Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition

Kestutis Kveraga, Jasmine Boshyan and Moshe Bar
Journal of Neuroscience 28 November 2007, 27 (48) 13232-13240; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3481-07.2007
Kestutis Kveraga
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jasmine Boshyan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Moshe Bar
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Object recognition is traditionally viewed as a hierarchical, bottom-up neural process. This view has been challenged recently by theoretical models and by findings indicating that top-down processes are involved in facilitating recognition. However, how such high-level information can be activated quickly enough to facilitate the bottom-up processing is yet unknown. We propose that such top-down facilitation is triggered by magnocellular information projected early and rapidly to the orbitofrontal cortex. Using human neuroimaging, we show that stimuli designed to bias processing toward the magnocellular pathway differentially activated the orbitofrontal cortex compared with parvocellular-biased stimuli. Although the magnocellular stimuli had a lower contrast than the parvocellular stimuli, they were recognized faster and just as accurately. Moreover, orbitofrontal activity predicted the performance advantage for the magnocellular, but not for the parvocellular-biased, stimuli, whereas the opposite was true in the fusiform gyrus. Last, analyses of effective connectivity using dynamic causal modeling showed that magnocellular-biased stimuli significantly activated pathways from occipital visual cortex to orbitofrontal cortex and from orbitofrontal cortex to fusiform gyrus. Conversely, parvocellular-biased stimuli significantly activated a pathway from the occipital visual cortex to fusiform gyrus. Our findings support the proposal that fast magnocellular projections linking early visual and inferotemporal object recognition regions with the orbitofrontal cortex facilitate object recognition by enabling the generation of early predictions.

  • top-down facilitation
  • fMRI
  • inferotemporal cortex
  • object recognition
  • prefrontal cortex
  • effective connectivity
  • dynamic causal models
  • vision

Introduction

Our visual system enables us to recognize objects quickly in varying viewing conditions. Object recognition has been commonly viewed as a feedforward, bottom-up process in which visual input is analyzed by a hierarchy of visual regions situated along the ventral visual stream. However, several models of object recognition have proposed that feedback, top-down information is important in facilitating object recognition (Grossberg, 1980; Kosslyn, 1994; Ullman, 1995), and others have noted that the efficiency of visual recognition cannot be fully explained by bottom-up processes alone (Mumford, 1994; Lamme and Roelfsema, 2000; Bullier, 2001). To facilitate the bottom-up processing, top-down information must be activated and available to the low-level areas before recognition is completed. A recent model specified a mechanism for such fast triggering of top-down facilitation (Bar, 2003). According to this model, a coarse version of the visual input, comprising mainly the low spatial frequencies (LSFs), is rapidly projected from early visual regions to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). The LSF image is sufficient to activate “initial guess” predictions about what objects might have given rise to such visual input. These predictions activate the corresponding visual representations in object processing regions in the ventral temporal cortex, which is hypothesized to facilitate recognition by biasing the bottom-up processes to concentrate on a small set of the most likely object representations (Fig. 1). This model has received strong support from a recent study integrating functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) (Bar et al., 2006), which found that OFC is differentially activated by images containing LSF information and that this activation occurs early (∼130 ms) in the recognition process, critically preceding activity in inferotemporal cortex (IT).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

An illustration of the top-down facilitation model. A partially processed, LSF image of the visual input is rapidly projected to OFC from early visual regions, although detailed slower analysis of the visual input is being performed along the ventral visual stream. This “gist” image activates predictions about candidate objects similar to the image in their LSF appearance, which are fed back to the ventral object recognition regions to facilitate bottom-up processing.

We propose that such rapid activation of OFC is triggered by a magnocellular (M) projection to OFC, which generates “initial guesses” based on M information. The M pathway is known to convey low-resolution, achromatic information rapidly (Maunsell et al., 1990; Shapley, 1990; Merigan and Maunsell, 1993; Bullier and Nowak, 1995; Chen et al., 2006). Conversely, parvocellular (P) and the much fewer koniocellular (K) neurons conduct information more slowly and can resolve fine details and chromatic contrast but require substantially higher luminance contrast (>8%) (Tootell et al., 1988) to detect achromatic stimuli (Hicks et al., 1983; Lee et al., 1990). The question of what type of information and which anatomical pathways trigger top-down facilitation is critical for understanding visual recognition, and it has never been tested explicitly. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine directly whether OFC could generate predictions from rapidly available M information by probing it with stimuli designed to bias visual processing toward either the M or P pathway. Because this M projection would convey predominantly M-biased visual information, we hypothesized that M-biased stimuli would activate OFC to a significantly greater extent than would P-biased stimuli. Furthermore, observers should be able to use top-down information earlier with an M projection to OFC and a subsequent feedback of predictions from OFC to IT, and therefore show improved recognition performance for M-biased stimuli.

Materials and Methods

Stimuli.

We used line drawings of objects that were either of low-luminance contrast and achromatic (M-biased), or chromatically defined and isoluminant (red-green; P-biased), to examine directly which of the two main pathways is used to trigger top-down facilitation in object recognition (Fig. 2). The third, koniocellular, pathway inputs are very few compared with the M and P projections, which constitute the vast majority of inputs into V1 (Callaway, 2005) and would not be sensitive to the red-green or achromatic, low-luminance-contrast stimuli in any case because of their response properties (Casagrande, 1994; Hendry and Reid, 2000). M- and P-biased stimuli have been shown in the past to be effective in studying visual search and attention (Steinman et al., 1997; Cheng et al., 2004) and M- and P-biased letter and mask stimuli have also been used in studying visual perception abnormalities in schizophrenia (Schechter et al., 2003; Butler et al., 2006). We chose to use line drawings rather than pictures of objects, because the uniform foreground of line drawings allows precise control over the luminance and chromatic properties of the stimuli that was necessary for this experiment. We used 160 line drawings of everyday objects as stimuli in this study; 40 line drawings were used solely to determine the stimulus characteristics essential to the experimental manipulation in separate pretesting stages and were not seen in the experiment proper. These characteristics, namely the foreground–background luminance contrast for the achromatic, M-biased, stimuli, and the isoluminance values for the chromatic stimuli, differ somewhat between individuals. Therefore, they were established for each participant immediately before commencing functional scanning, with the participant positioned in the scanner and with the exact viewing conditions subsequently used in the experiment proper. The following procedures used to establish the isoluminance point and the appropriate luminance contrast are standard techniques and have been successfully used in many studies exploring the M and P pathway contributions to attention, visual search, schizophrenia, and dyslexia (Steinman et al., 1997; Schechter et al., 2003; Cheng et al., 2004; Butler et al., 2006). The appropriate luminance contrast was determined by first finding the luminance threshold via a multiple staircase procedure, during which the subjects were required to report whether they could recognize the stimulus. One-fourth of the trials were catch trials in which the stimulus did not appear. Our luminance threshold-finding algorithm computed the mean of the turnaround points above and below medium-gray background and reliably converged around the true background value. From this threshold, the appropriate luminance (∼3.5% Weber contrast) value was computed for the grayscale line drawings to be used in the low-luminance-contrast (M-biased) condition.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Stimuli and procedure. We used an event-related design with 33% interspersed fixation trials. M-biased, P-biased, and fixation stimuli were presented in sequences optimized for hemodynamic response estimation efficiency. The stimuli in the figure are slightly brightened to enhance visibility in print. In the experiment, the stimulus pictures appeared with a light-gray screen background to prevent adaptation to darkness.

For the chromatically defined stimuli in the isoluminant (P-biased) condition, the isoluminance point was found using heterochromatic flicker photometry with the line drawings of objects displayed in alternating colors, specifically, pure red and pure green. Briefly, this procedure allows finding an isoluminance point for two colors by having a stimulus rapidly alternate between two colors. When the alternation frequency is in the range of 12–20 Hz, the flicker caused by luminance differences between these two colors seems to disappear for a very narrow range of luminance values, and the two colors fuse. In pilot testing, we found that the alternation frequency of ∼14 Hz gave the best isoluminance point estimates (narrow range within-subjects and low variability between-subjects). The color values at which the stimulus appears steady is the (very narrow) isoluminance interval. Subjects were required to report via a key press whether the stimulus was flickering or appeared steady. Depending on the response, the output of the red gun was adjusted up or down in a pseudorandom manner so that many passes over the isoluminance point occurred during the procedure. The average of the values in the narrow range when a subject reported a steady stimulus became the isoluminance value for the subject used in the experiment. Thus, isoluminant stimuli were defined only by chromatic contrast between foreground and background, which appeared equally bright to the observer. It is important to note that the foreground brightness value for the P stimuli was substantially higher than for the M stimuli. The participants were queried after the experiments about the visibility of the stimuli. Eleven of the 12 participants reported that the P stimuli appeared more visible to them.

Subjects.

Twelve paid volunteers (20–32 years of age; eight females), recruited from the greater Boston academic community, participated in the experiment. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, including normal color vision and no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, and were right-handed. Eleven of the subjects were native English speakers, and the non-native speaker was fluent in English; all had had at least 15 years of formal education. One subject was excluded from the analysis during filtering for statistical outliers. None of the subjects knew the purpose of the study. The informed consent of each participant was obtained before the experiment according to the procedures approved by Massachusetts General Hospital (Human Studies Protocol 2001P-001754).

Procedure.

In the experiment, participants were required to make an object size decision that required recognition of the stimulus, reporting whether an object they were shown was bigger or smaller than a shoe box. A third option (“Not sure”) was provided, but almost never used, likely because only stimuli with an unambiguous size relationship to a prototypical shoe box were used in the experiment. Note that this decision was orthogonal to the manipulation of interest (M-biased vs P-biased stimuli). The proportion of object size decisions (“bigger”/“smaller”) was equalized within-subjects and within-condition, and the key-press assignment was counterbalanced across subjects. Subjects were given instructions about all parts of the experiment verbally before entering the scanner, and then the same instructions were presented on the screen before each part of the experiment. Stimuli of both types were presented in a centered 256 × 256 pixel square for 1500 ms, with 480 stimulus trials collected in each condition. We used a rapid event-related design and therefore added 33% (240) null trials across four experimental runs. The event sequences were pseudorandom and optimized for hemodynamic response estimation efficiency by the rapid event-related design optimization program Optseq2 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq). Stimulus presentation and behavioral data acquisition were controlled by a custom program written in MatLab (MathWorks, Natick, MA) using Psychtoolbox (Brainard, 1997) and running on an Apple G4 laptop computer with Mac OS 9 (Apple Computers, Cupertino, CA). The stimulus images were projected by a Sharp Notevision LCD projector through the head end of the scanner bore and a translucent rear-projection screen and were synchronized with functional volume acquisitions via timing pulses. Subjects viewed stimuli in a mirror integrated into the Siemens head coil. The head and viewing position were stabilized with adjustable padded head restraints and cushions. Responses were collected with a fiber-optic button box using the index, middle, and ring fingers of the right hand. The time elapsed from stimulus onset was computed on-line for each trial and stored as reaction time along with the key-press code.

Data acquisition and analysis.

All MRI scans were acquired on a 1.5T Siemens Avanto scanner with a 12-channel “TIM” system. We first obtained high-resolution, T1-weighted structural images for the reconstruction of each subject's cortical surface. The functional scans were acquired using gradient-echo EPI with a repetition time (TR) of 2.5 s, echo time (TE) of 25 ms, flip angle of 90°, and 33 interleaved slices (3.125 × 3 × 3 mm resolution with 1 mm skip), tilted anterior ∼30° up from the AC-PC line to improve signal in the orbitofrontal cortex and minimize susceptibility artifacts in OFC (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). We acquired 720 functional volumes per subject in four functional runs, each lasting 7.5 min. We used the Massachusetts General Hospital FS-FAST and Freesurfer Analysis software to analyze fMRI data (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999, 2001; Segonne et al., 2004, 2005). The functional images were first realigned to the first volume in the series using the analysis of functional neuroimages motion-correction algorithm (Cox, 1996) and then smoothed with an isotropic 8 mm full width half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. The surface-based analysis techniques have been used successfully in the past and are reported in detail previously (Bar et al., 2006).

Results

Behavioral results

We first screened the data for outliers (>2 SD ± the mean) and incorrect responses from the reaction time (RT) data. This screening resulted in the exclusion of one of the 12 subjects as a result of being a statistical outlier. Correct response RT results revealed a significant recognition speed advantage for M-biased stimuli (mean p = 1010 ms; mean M = 905 ms; SEM ± 27.6; 95% CI 43–166; two-tailed paired samples t test; t11 = 3.8; p < 0.003) (Fig. 3A). The faster RT for M-biased stimuli did not arise because of a speed-accuracy trade-off, because the response accuracy was similar in the M and P conditions (meanP 68% vs meanM 73%; p < 0.06). Including incorrect trials did not qualitatively alter the RT relationship between the M and P conditions, resulting only in 10–20 ms shifts in mean RT for both conditions but preserving the M-P difference of ∼100 ms. However, our subjects apparently were unaware of any recognition advantage for M, with all but one reporting in the postexperiment debriefing that the chromatic (P) stimuli were easier to see than the grayscale (M) stimuli. This speed advantage for M-biased stimuli supports our proposal that magnocellular processing plays a critical role in facilitating object recognition, perhaps without subject awareness.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Comparison of M and P conditions. A, The reaction time results for M (gray; 905 ms) and P (red; 1010 ms). Only the correct responses, without outliers (more than ±2 SD), are included, although the M-P relationship does not change qualitatively when all trials are included (see Results, Behavioral results). B, Statistical parametric map of the activations in the M (Magno) versus P (Parvo) contrast. The average fMRI activity of all subjects is shown on the ventral surface of the “inflated” brain. The sulci are in dark gray, and the gyri are in light gray. The M > P activations are displayed in red-yellow; P > M activity is displayed in blue-cyan. The regions in OFC encircled in white, and the regions in the ventral temporal cortex encircled in red represent the inclusive average labels, or all the voxels from which BOLD data were extracted for our cortical ROI analyses. The following cortical regions were activated above a threshold of t > 3 by the M > P contrast: orbitofrontal cortex [−30, 28, −11], inferior frontal gyrus [−40, 33, 16], anterior inferior/middle temporal gyrus [−65, −21, −6], and the inferior parietal lobule [−40, −44, 49] in the left hemisphere; and orbitofrontal cortex [10, 23, −19], anterior inferior temporal gyrus [52, −12, −22], and inferior parietal lobule [39, −18, 60] in the right hemisphere. The P > M contrast activated the left middle and posterior fusiform cortex [−49, −68, −12], and the right mid-fusiform [28, −50, −12] gyrus and occipitotemporal sulcus [40, −80, −12].

Although the RTs we obtained are relatively long, it is important to keep in mind two things about the paradigm. First, both M and P stimuli were quite difficult to recognize because of the manipulation that biased processing toward particular (M or P) neurons, which required low luminance contrast (<4% Weber contrast) for M-biased stimuli to minimize the contribution of P cells, and isoluminant, color-only stimuli to minimize M neuron contribution and bias processing toward the P cells. Second, the RTs include not only the time to recognize the stimulus, but also the time to make the size decision (“Bigger or smaller than a shoe box?”), which requires recognition of the stimulus. Although this decision component added a time constant (in the aggregate), it also enabled us to obtain a tangible measure of recognition success, rather than having the subjects report their recognition success by pressing a key at the recognition instant, which cannot be verified.

fMRI results

The main contrast of interest in the present study was comparing the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response elicited by M-biased stimuli with the response elicited by P-biased stimuli. The statistical parametric map of fMRI activity resulting from this contrast is displayed in Figure 3B.

Activity in OFC and the ventrotemporal regions was of greatest interest in the present study because of our hypotheses about M- versus P-driven activation of OFC and the previously identified anatomical substrates of object recognition (Logothetis and Sheinberg, 1996; Tanaka, 1996; Malach et al., 2002). Hence, we focus here on the ventral surface activations. As can be seen in the statistical parametric map in Figure 3B, the M condition activated OFC to a significantly greater extent, whereas the P condition differentially engaged the ventral occipitotemporal cortex, particularly the middle and posterior fusiform gyrus. The left OFC site is close (<7 mm between centers) to the OFC site that was activated more for recognized versus not-recognized objects previously (Bar et al., 2001), as well as the OFC activation evoked when comparing LSF versus HSF stimuli (Bar et al., 2006).

This differential OFC activation, coupled with faster recognition of M-biased stimuli, supports the notion that M-biased stimuli trigger the activation of top-down facilitation in OFC, resulting in improved recognition efficiency. In addition, M-biased stimuli also elicited stronger activity, compared with P-biased stimuli, in the very anterior part of the right temporal cortex, which may be related to either the projection of M information to OFC or feedback from of OFC to IT when M information is available. The amygdala also showed increased activation for M-biased compared with P-biased stimuli.

Conversely, the object recognition regions in the middle and posterior fusiform gyri and the right occipitotemporal cortex were activated to a greater extent in the P condition (Fig. 3B). This larger activation for P versus M, along with slower recognition of P-biased object drawings might have arisen because of the lack of top-down input from OFC for P-biased stimuli, and this reduction of top-down feedback to the fusiform cortex necessitated more extensive (and slower) bottom-up analysis in the ventral temporal cortex. We examine and discuss activity in these cortical regions in detail below.

Region of interest analyses

To explore our hypotheses about the neural activity in OFC and in the ventral temporal object recognition regions in more depth, we performed region of interest (ROI) analyses in these areas. We extracted the BOLD signal from voxels in these regions that were active during the general task compared with the baseline (an ALL vs NULL contrast, minimum voxel extent of 15 voxels) and examined whether the BOLD signal in these ROIs was predictive of recognition efficiency as indicated by RT changes for M and P conditions. We normalized the RT change measure by transforming each subject's RT distribution into z-scores and then computing the z-values of each condition mean (M and P) in the overall RT distribution. This is a more sensitive measure of performance change for a particular condition than using raw RT differences, because it puts the deviation of each condition's mean from the grand mean in the context of that individual's RT distribution and makes the M and P condition differences more comparable across subjects. We then tested the correlation between the normalized RT means for M and P trials with the corresponding BOLD signal change in the M and P conditions. In OFC, the BOLD signal in the M condition was predictive of M condition recognition advantage, negatively correlating with the normalized RT mean for this condition (r = −0.87, p < 0.005 in the left OFC; r = −0.69, p < 0.04 in the right OFC) (Fig. 4, bottom panels). In other words, larger BOLD signal for the M condition in OFC was associated with greater recognition speed advantage for M-biased stimuli compared with P-biased stimuli. Conversely, OFC signal for the P condition did not predict recognition performance in either hemisphere (r = 0.47, p > 0.24 in the left OFC ROI; r = 0.14, p > 0.71 in the right OFC ROI) and indeed showed the opposite tendency (Fig. 4, top panels). We computed Fisher's Z, or the difference between the effect sizes (Rosenthal and Rosnow, 1991), for each pair of correlations to ascertain whether the dissociation between M and P BOLD signal correlations with recognition performance was significant. For left OFC correlations, we obtained Fisher's Z = −3.7, p < 0.0001; and for the right OFC, Z = −1.98, p < 0.024. These results suggest that OFC activity plays a critical role in facilitating recognition but only when the stimuli are able to engage magnocellular processing, as was indeed the case with our M-biased stimuli.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Correlation between BOLD signal in OFC and M and P recognition speed change from the grand mean. Top panels show the correlation plots for the P condition, the bottom panels for the M condition for all subjects that had enough significant voxels in these ROIs to meet the extent threshold (15 voxels). The left hemisphere correlation plots are displayed on the left. The P condition (red; top panels) correlations were not significant. The units on the X-axes are the z-values of the M (squares) or P (circles) condition mean, a normalized measure of change relative to each subject's overall RT distribution. Negative z-values denote an RT advantage for that condition. Note that almost all the RT mean z-values for the M condition (squares; bottom panels) are negative, whereas the inverse applies to the P condition.

We next examined activity in the ventrotemporal ROIs encompassing the fusiform gyri and occipitotemporal sulci in both hemispheres. These regions were activated to a greater extent by P stimuli compared with M stimuli (Fig. 3B). In both the left and the right hemispheres, signal in voxels activated by a task versus baseline contrast showed significant positive correlations for the P condition between performance and fMRI signal change (r = 0.62, p < 0.04 in the left hemisphere; r = 0.66, p > 0.03 in the right hemisphere) (Fig. 5, top panels). This finding suggests that inferotemporal object recognition regions are engaged proportionately more as recognition difficulty increases, at least when M-biased top-down guidance from OFC is absent. In the M condition, this relationship did not exist: BOLD signal in either hemisphere was not significantly correlated with recognition performance (r = −0.06, p < 0.85 in the left hemisphere; r = −0.46, p < 0.15 in the right hemisphere) (Fig. 5, bottom panels). Fisher's Z for these correlation pairs was Z = −1.57, p < 0.058 (left IT) and Z = −2.58, p < 0.005 (right IT), indicating a dissociation between the correlations of P and M BOLD signal with recognition performance. It seems somewhat counterintuitive that M BOLD signal in IT does not significantly correlate with behavior (although there is a nonsignificant trend in the right IT, perhaps reflecting the right hemisphere bias for M information). Although M information facilitates object recognition processes in IT, the activity in this case may mainly reflect P information processing (as indicated by the P BOLD correlations with behavior). In real-world situations, both types of information are present in the stimulus, and OFC presumably contributes M information to frame and guide P information processing in IT. However, in our paradigm, P information is unavailable in the M condition, and therefore IT may be unable to contribute much to the top-down information being sent from OFC. If most of the work in interpreting these M stimuli is done by OFC, it would lead to a relative lack of M-related modulation in IT.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Correlation between BOLD signal in the middle/posterior fusiform cortex and M and P recognition speed change from the grand mean. Top panels show the correlation plots for the P condition; the bottom panels show the correlation plots for the M condition for all subjects that had enough significant voxels in these ROIs to meet the extent threshold (15 voxels). The left hemisphere correlation plots are displayed on the left. In the M condition (bottom panels; squares), BOLD signal in the fusiform cortex was not significantly correlated with recognition performance in either hemisphere. The units on the X-axes are the z-values of the subjects' M (squares) or P (circles) condition mean, a normalized measure of change relative to each subject's overall RT distribution. Negative z-values denote an RT advantage; positive values indicate an RT cost.

In addition to OFC and IT, we examined activity in the right anterior temporal pole (Fig. 3B). In this ROI, BOLD signal elicited in the P condition positively correlated with RT (r = 0.71; p < 0.03), whereas the M condition negatively correlated with RT (r = −0.52; p < 0.15). This difference in r values was significant (Fisher's Z = −2.43; p < 0.008). This site may be a part of the feedback projection from OFC to the temporal lobe, possibly via the uncinate fasciculus, which connects OFC with the temporal poles (Catani et al., 2002; Mori et al., 2002). Given that for P stimuli the correlations in this ROI are similar to the correlations found in the more occipital part of the ventrotemporal cortex, and for the M stimuli the correlations in this focus resemble those obtained in OFC, this region may be a functionally important junction where top-down and bottom-up processes interact directly. Future research using imaging methods with better temporal resolution (e.g., MEG) will help in elucidating the neural processes in this region.

We also performed ROI analyses in the left and right amygdala to explore whether these nuclei were sensitive to the M versus P manipulation. In the right amygdala, the BOLD signal in the M condition indeed was significantly greater than in the P condition (t–10 = 3.4; p < 0.008). In the left amygdala, the pattern was the same (M > P) but did not reach significance (t10 = 1.6; p < 0.15). However, performing the same correlation analyses for the left and right amygdala revealed that these differences were not related to the M versus P recognition speed advantage observed in OFC (correlation, p > 0.60 and p > 0.81 for the M, and p > 0.31 and p > 0.81 for the P bold signal, in the left and right amygdala, respectively). This finding indicates that the amygdala receives a substantially greater M input than P input, possibly as part of a subcortical M projection to OFC. However, given the insensitivity of the amygdala responses to the M versus P recognition advantage, it appears to have had no direct relation to recognition performance per se.

Analyses of functional connectivity

Dynamic causal modeling

To examine functional changes in interregional connectivity related to our experimental manipulation (M vs P), we used dynamic causal modeling, a recently developed and validated method of inferring directional connectivity between neural regions based on fMRI time series from those regions (Friston et al., 2003; Penny et al., 2004a; Friston, 2006). Dynamic causal models (DCMs) have three sets of parameters: extrinsic input into one or more regions, intrinsic connectivity among the modeled regions, and bilinear parameters encoding the modulation of the specified intrinsic connections by experimental perturbations (Friston et al., 2003; Friston, 2006). Typically, a set of plausible DCMs is specified and compared using Bayesian model selection (BMS) (Penny et al., 2004a; Stephan and Penny, 2006). BMS performs pair-wise comparisons of all the models that take into account the parameter fit as well as the complexity of the models. BMS uses two approximations, the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) and the Aikake information criterion (AIC), to estimate the Bayes factor (BF), or the evidence of one model versus another given the observed data. BIC favors simpler models, whereas AIC favors more complex models (Kaas and Raftery, 1993). Therefore, a conservative convention has been established in which one model is deemed a better fit than another if both BIC and AIC BFs concur and both are > e or ∼2.78 (Penny et al., 2004a; Fairhall and Ishai, 2007; Stephan et al., 2007). As an additional safeguard, here, we take the smaller of the BIC and AIC BFs and use a robust method called the “positive evidence ratio” (PER) for group comparisons (Stephan and Penny, 2006), in which the number of individuals within the sample in whom a particular model is deemed optimal is divided by the number of individuals whose data favor another model.

To perform DCM and BMS analyses, we first reanalyzed our data with SPM5 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm5/). The following preprocessing steps were performed: all functional images were realigned to the first image in the series, motion-corrected, normalized to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template, and smoothed with an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. The general linear model (GLM) included regressors for the M- and P-biased conditions, as well as a third condition comprising all visual input. The latter condition was added specifically for DCM analysis to be used as a direct input into a lower visual region from which connections to higher regions propagate and are modulated by our experimental manipulations. The GLM also included the motion-correction parameters to regress out any remaining contributions of motion in the scanner. The activation pattern was very similar to what we had found in the surface-based analysis with FS-FAST/Freesurfer reported above. For DCM analysis, we obtained fMRI time series from three regions in the right hemisphere located in the middle occipital gyrus (MOG), fusiform gyrus (FG), and OFC for each subject that had significant activation clusters in the three regions. Activity in OFC and FG, as well as connectivity between these regions, are the focus of our experimental manipulation, and MOG is an early object form processing region (Ishai et al., 2000) that was strongly activated (t10 = 9.9; p < 0.00001 uncorrected) by all object stimuli in our study. Voxels within a 6 mm sphere centered on the most significant voxel in a cluster and activated at a significance level of p < 0.001 were extracted. For two subjects, the significance level had to be lowered to p < 0.01 to obtain time series from all three regions, and one subject could not meet these criteria and had to be excluded from the analysis. The mean Talairach coordinates (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) for the time series extracted from these regions were [41(2.1), −78(1.4), −7(1.1)] for MOG, [44(3.9), −50(5.8), −7(1.6)] for FG, and [7(2), 31(5.5), −18(1.6)] for OFC. The SE is reported in the parentheses, and the conversion from MNI to Talairach coordinates was performed with the mni2tal conversion utility (http://imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/downloads/MNI2tal/mni2tal.m).

We defined simple three-tier DCMs with the six possible permutations of intrinsic connectivity patterns among the three regions, given extrinsic visual input into MOG and forward connections from MOG to FG and OFC (Fig. 6). The intrinsic connectivity patterns in the DCMs were modeled after those tested in Fairhall and Ishai (2007). Comparison of the estimated DCMs with BMS (Penny et al., 2004a) generally favored a model with parallel forward connections to OFC and FG from MOG with a backward connection from OFC to FG (Fig. 6, model 4), with the next-best model positing a forward connection from MOG to OFC and a backward connection from OFC to FG (Fig. 6, model 3), both in agreement with the top-down framework tested here. Models 4 and 3 were the only models specifying a unidirectional feedback connection from OFC to FG, and they were strongly favored over the rest of the models. There was very strong positive evidence for model 4 in four subjects, whereas model 3 was favored over model 4 in two subjects, giving a PER of 2:1. Three other subjects showed strong evidence for models 4 and 3 versus the remaining models, but the two leading models could not be differentiated, and in one subject, model 2 was favored. It is important to note that the strong positive evidence for model 4 (and to a lesser extent, model 3) was not a result of an inherent bias in the models (e.g., because of differences in the complexity of their connections). Model 4 typically prevailed whether it was simpler or more complex than the model with which it was being compared. It is also the model that has the architecture proposed by Bar (2003) and tested here: that is, separate bottom-up projections from the early visual regions to IT and OFC, with the latter (M-biased) projection triggering top-down facilitation via a backward projection from OFC to IT.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Top, The six intrinsic connectivity patterns of dynamic causal models tested in our functional connectivity analyses. Bottom, Left, Intrinsic connection weights; middle and right, modulatory connections activated by P and M bias in the stimuli. The thick arrows indicate significant effects at p < 0.05.

We next examined the modulatory connectivity in this leading model, model 4. In DCM, modulatory connections reflect increases (or decreases) in connectivity between two regions given some experimental manipulation, compared with the intrinsic connections between the same regions that capture connectivity in the absence of experimental input (Friston et al., 2003; Friston, 2006). Our hypothesis, encoded in the bilinear, modulatory connectivity parameters, was that in the P-biased condition, the forward connection from MOG to FG would be positively modulated, whereas in the M-biased condition, the forward connection from MOG to OFC, and the backward connection from OFC to FG, would increase its connection strength. Examining each of the modulatory connections revealed that P-bias in the stimuli significantly increased connectivity in the forward MOG → FG connection, with the average rate constant of 0.09 ± 0.04 Hz (p < 0.045), whereas M-bias strengthened connectivity in the forward MOG → OFC pathway, 0.12 ± 0.04 Hz (p < 0.011) and in the backward OFC → FG connection, 0.07 ± 0.03 Hz (p < 0.025). These average modulations constitute increases of 32, 68, and 183% in connection strength over the intrinsic connections among these regions. Thus, our predictions for the activity in this network were confirmed, and these findings are consistent with the framework proposed by the top-down facilitation model tested here (Bar, 2003). It is important to note that these findings provide support for functional connections between particular regions in our network but are unable to specify the exact anatomical pathways that mediated the observed modulation of activity. Future studies with more complex DCM structures may be able to elaborate the structure of these pathways.

Discussion

The primary goal of this study was to test our hypothesis that fast magnocellular pathways connecting the early visual areas with OFC are used in top-down facilitation of object recognition. We used stimuli designed to bias visual processing toward either the magnocellular or the parvocellular processing pathways, and showed the following: (1) M-biased stimuli were recognized faster than P-biased stimuli despite the higher brightness and perceived recognizability of P-biased stimuli; (2) M-biased stimuli activated OFC more than P-biased stimuli did, whereas P-biased stimuli activated the ventrotemporal object recognition regions to a greater extent; (3) for M-, but not for P-biased stimuli, larger BOLD signal in OFC was correlated with a recognition speed advantage; (4) larger BOLD signal in the fusiform cortex was associated with an increase in recognition RT for P-biased stimuli; and (5) M-bias in stimuli increased conduction of information from MOG and OFC, and from OFC to FG; whereas P-bias increased conduction in the MOG → FG pathway (all relative to intrinsic connections).

These findings provide strong support for the recognition facilitation mechanism tested here (Bar, 2003), which posits that fast magnocellular pathways connecting early visual and object recognition regions with OFC are critical for top-down facilitation of object recognition. When this projection can be engaged early, as was the case for our M-biased stimuli, recognition speed and accuracy improve along with positive signal changes in OFC. When M-driven top-down facilitation is reduced, as was the case with our P-biased stimuli, OFC is less able to trigger top-down facilitation, resulting in greater engagement of the bottom-up object recognition regions in the fusiform cortex, as shown by increased effective connectivity in the MOG → FG pathway and the increased ventral activation for the P stimuli, and slower recognition.

Although the function of OFC, particularly the medial OFC, has been often implicated in reward association learning, decision-making, cognitive control, and emotion, it receives inputs from all the sensory modalities, making it one of the most polymodal cortical regions (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). The somewhat more lateral orbitofrontal regions have been shown to be sensitive to visual stimuli, including faces and facial expression (Blair et al., 1999; Aharon et al., 2001; Winston et al., 2002; Morris and Dolan, 2004; Nomura et al., 2004; Strauss et al., 2005), as well as visual object stimuli (Thorpe et al., 1983; Bar et al., 2001; Hurliman et al., 2005; Zago et al., 2005; Bar et al., 2006), consistent with the lateral sensory OFC network proposed previously (Ongur and Price, 2000; Ongur et al., 2003). OFC sends feedback projections to many cortical regions, including strong connections with object recognition regions in the ventral temporal cortex (Cavada et al., 2000). Indeed, the effectiveness of object recognition has been shown to be impaired when the connections between these ventrotemporal areas and the prefrontal cortex are severed (Tomita et al., 1999; Chafee and Goldman-Rakic, 2000). We reduced the effectiveness of these connections with our psychophysical manipulation, by using stimuli (P-biased) that were ill-suited for magnocellular processing and showed that it impaired the recognition of these stimuli. In other words, the low-resolution information processed by magnocellular neurons is critical for facilitation of recognition, because it is used in fast triggering of the relevant top-down processes. It is worth noting that we do not claim that recognition is impossible without this top-down, M-driven facilitation, but rather that without informative magnocellular input, recognition processes are less able to benefit from top-down guidance and thus require more time to be completed.

What exact anatomical pathways might be subserving these connections? One possibility may be via the dorsal visual stream, which is known to have a substantial magnocellular input (Bullier, 2001), projects to the parietal cortex, and has connections with the frontal eye fields in the prefrontal cortex and with OFC (Cavada and Goldman-Rakic, 1989; Fang et al., 2005). However, our analyses revealed relatively little difference between M and P activation in the parietal regions. Another intriguing possibility is a subcortical projection from the pulvinar or mediodorsal nuclei of the thalamus. These pathways are poorly understood, but it is thought that the pulvinar nucleus receives partially processed information from the sensory cortices and projects to the prefrontal cortex and possibly other subcortical structures (Goldman-Rakic and Porrino, 1985; Giguere and Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Guillery, 1995; Romanski et al., 1997; Grieve et al., 2000; Guillery and Sherman, 2002). This pathway may be part of a bypass route from the early visual regions to the higher-order regions, such as OFC (Sherman and Guillery, 2004). OFC also has massive reciprocal connections with multiple nuclei within the amygdala (Rempel-Clower and Barbas, 2000; Ghashghaei et al., 2007), which are intermingled with inputs from the temporal visual association cortices (Ghashghaei and Barbas, 2002). The amygdala receives a subcortical projection from the superior colliculus via the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus, and amygdala lesions result in impaired cortical visual processing (Vuilleumier et al., 2002; Dolan and Vuilleumier, 2003). This subcortical, possibly magnocellular, pathway (Schiller et al., 1979) has been proposed as an alternate visual input route enabling the processing of affective stimuli (emotional faces) in patients with striate cortex lesions (Morris et al., 1999), neonates (Morton and Johnson, 1991), and prosopagnosia patients (de Gelder et al., 2003). Morris et al. (1999) found increased right, but not left, amygdala activation in healthy subjects exposed to “unseen” (masked) fear faces, which is similar to the observed amygdala activation pattern in this study with M-biased stimuli (i.e., greater right amygdala activation). Moreover, Vuilleumier et al. (2001) showed that responses in the amygdala were not attenuated by lack of spatial attention to fearful faces, suggesting that the amygdala is involved in fast, preattentive responses to threat stimuli, possibly based on low-resolution information. Our findings of greater BOLD signal in the amygdala for M-biased stimuli, compared with the activity elicited by P-biased stimuli, provide some support to this hypothesis. Furthermore, the lack of a direct relationship between amygdala activity and recognition performance implies that the amygdala is not primarily involved in facilitating the recognition of stimuli, at least of the emotionally neutral stimuli we used here, beyond screening M information for danger and relaying it to OFC for additional interpretation. Future research, including neural fiber tracking with diffusion imaging and more complex dynamic causal models are needed to characterize the exact neural pathways involved in triggering and mediating top-down facilitation of object recognition.

Conclusions

Our results show that stimuli biased toward magnocellular processing were recognized faster and evoked greater activity in the orbitofrontal cortex, compared with stimuli biased to include primarily parvocellular information. The fMRI signal in the orbitofrontal cortex predicted recognition speed advantage for M versus P stimuli. Last, M-bias in the stimuli increased the connection strength between the middle occipital gyrus and the orbitofrontal cortex, as well as between the orbitofrontal and the inferior temporal cortex. These findings provide strong evidence that fast magnocellular projections to the orbitofrontal cortex, combined with a top-down projection from the orbitofrontal to the inferior temporal cortex, play a critical role in top-down facilitation of visual object recognition.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the James S. McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Research Award #21002039, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grants NS50615 and NS44319 (M.B.), National Institutes of Health National Center for Research Resources Grant 5P41RR014075, Massachusetts General Hospital Fund for Medical Discovery Grant (K.K.), and the Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery Institute. We thank A. S. Ghuman for insightful suggestions; R. B. Adams for statistical advice; K. E. Stephan and A. Ishai for help with DCM analyses; and E. Aminoff, M. J. Fenske, N. Gronau, and M. Mason for valuable discussions.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Moshe Bar, Martinos Center at Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 149 Thirteenth Street, Charlestown, MA 02129. bar{at}nmr.mgh.harvard.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Aharon I,
    2. Etcoff N,
    3. Ariely D,
    4. Chabris CF,
    5. O'Connor E,
    6. Breiter HC
    (2001) Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron 32:537–551.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Bar M
    (2003) A cortical mechanism for triggering top-down facilitation in visual object recognition. J Cogn Neurosci 15:600–609.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bar M,
    2. Tootell RB,
    3. Schacter DL,
    4. Greve DN,
    5. Fischl B,
    6. Mendola JD,
    7. Rosen BR,
    8. Dale AM
    (2001) Cortical mechanisms specific to explicit visual object recognition. Neuron 29:529–535.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bar M,
    2. Kassam KS,
    3. Ghuman AS,
    4. Boshyan J,
    5. Schmid AM,
    6. Dale AM,
    7. Hamalainen MS,
    8. Marinkovic K,
    9. Schacter DL,
    10. Rosen BR,
    11. Halgren E
    (2006) Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:449–454.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Blair RJ,
    2. Morris JS,
    3. Frith CD,
    4. Perrett DI,
    5. Dolan RJ
    (1999) Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain 122:883–893.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Brainard DH
    (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision 10:433–436.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Bullier J
    (2001) Integrated model of visual processing. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 36:96–107.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Bullier J,
    2. Nowak LG
    (1995) Parallel versus serial processing: new vistas on distributed organization of the visual system. Curr Opin Neurobiol 5:497–503.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Butler PD,
    2. Martinez A,
    3. Foxe JJ,
    4. Kim D,
    5. Zemon VM,
    6. Silipo G,
    7. Mahoney J,
    8. Shpaner M,
    9. Jalbrzikowski M,
    10. Javitt DC
    (2006) Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments. Brain 130:417–430.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Callaway EM
    (2005) Structure and function of parallel pathways in the primate early visual system. J Physiol (Lond) 566:13–19.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Casagrande VA
    (1994) A third parallel visual pathway to primate area V1. Trends Neurosci 17:305–310.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Catani M,
    2. Howard RJ,
    3. Pajevic S,
    4. Jones DK
    (2002) Virtual in vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. NeuroImage 17:77–94.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Cavada C,
    2. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1989) Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe. J Comp Neurol 287:422–445.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Cavada C,
    2. Company T,
    3. Tejedor J,
    4. Cruz-Rizzolo RJ,
    5. Reinoso-Suarez F
    (2000) The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 10:220–242.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Chafee MV,
    2. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (2000) Inactivation of parietal and prefrontal cortex reveals interdependence of neural activity during memory-guided saccades. J Neurophysiol 83:1550–1566.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Chen CM,
    2. Lakatos P,
    3. Shah AS,
    4. Mehta AD,
    5. Givre SJ,
    6. Javitt DC,
    7. Schroeder CE
    (2006) Functional anatomy and interaction of fast and slow visual pathways in macaque monkeys. Cereb Cortex 17:1561–1569.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Cheng A,
    2. Eysel UT,
    3. Vidyasagar TR
    (2004) The role of the magnocellular pathway in serial deployment of visual attention. Eur J Neurosci 20:2188–2192.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Cox RW
    (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comp Biomed Res 29:162–173.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Dale AM,
    2. Sereno MI
    (1993) Improved localization of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach. J Cogn Neurosci 5:162–176.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Dale AM,
    2. Fischl B,
    3. Sereno MI
    (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage 9:179–194.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. de Gelder B,
    2. Frissen I,
    3. Barton J,
    4. Hadjikhani N
    (2003) A modulatory role for facial expressions in prosopagnosia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:13105–13110.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Dolan RJ,
    2. Vuilleumier P
    (2003) Amygdala automaticity in emotional processing. Ann NY Acad Sci 985:348–355.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Fairhall SL,
    2. Ishai A
    (2007) Effective connectivity within the distributed cortical network for face perception. Cereb Cortex 17:2400–2406.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    1. Fang PC,
    2. Stepniewska I,
    3. Kaas JH
    (2005) Ipsilateral cortical connections of motor, premotor, frontal eye, and posterior parietal fields in a prosimian primate, Otolemur garnetti. J Comp Neurol 490:305–333.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Sereno MI,
    3. Dale AM
    (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis: II. Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. NeuroImage 9:195–207.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Liu A,
    3. Dale AM
    (2001) Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans Med Imag 20:70–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Friston KJ
    1. Friston KJ
    (2006) in Statistical parametric mapping: the analysis of functional brain images, Dynamic causal models for fMRI, ed Friston KJ (Elsevier, Amsterdam), pp 541–560.
  28. ↵
    1. Friston KJ,
    2. Harrison L,
    3. Penny W
    (2003) Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage 19:1273–1302.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Ghashghaei T,
    2. Barbas H
    (2002) Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience 115:1261–1279.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Ghashghaei T,
    2. Hilgetag CC,
    3. Barbas H
    (2007) Sequence of information processing for emotions based on the anatomic dialogue between prefrontal cortex and amygdala. NeuroImage 34:905–923.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Giguere M,
    2. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1988) Mediodorsal nucleus: areal, laminar, and tangential distribution of afferents and efferents in the frontal lobe of rhesus monkeys. J Comp Neurol 277:195–213.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Goldman-Rakic PS,
    2. Porrino LJ
    (1985) The primate mediodorsal (MD) nucleus and its projection to the frontal lobe. J Comp Neurol 242:535–560.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Grieve KL,
    2. Acuna C,
    3. Cudeiro J
    (2000) The primate pulvinar nuclei: vision and action. Trends Neurosci 23:35–39.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Grossberg S
    (1980) How does a brain build a cognitive code? Psychol Rev 87:1–51.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Guillery RW
    (1995) Anatomical evidence concerning the role of the thalamus in corticocortical communication: a brief review. J Anat 187:583–592.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Guillery RW,
    2. Sherman SM
    (2002) Thalamic relay functions and their role in cortiocortical communication: generalizations from the visual system. Neuron 33:163–175.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Hendry SHC,
    2. Reid RC
    (2000) The koniocellular pathway in primate vision. Annu Rev Neurosci 431:168–181.
    OpenUrl
  38. ↵
    1. Hicks TP,
    2. Lee BB,
    3. Vidyasagar TR
    (1983) The responses of cells in macaque lateral geniculate nucleus to sinusoidal gratings. J Physiol (Lond) 337:183–200.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    1. Hurliman E,
    2. Nagode JC,
    3. Pardo JV
    (2005) Double dissociation of exteroceptive and interoceptive feedback systems in the orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex of humans. J Neurosci 25:4641–4648.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Ishai A,
    2. Ungerleider LG,
    3. Martin A,
    4. Haxby JV
    (2000) The representation of objects in the human occipital and temporal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 12:35–51.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Kaas RE,
    2. Raftery AE
    (1993) Bayes factors and model uncertainty. University of Washington: Technical Report 254.
  42. ↵
    1. Kosslyn SM
    (1994) Image and brain (MIT, Cambridge, MA).
  43. ↵
    1. Kringelbach ML,
    2. Rolls ET
    (2004) The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog Neurobiol 72:341–372.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Lamme VA,
    2. Roelfsema PR
    (2000) The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci 23:571–579.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Lee BB,
    2. Pokorny J,
    3. Smith VC,
    4. Martin PR,
    5. Valberg AJ
    (1990) Luminance and chromatic modulation sensitivity of macaque ganglion cells and human observers. J Opt Soc Am A 7:2223–2236.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Logothetis NK,
    2. Sheinberg DL
    (1996) Visual object recognition. Ann Rev Neurosci 19:577–621.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Malach R,
    2. Levy I,
    3. Hasson U
    (2002) The topography of high-order human object areas. Trends Cogn Sci 6:176–184.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Maunsell JH,
    2. Nealey TA,
    3. dePriest DD
    (1990) Magnocellular and parvocellular contributions to responses in the middle temporal visual area (MT) of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 10:3323–3334.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  49. ↵
    1. Merigan WH,
    2. Maunsell JH
    (1993) How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annu Rev Neurosci 16:369–402.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Mori S,
    2. Kaufmann WE,
    3. Davatzikos C,
    4. Stieltjes B,
    5. Amodei L,
    6. Fredericksen K,
    7. Pearlson GD,
    8. Melhem ER,
    9. Solaiyappan M,
    10. Raymond GV,
    11. Moser HW,
    12. van Zijl PC
    (2002) Imaging cortical association tracts in the human brain using diffusion-tensor-based axonal tracking. Magn Res Med 47:215–223.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Morris JS,
    2. Dolan RJ
    (2004) Dissociable amygdala and orbitofrontal responses during reversal fear conditioning. NeuroImage 22:372–380.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Morris JS,
    2. Ohman A,
    3. Dolan RJ
    (1999) A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:1680–1685.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Morton J,
    2. Johnson MH
    (1991) CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychol Rev 98:164–181.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Koch C,
    2. Davis J
    1. Mumford D
    (1994) in Large scale neuronal theories of the brain, Neuronal architectures for pattern-theoretic problems, eds Koch C, Davis J (MIT, Cambridge, MA), pp 125–152.
  55. ↵
    1. Nomura M,
    2. Ohira H,
    3. Haneda K,
    4. Iidaka T,
    5. Sadato N,
    6. Okada T,
    7. Yonekura Y
    (2004) Functional association of the amygdala and ventral prefrontal cortex during cognitive evaluation of facial expressions primed by masked angry faces: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage 21:352–363.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Ongur D,
    2. Price JL
    (2000) The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex 10:206–219.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Ongur D,
    2. Ferry AT,
    3. Price JL
    (2003) Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. J Comp Neurol 460:425–449.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Penny WD,
    2. Stephan KE,
    3. Mechelli A,
    4. Friston KJ
    (2004a) Comparing dynamic causal models. NeuroImage 22:1157–1172.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Rempel-Clower NL,
    2. Barbas H
    (2000) The laminar pattern of connections between prefrontal and anterior temporal cortices in the rhesus monkey is related to cortical structure and function. Cereb Cortex 10:851–865.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Romanski LM,
    2. Giguere M,
    3. Bates JF,
    4. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1997) Topographic organization of medial pulvinar connections with the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 379:313–332.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Rosenthal R,
    2. Rosnow RL
    (1991) Essentials of behavioral research: methods and data analysis (McGraw-Hill, New York), Ed 2..
  62. ↵
    1. Schechter I,
    2. Butler PD,
    3. Silipo G,
    4. Zemon V,
    5. Javitt DC
    (2003) Magnocellular and parvocellular contributions to backward masking dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Res 64:91–101.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Schiller PH,
    2. Malpeli JG,
    3. Schein SJ
    (1979) Composition of geniculo-striate input to superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 42:1124–1133.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    1. Segonne F,
    2. Dale AM,
    3. Busa E,
    4. Glessner M,
    5. Salvolini U,
    6. Hahn HK,
    7. Fischl B
    (2004) A hybrid approach to the skull-stripping problem in MRI. NeuroImage 22:1160–1175.
    OpenUrl
  65. ↵
    1. Segonne F,
    2. Grimson E,
    3. Fischl B
    (2005) A genetic algorithm for the topology correction of cortical surfaces. Inf Process Med Imaging 19:393–405.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Shapley R
    (1990) Visual sensitivity and parallel retinocortical channels. Annu Rev Psychol 41:635–658.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Chalupa LM,
    2. Werner JS
    1. Sherman SM,
    2. Guillery RW
    (2004) in The visual neuroscience, The visual relays in the thalamus, eds Chalupa LM, Werner JS (MIT, Cambridge, MA), pp 565–592.
  68. ↵
    1. Steinman BA,
    2. Steinman SB,
    3. Lehmkuhle S
    (1997) Transient visual attention is dominated by the magnocellular stream. Vision Res 37:17–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Friston KJ
    1. Stephan KE,
    2. Penny WD
    (2006) in Statistical parametric mapping: the analysis of functional brain images, Dynamic causal models and Bayesian selection, ed Friston KJ (Elsevier, Amsterdam), pp 577–585.
  70. ↵
    1. Stephan KE,
    2. Marshall JC,
    3. Penny WD,
    4. Friston KJ,
    5. Fink GR
    (2007) Interhemispheric integration of visual processing during task-driven lateralization. J Neurosci 27:3512–3522.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  71. ↵
    1. Strauss MM,
    2. Makris N,
    3. Aharon I,
    4. Vangel MG,
    5. Goodman J,
    6. Kennedy DN,
    7. Gasic GP,
    8. Breiter HC
    (2005) fMRI of sensitization to angry faces. NeuroImage 26:389–413.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Talairach J,
    2. Tournoux P
    (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain (Georg Thieme Verlag, Stuttgart).
  73. ↵
    1. Tanaka K
    (1996) Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annu Rev Neurosci 19:109–139.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Thorpe SJ,
    2. Rolls ET,
    3. Maddison S
    (1983) The orbitofrontal cortex: neuronal activity in the behaving monkey. Exp Brain Res 49:93–115.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Tomita H,
    2. Ohbayashi M,
    3. Nakahara K,
    4. Hasegawa I,
    5. Miyashita Y
    (1999) Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval. Nature 401:699–703.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Tootell RB,
    2. Hamilton SL,
    3. Switkes E
    (1988) Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. IV. Contrast and magno-parvo streams. J Neurosci 8:1594–1609.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  77. ↵
    1. Ullman S
    (1995) Sequence seeking and counter streams: a computational model for bidirectional information flow in the visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 1:1–11.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Vuilleumier P,
    2. Armony JL,
    3. Clarke K,
    4. Husain M,
    5. Driver J,
    6. Dolan RJ
    (2002) Neural response to emotional faces with and without awareness: event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual extinction and spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 40:2156–2166.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Vuilleumier P,
    2. Armony JL,
    3. Driver J,
    4. Dolan RJ
    (2001) Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 30:829–841.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Winston JS,
    2. Strange BA,
    3. O'Doherty J,
    4. Dolan RJ
    (2002) Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nat Neurosci 5:277–283.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Zago L,
    2. Fenske MJ,
    3. Aminoff E,
    4. Bar M
    (2005) The rise and fall of priming: how visual exposure shapes cortical representations of object. Cereb Cortex 15:1655–1665.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 27 (48)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 27, Issue 48
28 Nov 2007
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition
Kestutis Kveraga, Jasmine Boshyan, Moshe Bar
Journal of Neuroscience 28 November 2007, 27 (48) 13232-13240; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3481-07.2007

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition
Kestutis Kveraga, Jasmine Boshyan, Moshe Bar
Journal of Neuroscience 28 November 2007, 27 (48) 13232-13240; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3481-07.2007
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Memory Retrieval Has a Dynamic Influence on the Maintenance Mechanisms That Are Sensitive to ζ-Inhibitory Peptide (ZIP)
  • Neurophysiological Evidence for a Cortical Contribution to the Wakefulness-Related Drive to Breathe Explaining Hypocapnia-Resistant Ventilation in Humans
  • Monomeric Alpha-Synuclein Exerts a Physiological Role on Brain ATP Synthase
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Influence of Reward on Corticospinal Excitability during Movement Preparation
  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.