Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Asymmetry of Anticipatory Activity in Visual Cortex Predicts the Locus of Attention and Perception

Chad M. Sylvester, Gordon L. Shulman, Anthony I. Jack and Maurizio Corbetta
Journal of Neuroscience 26 December 2007, 27 (52) 14424-14433; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3759-07.2007
Chad M. Sylvester
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gordon L. Shulman
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anthony I. Jack
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Maurizio Corbetta
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Additional Files
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Task structure. Each trial began with an auditory preparatory cue indicating which of two peripheral locations subjects should covertly attend. After a variable SOA, Gabor patches appeared briefly at both locations, coincident with an auditory cue indicating the target stimulus. Subjects reported the orientation of the Gabor at the target location with a button press. This study focused on BOLD activity after the preparatory cue but before the onset of the visual targets.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Covert attention to a particular location, in the absence of visual stimulation, modulates activity representing all locations. A, Visual cortex contains a spatiotopic map in which nearby portions of cortex represent nearby portions of space. Representations of the target locations in the experimental task, as well as unattended locations in the lower and central fields, are displayed on a flattened representation of early visual cortex. These outlines are based on independent localizer scans. B, C, Endogenous BOLD activity after cues to covertly attend to the top left (B) or top right (C) target location (5° eccentricity) in the absence of visual stimulation. Note the swath of positive modulation corresponding to the attended location for each condition. BOLD activity in early visual cortex corresponding to the rest of the visual field is mostly negative. This creates a sharp peak of activity at the portion of visual cortex representing the attended location. D, The difference in endogenous BOLD activity between trials with leftward and rightward cues. Portions of cortex representing the attended locations show the largest differences. Cortex representing the lower visual field also shows a weak preference for attending to the contralateral hemifield. Portions of cortex representing the center of the visual field, however, show lower BOLD activity for attending to the contralateral versus ipsilateral hemifield, thus showing a preference for ipsilateral attention. White lines are approximate borders between retinotopic visual areas based on a standard atlas (Van Essen, 2002). Black lines are approximate isoeccentricity lines based on an average of six subjects from another study (Jack et al., 2006). Data maps are summed over the last two pretarget MR frames (frames 5 and 6), smoothed with a 5 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel, averaged across subjects, and projected onto a flattened representation of posterior occipital cortex using the PALS (population-average, landmark, and surface-based) atlas (Van Essen et al., 2001).

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    A network of regions modulated with the covert locus of attention. Most regions displayed higher activity for contralateral attention relative to ipsilateral attention. Homologous regions in opposite hemispheres (e.g., left and right FEF) can be thought of as portions of a single priority map with opposite (leftward attention versus rightward attention) spatial preferences. Regions from the left hemisphere of a single subject are displayed above. Time courses are averaged across subjects and display only prestimulus, preparatory BOLD activity.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Activity in portions of cortex representing the left and right target locations is positively correlated across trials. A, Plot of trial-by-trial preparatory activity in the portion of visual cortex representing the right location (left hemisphere) versus preparatory activity in the portion representing the left location (right hemisphere). Each dot represents a single trial. The histograms on the x and y axes are projections of the trial data and display the distributions of preparatory activity for the left and right hemispheres, respectively. Although the signal related to the locus of spatial attention goes in opposite directions for the regions from opposite hemispheres, preparatory activity is nevertheless positively correlated across trials. Accounting for this positive correlation could dramatically improve the ability to decode the locus of spatial attention. This can be seen by noting that although there is a clear separation between the dots corresponding to the leftward and rightward cued trials, the histograms reveal that there is much less separation within either single region. B, Same as A for FEF. C, D, Histograms of preparatory activity derived by subtracting right hemisphere activity from left hemisphere activity for the same data plotted in A and B. The relative activity is clearly better at distinguishing between leftward versus rightward covert attention than activity in either left or right regions alone. All data are from a random half of the trials from a single subject (2).

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Comparing activity between portions of cortex representing the two target locations dramatically improves decoding of the locus of attention compared with measuring activity for a single location. This improvement is mostly caused by removal of positive correlation. Bars above display how well the activity in each region discriminates between leftward and rightward cued trials, according to different decoding schemes. White bars indicate sampling only one of the two target locations, gray bars indicate comparing cortical activity for left and right locations on the same trial, and black bars indicate comparing activity when the trial pairings are randomly assigned. The bracket i represents the increase in the ADI caused by the removal of positively correlated activity. The bracket ii shows the increase in the ADI caused by the constructive combining of signals related to the locus of attention that go in opposite directions. In A, the subtractions (black and gray bars) are between homologous regions in opposite hemispheres. B compares subtracting homologous regions (V3AH) versus subtracting nonhomologous regions in opposite hemispheres (V3AN), using V3A as an example. V3AH is identical to V3A in A. The white bar for V3AN is the same as for V3AH and represents the average discriminability between individual left hemisphere and right hemisphere V3A. The gray bar for V3AN represents the average ADI for all nonhomologous opposite hemisphere subtractions involving V3A (L FEF − R V3A, L V3A − R FEF, L IPS − R V3A, etc.) with trial order preserved, whereas the black bar is the same with trials shuffled before the subtraction. Note that the improvement in the ADI caused by the removal of positively correlated noise is higher for homologous versus nonhomologous subtractions (compare Hi and Ni).

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    The relative activity difference between left and right V3A predicts performance in the upcoming trial. Relatively more right V3A activity predicts correct performance for leftward trials whereas relatively more left V3A activity predicts correct performance for rightward trials. The only exception across the six subjects and two target locations is leftward trials for subject 3. Mean (left V3A minus right V3A) magnitudes are plotted above separately for each subject and condition. The average performance discriminability (correct vs incorrect) based on the full distributions of these magnitudes was significantly greater than chance at 0.54, averaged across subjects and target location. Although modest, this finding was consistent across subjects (as shown above) and replicated in an independent dataset (see Results, Spatial attention signals predict perception).

  • Figure 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 7.

    Sustained signal modulations are not required to encode the locus of attention. A, Average signal modulation in FEF and the portion of visual cortex representing the target location, averaged across subjects and hemispheres. Prestimulus activity in FEF is sustained whereas activity in visual cortex returns nearly to baseline by the end of the preparatory period. B, Time point-by-time point attention discriminability indices for the same regions. The regions indicate the locus of attention equally well at the end of the preparatory period. This suggests that sustained BOLD activity is neither necessary nor sufficient as a marker of attention to a particular location.

Tables

  • Figures
  • Additional Files
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Activities in portions of cortex representing different locations are highly correlated.

    FEFIPSV3AV1-V4
    FEF0.5540.5440.5230.517
    IPS0.5440.5690.5010.573
    V3A0.5230.5010.749*0.642
    V1–V40.5170.5730.6420.814*
    • Interhemispheric regional correlations in preparatory activity between regions modulating with the direction of the cue. Bold values indicate correlations between homologous portions of the same region (e.g., L FEF and R FEF) in opposite hemispheres. All other values indicate the average of two interhemispheric correlations (e.g., L FEF with R IPS and R FEF with L IPS). Note that homologous pairings are usually more highly correlated than nonhomologous pairings.

    • View popup
    Table 2.

    Activities in portions of cortex representing different locations are highly correlated

    Lower fieldCentral fieldStimulus
    Lower field0.780*0.7200.761
    Central field0.7200.824*0.722
    Stimulus0.7610.7220.903*
    • Interhemispheric correlations between portions of visual cortex representing different locations. Bold values indicate correlations between portions of visual cortex representing mirror locations in opposite (left/right) hemifields. All other values indicate the average of two interhemispheric correlations (e.g., L target with R lower field and R target with L lower field). Note that correlations are highest for portions of cortex representing mirror locations. Correlations are computed separately for each subject and separately for trials with leftward and rightward cues, and then averaged across subject and cue. Asterisks indicate regions for which correlations between homologous regions were significantly higher than correlations with any other opposite-hemisphere pairing.

Additional Files

  • Figures
  • Tables
  • Supplemental Data

    Files in this Data Supplement:

    • supplemental material - Supplemental Material
    • supplemental material - Figure 1
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 27 (52)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 27, Issue 52
26 Dec 2007
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • 2007 Author and Subject Index
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Asymmetry of Anticipatory Activity in Visual Cortex Predicts the Locus of Attention and Perception
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Asymmetry of Anticipatory Activity in Visual Cortex Predicts the Locus of Attention and Perception
Chad M. Sylvester, Gordon L. Shulman, Anthony I. Jack, Maurizio Corbetta
Journal of Neuroscience 26 December 2007, 27 (52) 14424-14433; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3759-07.2007

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Asymmetry of Anticipatory Activity in Visual Cortex Predicts the Locus of Attention and Perception
Chad M. Sylvester, Gordon L. Shulman, Anthony I. Jack, Maurizio Corbetta
Journal of Neuroscience 26 December 2007, 27 (52) 14424-14433; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3759-07.2007
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Memory Retrieval Has a Dynamic Influence on the Maintenance Mechanisms That Are Sensitive to ζ-Inhibitory Peptide (ZIP)
  • Neurophysiological Evidence for a Cortical Contribution to the Wakefulness-Related Drive to Breathe Explaining Hypocapnia-Resistant Ventilation in Humans
  • Monomeric Alpha-Synuclein Exerts a Physiological Role on Brain ATP Synthase
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Individual Differences in Amygdala-Medial Prefrontal Anatomy Link Negative Affect, Impaired Social Functioning, and Polygenic Depression Risk
  • Influence of Reward on Corticospinal Excitability during Movement Preparation
  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.