing rates). Second, increased clustering of the spikes generated by individual mPFC cells could participate by allowing for enhanced temporal summation of inputs into the rhinal cortices. To assess this possibility, we considered the proportion of interspike intervals ≤50 ms generated by mPFC cells (n = 124). Finally, if the discharges of different mPFC neurons were more synchronized, this could facilitate rhinal interactions via summation of inputs. Thus, to take this factor into account, we cross-correlated the activity of simultaneously recorded mPFC neurons (181 cell couples) and summed the activity in the center bins (±50 ms). CCs were normalized for firing rate variations using a shuffling procedure (see Materials and Methods).
To investigate the contribution of these three different parameters to the occurrence of mPFC-facilitated rhinal transmission, we constructed a regression model that included synchrony, spike clustering, and firing rate as independent parameters (Fig. 7 A–C, x-axes, respectively), and the ratio of significant STJHs as the dependent variable (Fig. 7 A–C, y-axis). These parameters were calculated in nonoverlapping 0.25 s windows (from 10 s before to 15 s after the CS; similar results were obtained when these analyses were performed only on pre-CS, post-CS, or postreward periods).
At the population level, the multiple regression model (including all three independent parameters) was significant (p < 0.01, likelihood ratio test). A backward stepwise procedure that dropped one variable at a time revealed that both increased synchrony among simultaneously recorded mPFC cells (p < 0.01, likelihood ratio test) (Fig. 7 A), as well as higher clustering of the spikes generated by individual mPFC neurons (p < 0.05) (Fig. 7 B), contributed significantly. In contrast, this procedure revealed that the firing rate of mPFC neurons was not a significant contributor (p = 0.2) (Fig. 7 C).
Last, to consider the variability between cells or cell couples, we performed separate regressions for each individual STJH (n = 124). A representative example is shown in Figure 7 D–F. The distribution of p values for the three factors for these individual models revealed that firing rate, mPFC spike clustering, and mPFC synchrony had significant effects (p < 0.05) in 12, 18, and 27% of cases, respectively. The difference between these percentages was significant (p < 0.01, χ 2 ).
Overall, these analyses suggest that several interacting aspects of mPFC activity contribute to enhance rhinal correlation, particularly increases in the synchrony and, to a lesser extent, in the clustering of mPFC action potentials.
Discussion
The present study was undertaken to examine the relative timing of mPFC and rhinal activity during the acquisition of a hippocampal-dependent trace-conditioning task. The importance of this issue stems from findings implicating the mPFC in the consolidation of hippocampal-dependent memories and evidence suggesting that an mPFC-driven modulation of neocortical-hippocampal interactions at the level of the rhinal cortices might underlie this effect. Our results indicate that in relation to mPFC activity, rhinal interactions are facilitated. At early stages of learning, this effect was most pronounced in relation to reward whereas at late learning stages, it also occurred in relation to the CS. Moreover at late, but not early stages of learning, CS-related mPFC activity was prevalently associated with enhanced transmission from ER to PR neurons. Because the ER to PR connection is an obligatory step in the multisynaptic pathway that links the hippocampus to the neocortex, these results suggest that mPFC activity facilitates rhinal transfer of hippocampal impulses toward the neocortex. The significance of these findings for the consolidation of hippocampal-dependent memories is considered below.
Learning-related changes in mPFC-rhinal interactions
The observation that hippocampal lesions produce a temporally graded amnesia for recently learned events (Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1986; Kim and Fanselow, 1992) has led to the view that the hippocampal network forms rapid representations of events to be remembered. Subsequently, through repeated interactions between the hippocampus and neocortex, the links between distributed neocortical representations would become strong enough to support long-term storage of the new information (Squire and Alvarez, 1995; Buzsaki, 1996; Sutherland and McNaughton, 2000). It was proposed that this gradual transfer of new information to the neocortex is required for their integration in existing memory networks as a more rapid transfer would corrupt the old memories (McClelland et al., 1995).
What are the anatomical substrates of hippocampal-neocortical interactions? Most hippocampal projections to the neocortex involve a stepwise progression of impulses through the ER cortex and PR areas 35–36 before reaching the neocortex (Burwell and Witter, 2002). However, because impulse transfer in this multisynaptic pathway is subjected to strong inhibitory pressures (Biella et al., 2001; Martina et al., 2001; Pinto et al., 2006), rhinal transfer of hippocampal and neocortical information occurs with a low probability (Biella et al., 2002; Pelletier et al., 2004, 2005; de Curtis and Pare, 2004).
A notable exception to this indirect scheme of hippocampo–neocortical connectivity is the mPFC, which receive monosynaptic inputs from CA1 pyramidal cells (Swanson, 1981; Ferino et al., 1987; Jay and Witter, 1991; Thierry et al., 2000). Importantly, most hippocampal neurons that project to the mPFC have axon collaterals ending in the ER cortex (Swanson, 1981). In turn, the mPFC is in a key position to influence rhinal transfer of hippocampal inputs to the neocortex because it sends strong projections to the PR and ER cortices (Room et al., 1985; Sesack et al., 1989; Hurley et al., 1991), most of which involve excitatory synapses onto principal rhinal neurons (Apergis-Schoute et al., 2006).
Our results support and extend these observations. Indeed, we found that at late, but not early stages of learning, CS-evoked mPFC activity is associated with facilitated interactions between the ER and PR cortices. Importantly, this facilitation occurred in a specific direction: from ER to PR neurons, as would be expected for an mPFC-facilitated rhinal transfer of hippocampal information toward the neocortex. Note that this is consistent with the fact that mPFC afferents to the ER cortex focus on deep layers, where laterally directed projections originate (Burwell and Witter, 2002). The fact that there was no preferential directionality in relation to rewards suggests that the direction of facilitated rhinal interactions is not entirely determined by mPFC inputs, but also by the pattern of afferent activity that prevails in the rhinal cortices when mPFC neurons are engaged.
What mechanism could explain the gradual appearance of the CS-related mPFC facilitation of rhinal interactions? It is possible that during repeated training sessions, direct hippocampal projections to the mPFC gradually induce a potentiation of afferents conveying CS information to mPFC neurons (Sierra-Mercado et al., 2006). As a result, subsequent presentations of the CS would directly activate mPFC neurons, which in turn would facilitate rhinal transfer of hippocampal inputs to the neocortex. This might occur both when the CS is presented and during slow-wave sleep, when hippocampal memories are thought to be “replayed” (Buzsaki, 1989; Sutherland and McNaughton, 2000).
Although the electrodes were lowered 100 μm each day in the present study, it is unlikely that the activity modifications we observed were attributable to the fact that we sampled neurons from different layers. Indeed, depending on the cues, mPFC-related rhinal activity varied or was stable across training sessions. For instance, whereas the CS-related increase in the proportion of significant STJHs and the associated shift in directionality developed gradually, no temporal trend was seen in relation to the reward for both parameters. Second, the results we obtained were very different from the ones seen when the STJHs were conditioned on basolateral amygdala (BLA) spikes (Paz et al., 2006). Finally, in the latter study, we recorded during a behavioral paradigm that consisted of unexpected rewards during the whole recording period. Although the electrodes were lowered just as in the current study, the increase in the proportion of significant STJHs seen in relation to these unexpected rewards did not change, indicating that (at least for BLA-based STJHs) the findings cannot be attributed to the fact that we recorded neurons in different layers.
Comparison between the impact of mPFC and amygdala activity on rhinal interactions
The relationship between the activity of mPFC and rhinal neurons evidenced here contrasts with that observed previously between amygdala and rhinal cells (Paz et al., 2006). Indeed, it was found that BLA activity facilitates rhinal interactions in relation to reward delivery, but in a direction opposite to that seen with mPFC firing: from PR to ER cells. Also different from the present results was the fact that this effect was most pronounced in the early stages of learning, when the reward was still unexpected. At this late phase of learning, after the association between CS and rewards was acquired, the BLA-related facilitation of rhinal interactions occurred when the cats were anticipating the reward, but at a much longer post-CS latency than seen in relation to mPFC activity. Moreover, consistent with the idea that the BLA and mPFC play distinct roles in memory formation, the CS-related increase in the proportion of significant STJHs developed earlier for BLA than mPFC spikes. Indeed, the BLA effect reached a plateau at the third training session (Paz et al., 2006) compared with the sixth training session for the mPFC (present results).
The contrasting time dependence of mPFC and amygdala-related effects in the rhinal cortices parallels the results of lesion and pharmacobehavioral studies (McGaugh, 2004). Indeed, both approaches suggest that amygdala activity mainly facilitates memory during and shortly after encoding whereas the mPFC contributes at longer delays. These observations are consistent with the opposite directionality of the mPFC and amygdala effects in the rhinal cortices. The amygdala-related facilitation of PR to ER communication (rhinal transfer of neocortical inputs to the hippocampus) is consistent with a facilitation of memory encoding whereas the mPFC-related facilitation of ER to PR interactions (rhinal transfer of hippocampal inputs to the neocortex) suggest that it enhances memory consolidation.
Finally, there is a confounding factor when interpreting our study: the behavioral paradigm we used does not dissociate between consolidation and task performance at late stages of acquisition. Because the mPFC and rhinal cortices are known to be involved in the latter process as well (see Introduction), our results could indicate that the mPFC, by facilitating transfer of information within the rhinal cortices, supports task performance (rather than memory consolidation) at these late stages of training. More work will be needed to address this distinction.
Conclusions
Combined with the lesion and functional imaging studies reviewed in the Introduction, our results support the idea that the mPFC, by facilitating rhinal transfer of hippocampal inputs to the neocortex, is an important player in the slow iterative consolidation process supporting the integration of hippocampal-dependent memories into neocortical networks. Coupled to the results obtained in the BLA (Paz et al., 2006), our observations reinforce the view that memory formation engages a distributed network of structures, each contributing in different ways to the formation and consolidation of memories.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant 0208712 and by National Institute of Mental Health Grant RO1 MH073610.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Denis Paré, Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, 197 University Avenue, Newark, NJ 07102. pare{at}axon.rutgers.edu