Abstract
In Drosophila, formation of aversive olfactory long-term memory (LTM) requires multiple training sessions pairing odor and electric shock punishment with rest intervals. In contrast, here we show that a single 2 min training session pairing odor with a more ethologically relevant sugar reinforcement forms long-term appetitive memory that lasts for days. Appetitive LTM has some mechanistic similarity to aversive LTM in that it can be disrupted by cycloheximide, the dCreb2-b transcriptional repressor, and the crammer and tequila LTM-specific mutations. However, appetitive LTM is completely disrupted by the radish mutation that apparently represents a distinct mechanistic phase of consolidated aversive memory. Furthermore, appetitive LTM requires activity in the dorsal paired medial neuron and mushroom body α′β′ neuron circuit during the first hour after training and mushroom body αβ neuron output during retrieval, suggesting that appetitive middle-term memory and LTM are mechanistically linked. Last, experiments feeding and/or starving flies after training reveals a critical motivational drive that enables appetitive LTM retrieval.
Introduction
Studies in humans and several animal models have demonstrated that multiple training trials with rest intervals (spaced training) is most effective in producing long-term memory (LTM) (Ebbinghaus, 1885; Carew et al., 1972; Frost et al., 1985; Tully et al., 1994). A single trial, or even multiple training trials without rest intervals (massed training), usually only forms robust short-term memory (STM). However, there are some notable exceptions that indicate that spaced training is not always essential to form LTM. In conditioned taste aversion learning in rodents (and pond snails), a single exposure of a tastant, followed by malaise (or salt exposure), leads to a long-lasting avoidance of the associated taste (Garcia et al., 1955; Sugai et al., 2007). Similarly, in the passive avoidance task in chicks, one peck at a bitter-tasting colored bead leads to avoidance of a bead of that color for days (Lossner and Rose, 1983). Long-lasting memories formed by a single trial can also be appetitive. In rodents, LTM for taste preference can be formed if presentation of a tastant is followed by an injection of a nutritional supplement (Garcia et al., 1967). Furthermore, a single trial of conditioning pairing odorant/tastant and sucrose in the pond snail forms an appetitive LTM for that odorant/tastant (Fulton et al., 2005). Last, a single trial of appetitive conditioning forms a protein synthesis-independent long-lasting memory in honeybees (Menzel, 2001). The common factor in these five cases is the involvement of gustation and feeding behavior suggesting that fast acquisition of stable memory may be a conserved feature of feeding-related learning.
In Drosophila, formation of aversive LTM requires 5–10 spaced training trials, pairing odor with punitive shock, with 15 min rest intervals (Tully et al., 1994). Flies can also be trained with odor and a more ethologically relevant sucrose reward (Tempel et al., 1983; Schwaerzel et al., 2003, 2007; Keene et al., 2006; Kim et al., 2007), and even in the first report, it was noted that memory after two trials of appetitive conditioning persisted longer than that after two trials of aversive conditioning (Tempel et al., 1983). We therefore sought to determine whether appetitive olfactory conditioning could form LTM and whether appetitive LTM formation shared rules and mechanisms with that of aversive LTM. There are a number of reasons to favor an appetitive protocol. First, sugar is a salient stimulus to the fly because it represents food. Second, the gustatory receptors that sense sucrose (Dahanukar et al., 2007; Jiao et al., 2007; Slone et al., 2007), and the receptor neurons that express these receptors (Thorne et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2004; Marella et al., 2006), are well described giving us instant access to unconditioned stimulus processing and the neural circuits delivering that information to deeper layers of the brain. Last, appetitive memory formation and retrieval requires motivation because only hungry flies display learned behavior (Tempel et al., 1983; present study).
Materials and Methods
Flies.
Fly stocks were raised on standard cornmeal food at 25°C and 60% relative humidity. The wild-type Drosophila strain used in this study was Canton-S. The flies harboring the heat-shock-inducible hs-dCreb2-b repressor transgene are those described as 17-2 (Yin et al., 1994). Flies harboring the uas-dCreb2-b repressor transgene on the second chromosome were described previously (Yu et al., 2006). We expressed dCreb2-b in the mushroom bodies (MBs) by crossing female flies harboring the uas-dCreb2-b repressor transgene to male flies containing the c739-GAL4, c772-GAL4, and MB247-GAL4 driver insertions (Zars et al., 2000; McGuire et al., 2001). All resultant c739-GAL4;uas-dCreb2-b, c772-GAL4/uas-dCreb2-b, and uas-dCreb2-b;MB247-GAL4 flies are heterozygous for each transgene. Heterozygote control uas-dCreb2-b/+, c739/+, c772/+, and MB247/+ flies were generated by crossing wild-type female flies to uas-dCreb2-b, c739, c772, and MB247 male flies. Mutant radish (rsh), crammer (cerP ), and tequila (teqf01792 ) strains are those described previously (Folkers et al., 1993; Comas et al., 2004; Didelot et al., 2006). We used flies carrying a single insertion of the uas-shi ts1 transgene (Kitamoto, 2001) on the third chromosome. We previously described the dorsal paired medial (DPM) neuron-expressing c316-GAL4 (Waddell et al., 2000) and Mz717-GAL4 (Keene et al., 2004) drivers. The c739-GAL4 that expresses in the MB αβ lobes and the c305a-GAL4 line that primarily expresses in the MB α′β′ lobes were described previously (McGuire et al., 2001; Krashes et al., 2007). The c547, Ruslan, and Feb170 GAL4 lines that express in ellipsoid body (EB) ring neurons were described previously (Renn et al., 1999; Wu et al., 2007). We generated c305a-GAL4;uas-shi ts1, c739-GAL4;uas-shi ts1, c316-GAL4/uas-shi ts1, Mz717-GAL4/uas-shi ts1, c547-GAL4;uas-shi ts1, Ruslan-GAL4;uas-shi ts1, and Feb170-GAL4;uas-shi ts1 flies by crossing uas-shi ts1 females to c305a, c739, c316, Mz717, c547, Ruslan, and Feb170 male flies. Heterozygote control uas-shi ts1/+, c305a/+, c739/+, c316/+, Mz717/+, c547/+, Ruslan/+, and Feb170/+ flies were generated by crossing wild-type females to uas-shi ts1, c305a, c739, c316, Mz717, c547, Ruslan, and Feb170 male flies. All flies tested are heterozygote for the listed transgenes, and a mixed population of sexes were assayed, except for Feb170-GAL4;uas-shi ts1, in which only females were tested because Feb170 is on the X-chromosome.
Cycloheximide feeding, heat-shock, and cold-shock anesthesia protocols.
Wild-type flies were food deprived at 25°C for 16 h in milk bottles containing one 10 × 6 cm Whatmann 3MM filter paper soaked with 3% ethanol solution or 35 mm cycloheximide (CXM) in 3% ethanol. Flies were then transferred to milk bottles containing one 10 × 6 cm Whatmann 3MM filter paper soaked with water and given 1 h to recover before training. Previous experiments determined that a 12–15 h CXM exposure before olfactory conditioning attenuates 24 h memory performance after an aversive spaced training protocol (Tully et al., 1994).
We induced expression of the hs-dCreb2-b transgene with heat shock using a variation of the previously published protocol (Yin et al., 1994). After 14–16 h of food deprivation, flies were transferred to 37°C preheated milk bottles and incubated in a 37°C incubator for 30 min. Flies were then transferred back to milk bottles at room temperature containing one 8 × 3 cm Whatmann 3MM filter paper soaked with water and given 2 h to recover before training.
Cold-shock anesthesia was delivered as described previously (Folkers et al., 1993). Briefly, flies were transferred to prechilled plastic vials 1 h before, immediately after, 2 h after, or 12 h after training, and the vials were put in a 4°C ice bucket for 2 min. Locomotor activity ceased immediately. Flies were allowed to recover by transferring them to room-temperature vials containing a water-dampened filter paper. They started moving within 30 s and were stored at 25°C until testing.
Behavioral analyses.
The olfactory appetitive conditioning paradigm was performed as described previously (Keene et al., 2006; Krashes et al., 2007). Flies were food deprived for 16–20 h before conditioning in milk bottles containing a 10 × 6 cm Whatmann 3MM filter paper soaked with water. The training tube [positive conditioned stimulus (CS+)] contains a filter paper with dried sucrose that covers the entire wall of the training tube. The filter paper was made by applying a saturated sucrose solution and allowing it to dry before use. Another tube (CS−) was prepared with a filter paper that was soaked in water (and allowed to dry). Conditioning was performed as follows: ∼100 starved flies were loaded into the elevator section of a T-maze. Flies were transferred to the CS− tube, and odor was delivered for 2 min. After 30 s of clean air stream, they were transferred back into the elevator and then into the (CS+) tube containing sugar reinforcement in the presence of another odor for 2 min. Memory was tested at stated times after training. Except in Figure 7, flies were stored in empty vials containing only a water-dampened filter paper between training and testing. The performance index (PI) is calculated as the number of flies running toward the conditioned odor minus the number of flies running toward the unconditioned odor, divided by the total number of flies in the experiment. A single PI value is the average score from two groups of flies of the identical genotype trained with the reciprocal CS+/CS− odor combination (3-octanol or 4-methylcyclohexanol). To reduce variation between experiments, all genotypes were tested in parallel in each experimental session. We previously determined that the c316/+, c316-GAL4/uas-shi ts1, Mz717/+, and Mz717-GAL4/uas-shi ts1 flies have normal odor and sucrose acuity (Waddell et al., 2000; Keene et al., 2004, 2006) and c305a/+, c305a-GAL4;uas-shi ts1, c739/+, and c739-GAL4;uas-shi ts1 flies have normal odor acuity (Krashes et al., 2007). CXM feeding and manipulation of rsh, cerP , teqf01792 , and hs-dCreb2-b (with and without heat shock) has previously been determined to have no adverse effect on odor acuity (Folkers et al., 1993; Yin et al., 1994; Comas et al., 2004; Didelot et al., 2006). We tested the sucrose acuity of the remaining stocks (Table 1) using a previously reported method (Keene et al., 2006).
Olfactory acuity and sucrose acuity for strains used in this study
We assayed fast phototaxis in a countercurrent apparatus (Benzer, 1967) using a simplified protocol of that described. Flies were knocked down into the start tube, the machine was laid flat on the table, and flies were given 30 s to run toward the light. Flies were then trapped and counted. The phototaxis index is the percentage of flies in the tube nearest the light. We similarly assayed negative geotaxis, except the countercurrent apparatus was positioned vertically. The geotaxis index is the percentage of flies in the top tube after 30 s.
Statistical analyses were performed using KaleidaGraph (Synergy Software, Reading, PA). Overall, ANOVAs were followed by planned pairwise comparisons between the relevant groups with a Tukey's honestly significant difference post hoc test. Unless stated otherwise, all olfactory conditioning experiments are n ≥ 8.
Results
Persistent memory after a single session of appetitive conditioning
Formation of aversive olfactory LTM in Drosophila requires 5–10 sessions of associative conditioning with 15 min rest intervals between training bouts (spaced training) (Tully et al., 1994). The requirement for spaced training has been suggested to reflect the presence of a mechanistic threshold for LTM induction (Yin et al., 1994; Isabel et al., 2004) because a single training session, or even 10 training sessions, with no rest (massed training) does not form LTM (Tully et al., 1994).
Two reports of appetitive conditioning in Drosophila using a two-trial massed procedure observed measurable levels of memory up to 24 h after training (Tempel et al., 1983; Schwaerzel et al., 2007), and we previously determined that flies trained with a 2 min odor exposure paired with sucrose revealed robust memory performance up to 6 h (Keene et al., 2006; Krashes et al., 2007). We therefore further investigated the perdurance of appetitive memory following our previously described single conditioning session protocol.
Briefly, flies starved for 16–20 h were exposed to one odor without sugar reinforcement for 2 min, followed by another odor with sugar reinforcement for 2 min. Flies were then transferred to empty food vials (with a damp filter paper) and stored until testing. To test memory, flies were transported to a choice point in a T-maze where they were given 2 min to choose between the two odors experienced during training. We tested appetitive olfactory memory 1, 3, 6, 9, 12, 24, 27, 30, 33, and 36 h after training. To our surprise, we found no significant decline in memory performance across this time period (Fig. 1). We reasoned that this robust 24 h memory after a single session of appetitive conditioning might be LTM. To test this, and to make as true a comparison as possible to previously described aversive LTM, we used a collection of the tools and protocols that were used in the defining studies (Tully et al., 1994; Yin et al., 1994).
Persistent memory after a single session of appetitive conditioning. Wild-type flies were food deprived for 16–20 h and were conditioned with odor and sucrose reinforcement (as described in Materials and Methods). After training, they were housed in empty vials with water-dampened filter paper until they were tested. Different populations were tested once for odor memory 1, 3, 6, 9, 12, 24, 27, 30, 33, and 36 h after training. Beyond 36 h, a significant number of animals perished, presumably of starvation. However, those that survived displayed robust memory performance. Error bars are SEM. All n ≥ 8.
A single appetitive training session forms memory that requires new protein synthesis
A hallmark of LTM in all organisms is a requirement for new protein synthesis after training (McGaugh, 1966; Daniels, 1971; Jaffe, 1980; Davis and Squire, 1984; Mizumori et al., 1985; Montarolo et al., 1986; Rose and Jork, 1987; Castellucci et al., 1989; Tully et al., 1994; Schafe and LeDoux, 2000). The classic method to assess a role for new protein synthesis in memory is to feed animals the protein synthesis inhibitor CXM before or after training. A CXM feeding regimen was established in Drosophila that results in an ∼50% decrease in overall protein synthesis (Tully et al., 1994), and this protocol has subsequently been used by several groups (Ge et al., 2004; Mery and Kawecki, 2005; Yu et al., 2006). However, our appetitive conditioning protocol requires that the flies are food deprived before training, and therefore we could not administer CXM in a glucose solution without compromising acquisition (data not shown). We therefore administered 35 mm CXM in a 3% ethanol solution to the flies during the period of food deprivation before training. Following this protocol, we trained CXM-fed flies (and flies only fed 3% ethanol) and tested them for memory at several time points after training. We observed a striking time-dependent decline in memory performance after CXM feeding, consistent with the notion that long-lasting appetitive odor memory requires new protein synthesis (Fig. 2).
A single appetitive training session forms memory that requires new protein synthesis. Flies were either fed 35 mm CXM in 3% ethanol solution (open squares) or 3% ethanol alone (filled diamonds) during a 16 h starvation period before training. All flies were then trained, and different populations were tested once for odor memory 1, 3, 6, 12, or 24 h later. Error bars are SEM. Asterisks denote a significant difference (p < 0.05) at that time point from the performance of the other group. All n ≥ 8.
CXM feeding did not significantly affect memory before 6 h after training, similar to memory formed after aversive conditioning (Tully et al., 1994). Three-hour memory performance, often referred to as middle-term memory (MTM), of CXM-fed flies was comparable to that of flies food deprived on vehicle alone (p > 0.1). However, memory tested 6, 12, or 24 h after training revealed a significant difference between the CXM and vehicle groups (all p < 0.002), suggesting that a protein synthesis-dependent memory phase partly guides behavior at that time.
Previous work demonstrated that 1 d memory after massed training was unaffected by CXM administration (Tully et al., 1994), suggesting the CXM-treated flies are healthy 24 h after training and that they have adequate acuity and agility to perform in the memory assay. However, because drug administration is a relatively crude approach, it was important that we similarly demonstrate a specific effect on appetitive LTM. Furthermore, whereas flies were stored on food before and after electric-shock training (Tully et al., 1994), the flies in our appetitive assay are subjected to long periods of starvation before and after training. It was therefore conceivable that the observed decline in performance after CXM treatment was simply a result of a time-dependent decrease in health that was exacerbated by starvation. We therefore performed a control experiment to rule out this potential explanation.
We food deprived flies for 16–20 h in the presence of CXM or vehicle alone, and instead of training the flies, we transferred them to vials with a sucrose-impregnated filter paper for 2 min. We reasoned this would emulate the sucrose exposure they normally receive in the training session. Flies were then transferred to vials containing a damp filter paper and stored for 24 h. At that time, flies were conditioned and tested for 1 h memory. One-hour memory performance of CXM-fed flies (0.33 ± 0.03) was comparable (n = 8; p > 0.6) to those fed vehicle alone (0.31 ± 0.03). Therefore, the combination of CXM administration and prolonged food deprivation does not generally compromise learning and memory performance 25 h after CXM feeding. Furthermore, CXM feeding did not affect sucrose acuity (Table 1) or odor acuity.
Therefore, these data demonstrate that one appetitive olfactory conditioning session forms bona fide LTM that is dependent on new protein synthesis, implying a clear difference in the requirements to induce appetitive and aversive olfactory LTM in Drosophila.
Appetitive LTM depends on the action of cAMP response element-binding protein in the MBs
The transcription factor cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) is universally required for LTM, and it is widely believed that the requirement for new protein synthesis after training is, at least in part, a reflection of a need to translate transcripts from CREB-induced genes (Yin et al., 1994; Lonze and Ginty, 2002; Barco et al., 2003). A dominant-negative dCreb2-b repressor transgene driven by the heat-shock promoter (hs-dCreb2-b) has been reported to produce a heat-shock-dependent decrement of LTM formation (Yin et al., 1994; Perazzona et al., 2004). Inducing dCreb2-b with a 30 min heat shock 3 h before training specifically disrupted aversive LTM (Yin et al., 1994; Perazzona et al., 2004). We therefore tested whether induction of the dCreb2-b transgene also impaired appetitive LTM after a single training session.
We induced the dCreb2-b transgene by heat shocking food-deprived flies at 37°C for 30 min. We then allowed the flies to recover and express the transgene for 2 h before training. We subsequently trained the flies and measured their memory 24 h later. Induction of the dCreb2-b transgene severely disrupted 24 h appetitive memory. Performance of hs-dCreb2-b flies with heat shock was statistically different from hs-dCreb2-b flies without heat shock, wild-type flies with heat shock, and wild-type flies without heat shock (Fig. 3 A) (all p < 0.006). Furthermore, the performance of hs-dCreb2-b flies without heat shock was not statistically different from that of wild-type flies with and without heat shock (p > 0.9 and p > 0.4, respectively), suggesting that any residual dCreb2-b expression from the uninduced transgene did not disrupt appetitive LTM.
Inducible or region-restricted expression of dCREB2b disrupts appetitive LTM but not MTM. Wild-type flies and flies harboring an hs-dCreb2-b transgene were either heat shocked (+hs) for 30 min 2 h before training or were untreated (−hs). A , B , All groups were then trained and tested for 24 h memory ( A ) or 3 h memory ( B ). Flies harboring hs-dCreb2-b that were heat shocked displayed defective LTM compared with all other groups, but MTM was unaffected. C , Expressing a uas-dCreb2-b transgene in the MBs with c772, MB247, and c739 GAL4 drivers disrupts LTM. Appetitive LTM performance of c772/uas-dCreb2-b, uas-dCreb2-b;MB247, and c739/uas-dCreb2-b flies was statistically different from all other groups. D , Appetitive MTM was not affected by expressing a uas-dCreb2-b transgene in the MBs. Error bars are SEM. Asterisks denote a significant difference (p < 0.05) from all other unmarked groups.
dCreb2-b induction specifically disrupted LTM after multiple spaced trials of aversive conditioning and leaves both MTM and anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM) unaffected (Yin et al., 1994). We therefore tested whether dCreb2-b expression specifically affected appetitive LTM by assaying appetitive MTM 3 h after training. Three-hour appetitive memory performance of flies with induced dCreb2-b expression was comparable to all other groups tested: hs-dCreb2-b flies without heat shock and wild-type flies with and without heat shock (Fig. 3 B) (all p > 0.06). These data therefore demonstrate that formation of appetitive LTM after a single appetitive training session requires the acute action of CREB-dependent transcription.
Although controlling dCreb2-b expression with the heat-shock promoter allows fine temporal control, it does not provide any tissue specificity. Therefore, to compensate for this limitation, we also used the GAL4/UAS system to restrict expression of the dCreb2-b repressor to the MBs. We combined a uas-dCreb2-b transgene (Yu et al., 2006) with three different MB drivers: c739-GAL4 (McGuire et al., 2001), c772-GAL4, and MB247-GAL4 (Zars et al., 2000). These drivers all express GAL4 strongly in the MB αβ neurons, whereas in addition, MB247 expresses GAL4 in the γ neurons and c772 also expresses in γ and a few α′β′ neurons (Krashes et al., 2007). Similar to our results with the hs-dCreb2-b repressor, flies expressing uas-dCreb2-b in the MBs exhibited significantly reduced appetitive LTM measured 24 h after a single training session. Memory of c739/uas-dCreb2-b, c772/uas-dCreb2-b, and uas-dCreb2-b;MB247 flies was statistically different from wild-type flies and flies that are heterozygous for the single transgenes alone: uas-dCreb2-b/+, c739/+, c772/+, and MB247/+ (Fig. 3 C) (all p < 0.03).
The GAL4 approach expresses the uas-dCreb2-b repressor throughout the development of the flies. We therefore verified that this manipulation did not affect earlier phases of appetitive memory after a single training session. MTM measured 3 h after training revealed no statistical differences between all groups of flies tested: wild-type, uas-dCreb2-b/+, c739/uas-dCreb2-b, c772/uas-dCreb2-b, uas-dCreb2-b;MB247, c739/+, c772/+, and MB247/+ flies (Fig. 3 D) (all p > 0.3). Furthermore, all groups tested exhibited comparable odor and sucrose acuity to that of wild-type flies (Table 1).
Therefore, these data combining two approaches with either temporal or spatial control suggest that appetitive LTM, but not MTM, formed by a single training session requires the function of dCreb in the MBs. These data are consistent with findings for aversive LTM after spaced training (Yin et al., 1994; Perazzona et al., 2004; Yu et al., 2006).
Appetitive LTM is rapidly consolidated within 2 h after training and requires radish
The period of active memory consolidation in insects can be revealed by its sensitivity to cold-shock anesthesia (Erber, 1976; Quinn and Dudai, 1976; Tully et al., 1994). We therefore constructed a profile of memory consolidation after appetitive conditioning by assaying the effect on 24 h LTM of cold-shock anesthesia administered at different times before and after training. Flies were anesthetized by transferring them to prechilled vials and plunging them into a 4°C ice bath for 2 min. This induces a rapid anesthesia that is reversed a few minutes after returning flies to 25°C. LTM was severely reduced if cold shock was administered immediately after training but was unaffected if flies were anesthetized 1 h before training or 2 or 12 h after training. LTM performance of flies anesthetized immediately after training was significantly different from that of untreated flies (p < 0.006) and those anesthetized 1 h before training (p < 0.05), as well as those anesthetized 2 or 12 h after training (p < 0.03 and p < 0.001, respectively) (Fig. 4 A). Furthermore, LTM performance of flies anesthetized 1 h before training, or 2 or 12 h after training, was comparable to LTM of untreated flies (all p > 0.8). These data suggest that appetitive memory formed by our single 2 min training protocol is consolidated to the anesthesia-resistant form(s) within 2 h after training.
Appetitive memory is quickly consolidated and is disrupted by crammer, tequila, and radish mutation. A , Appetitive memory becomes resistant to disruption by cold-shock anesthesia within 2 h after training. Different populations of wild-type flies were subjected to a 2 min cold-shock anesthesia 1 h before, immediately after, or 2 or 12 h after training (open diamonds). They were then allowed to recover and were tested for 24 h appetitive memory. Only the performance of flies that were anesthetized immediately after training differed from that of the other groups and from flies that had not been anesthetized (filled square). All n ≥ 8. B , Twenty-four-hour appetitive memory is disrupted in crammer, tequila, and radish mutant flies. Twenty-four-hour memory performance of cer, teq, and rsh mutant flies was statistically different from wild-type flies. All n ≥ 12. C , Three-hour appetitive MTM is unaffected by cer and teq mutation but is significantly disrupted by rsh mutation. Three-hour memory performance of rsh flies was statistically different from that of all other groups. All n ≥ 11. Error bars are SEM. Asterisks denote a significant difference (p < 0.05) from all other unmarked groups.
The published literature for aversive olfactory memory in Drosophila supports the existence of two forms of consolidated memory. Single and massed trials of aversive conditioning form ARM (Tully et al., 1994) that depends on the function of a wild-type radish gene (Folkers et al., 1993; Folkers et al., 2006). In contrast, spaced training forms both ARM and protein synthesis-dependent LTM (Tully et al., 1994), although it is debated whether LTM and ARM exist in parallel or are mutually exclusive (Tully et al., 1994; Isabel et al., 2004). Because formation of appetitive LTM does not require spaced interval training, we investigated the involvement of rsh in appetitive LTM after a single training session.
We trained wild-type and rsh mutant flies (Folkers et al., 1993) and tested them for 24 h LTM. Surprisingly, appetitive LTM was abolished in rsh mutant flies, suggesting that both ARM and protein synthesis-dependent components depend on rsh function (Fig. 4 B). Performance of rsh mutant flies was statistically different from wild-type flies (p < 0.0001). We also tested 3 h appetitive MTM performance of rsh mutant flies. MTM performance of rsh flies was statistically different from and approximately half that of wild-type flies (p < 0.01) (Fig. 4 C), similar to that observed with aversive MTM (Folkers et al., 1993, 2006). These data are consistent with a published report that 3 h appetitive MTM is disrupted after a 1 min cold-shock anesthesia administered 30 min before testing (Schwaerzel et al., 2007).
Taken with previously published work, these data suggest that a single appetitive training session forms both anesthesia-sensitive memory, rsh-dependent consolidated memory, and protein synthesis-dependent LTM. Furthermore, the finding that LTM is absent in rsh mutants suggests that rsh is required for appetitive LTM, questioning the proposed mechanistic independence of ARM and LTM (Tully et al., 1994; Isabel et al., 2004).
The crammer and tequila LTM-specific mutants disrupt appetitive LTM
Our unexpected finding that the rsh mutation abolished appetitive LTM after a single training session led us to question the role of the crammer (cer) and tequila (teq) genes that have both been reported to specifically disrupt protein synthesis-dependent aversive LTM (Comas et al., 2004; Didelot et al., 2006). The cer gene encodes an inhibitor of the cathepsin subfamily of cysteine protease and is expressed in MB neurons and surrounding glial cells (Comas et al., 2004), whereas teq encodes a Drosophila ortholog of neurotrypsin serine protease [although this has been debated (Sonderegger and Patthy, 2007)] and its expression increases transiently after spaced training (Didelot et al., 2006).
We tested appetitive LTM performance of mutant flies carrying hypomorphic transposable element insertions in the cer (cerP ) and teq (teqf01792 ) genes (Comas et al., 2004; Didelot et al., 2006). Appetitive LTM measured 24 h after training was severely disrupted in both cerP and teqf01792 flies. Performance of cerP and teqf01792 mutant flies was statistically different from wild-type flies (both p < 0.0002) (Fig. 4 B). To verify that the mutant defect was specific to LTM, we also assayed the cerP and teqf01792 mutant flies for MTM 3 h after training. MTM performance of cerP and teqf01792 mutant flies was comparable to that of wild-type flies (both p > 0.8) (Fig. 4 C), suggesting that a single training session induces MTM but not LTM in cerP and teqf01792 mutant flies.
Therefore, the LTM-specific cerP and teqf01792 mutant flies provide independent evidence that a single appetitive training session forms protein synthesis-dependent LTM with some mechanistic similarity to aversive LTM that can only be formed by multiple spaced training trials.
DPM neurons and MB α′β′ neurons are critical for formation of appetitive LTM
We previously demonstrated that neurotransmission from both DPM neurons and MB α′β′ neurons is critical during the first 30–60 min after training for stable 3 h appetitive MTM (Keene et al., 2006; Krashes et al., 2007), whereas output from MB αβ neurons is required for appetitive MTM retrieval (Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Krashes et al., 2007). Because our experiments demonstrate that cold-shock anesthesia within the first few minutes after training severely disrupts appetitive memory (Fig. 4 A), we tested for a role of DPM neurons and MB α′β′ neurons during this time window in appetitive LTM. For these experiments, we used neuron-specific expression of the dominant temperature-sensitive shibire ts1 (shi ts1) transgene (Kitamoto, 2001). shi ts1 blocks dynamin-dependent membrane recycling and thereby synaptic vesicle release at the restrictive temperature of 31°C, and this blockade is reversible by returning flies to the permissive temperature of 25°C.
We used the c316-GAL4 and Mz717-GAL4 lines to test the role of DPM neurons (Waddell et al., 2000; Keene et al., 2004), the c305a-GAL4 line to test the role of MB α′β′ neurons (Krashes et al., 2007), and the c739-GAL4 line to test for the role of MB αβ neurons (McGuire et al., 2001; Krashes et al., 2007) in appetitive LTM. At 25°C, appetitive LTM performance of wild-type, uas-shi ts1, c316/+, c316/uas- shi ts1, Mz717/+, Mz717/uas- shi ts1, c305a/+, c305a; uas-shi ts1, c739/+, and c739;uas-shi ts1flies was comparable across groups (all p > 0.9) (Fig. 5 A). We therefore tested whether DPM, MB α′β′, and MB αβ neuron output was required during the first hour after training (Fig. 5 B). Flies were trained at the permissive temperature and shifted immediately after training to the restrictive temperature for 60 min, blocking DPM, MB α′β′, and MB αβ neurotransmission. The flies were then stored at the permissive temperature and tested for memory performance 23 h later. DPM and MB α′β′ neuron manipulation severely impaired LTM, but blocking MB αβ neurons did not (Fig. 5 B). Memory of c316/uas-shi ts1, Mz717/uas-shi ts1 and c305a; uas-shi ts1 flies was statistically different from wild-type, uas- shi ts1, and c739;uas-shi ts1 flies (all p < 0.03), whereas memory of c739; uas-shi ts1 flies was comparable to that of wild-type and uas-shi ts1 flies (both p > 0.9). These data suggest that output from both DPM and MB α′β′ neurons is required during the first hour after training for appetitive LTM, whereas MB αβ neuron output is dispensable.
Neurotransmission from MB α′β′ neurons and DPM neurons is required for consolidation of appetitive LTM, whereas transmission from MB αβ neurons is only required for retrieval. The temperature shift protocols are shown pictographically above each graph. A , The permissive temperature of 25°C does not affect 24 h appetitive LTM of any of the lines used in this study. All genotypes were trained and tested for 24 h memory at 25°C. All n ≥ 8. B , Blocking DPM neuron or MB α′β′ neuron output, but not MB αβ neuron output, for 1 h immediately after training severely impairs 24 h appetitive LTM. Flies were trained at 25°C, and immediately after training, they were shifted to 31°C for 60 min. Flies were then returned to 25°C and stored in empty vials until they were tested for 24 h appetitive LTM at 25°C. All n ≥ 11. C , Blocking MB αβ neuron output, but not DPM neuron or MB α′β′ neuron output, during testing abolishes 24 h appetitive LTM. Flies were trained at 25°C and stored in empty vials for 24 h. They were then shifted to 31°C for 15 min before they were tested for appetitive LTM. All n ≥ 8. Error bars are SEM. Asterisks denote a significant difference (p < 0.05) from all other unmarked groups. All flies harbor one copy of the uas-shi ts1 transgene.
MB αβ neuron output is required for retrieval of aversive odor memory after a single training trial (Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Krashes et al., 2007), for appetitive odor memory after two trials (Schwaerzel et al., 2003), and for aversive memory after spaced training (Isabel et al., 2004). We therefore tested the role of DPM, MB α′β′, and MB αβ neuron output during appetitive LTM retrieval. We trained flies at the permissive temperature and stored them at the permissive temperature for 24 h. At this time point, we blocked DPM, MB α′β′ neuron, or MB αβ neuron output by shifting the flies to the restrictive temperature and tested them for 24 h LTM. Strikingly, this manipulation severely impaired memory if αβ neuron output was blocked but did not affect performance if DPM or MB α′β′ neurons were blocked (Fig. 5 C). Memory of c739;uas-shi ts1 flies was statistically different from wild-type, uas-shi ts1, c316/uas-shi ts1, Mz717/uas-shi ts1, and c305a;uas-shi ts1 flies (all p < 0.01), whereas the performance of c316/uas-shi ts1, Mz717/uas-shi ts1, and c305a;uas-shi ts1 flies was comparable to that of wild-type and uas-shi ts1 flies (all p > 0.08).
Therefore, the fly processes appetitive LTM using the same parallel and sequential neural circuit mechanism that it uses to process MTM (Krashes et al., 2007). MB α′β′ neurons and DPM neurons are transiently required within the first hour after training to consolidate appetitive LTM, and output from αβ neurons is exclusively required to retrieve LTM. These data, taken with our previous work, indicate that appetitive LTM is mechanistically linked to appetitive MTM (Keene et al., 2006; Krashes et al., 2007).
Evidence against a role for EB ring neurons in LTM retrieval
A plausible caveat of the c739 driver line is that, in addition to strongly expressing in MB αβ neurons, it expresses in a specific subset of EB ring neurons in the central complex (Hanesch et al., 1989; Renn et al., 1999; Wu et al., 2007). Wu et al. (2007) proposed that aversive LTM consolidation involved “transfer” from MB to the ring neurons in the EB. Using the Feb170 driver line, they concluded that transmission from EB neurons was required to retrieve aversive LTM. We therefore tested for a role of Feb170 EB neurons in appetitive LTM retrieval. We assayed Ruslan-GAL4 (Dubnau et al., 2003; Wu et al., 2007) and c547 (Renn et al., 1999) that also express in EB neurons, in parallel for comparison (Fig. 6). We first noticed that the majority of Feb170;uas-shi ts1 flies died during starvation. Furthermore, on shifting to the restrictive temperature, surviving Feb170;uas-shi ts1 and c547;uas-shi ts1 flies exhibited reduced mobility, consistent with a previous study describing a role for the central complex in locomotion (Strauss and Heisenberg, 1993). Therefore, although appetitive LTM performance of Feb170;uas-shi ts1 flies and c547;uas-shi ts1 flies was significantly impaired, this is confounded by poor locomotor activity. Indeed, additional testing revealed that fed Feb170;uas-shi ts1 and c547;uas-shi ts1 flies failed olfactory acuity (Table 1), fast phototaxis (Fig. 6 E) and geotaxis (Fig. 6 F) tests that require locomotor competence at the restrictive temperature (29–31°C). In contrast, Ruslan;uas-shi ts1 flies had normal locomotor behavior and appetitive LTM retrieval. Although these lines may inactivate different subsets of EB neurons (Hanesch et al., 1989; Renn et al., 1999), these data challenge the utility of the Feb170 line for memory analysis and question the proposed role for the EB in LTM retrieval.
Evidence against a role for EB ring neurons in LTM retrieval. A–C , Projections of the entire midbrain of flies driving a uas-CD8::GFP transgene with the c547 ( A ), Ruslan ( B ), and Feb170 ( C ) enhancer trap lines. All lines show clear expression in R2/R4 ring neurons in the outer layers of the EB compared with R1/R3 neurons in c739 (data not shown). The driver name is listed in the lower left-hand corner of each panel. D , Blocking c547 or Feb170 neurons, but not Ruslan neuron output, for 15 min before and during testing impairs 24 h appetitive LTM performance. The temperature shift protocol is shown pictographically. All genotypes were trained at 25°C and tested for 24 h memory at 31°C. All flies with uas-shi ts1, n ≥ 8; heterozygous GAL4/+ flies, n ≥ 4. E , Blocking c547 or Feb170 neurons, but not Ruslan neurons, impairs phototaxis performance. All flies were tested at 31°C. F , Blocking c547 neurons or Feb170 neurons impairs negative geotaxis performance. All flies were tested at 31°C. All n ≥ 6. Error bars are SEM. Asterisks denote a significant difference (p < 0.05) from all other unmarked groups. All flies harbor one copy of the uas-shi ts1 transgene.
Extension of the LTM assay: satiety state regulates memory retrieval
It is necessary to food deprive flies before and after appetitive learning for them to display appetitive memory. This critical hunger drive is of great interest, but it imposes an obvious limitation on the appetitive LTM assay because starving flies for more than 2 d compromises viability. In our experiments described here, the flies that were tested for 36 h memory (Fig. 1) were starved for 16–20 h before training and up to 36 h after training. Beyond 36 h, a significant number of flies died, presumably of starvation. We therefore investigated whether we could extend the utility of the assay by feeding and restarving flies after training (Fig. 7). We food deprived two groups of flies for 16–20 h, trained them both using the standard protocol, and put them both into food vials for 24 h. One group was allowed to feed for an additional 24 h (group A), whereas the other was food deprived for 24 h (group B). We subsequently assayed the flies in groups A and B in parallel for 48 h appetitive memory. Strikingly, flies in group A that had been fed ad libitum between training and testing displayed little memory performance (0.08 ± 0.03), whereas those that had been restarved in group B exhibited robust memory performance (0.25 ± 0.03). The appetitive LTM performance of group A flies was significantly different from that of the group B (p < 0.005). These data therefore demonstrate that feeding to satiety suppresses memory retrieval and that this suppression can be reversed by subsequently restarving the flies before testing. Furthermore, this feeding and restarving protocol allows one to extend the time period that is available to study the mechanisms of appetitive memory.
Experiment to test whether satiation reversibly suppresses memory retrieval. Wild-type flies were starved for 16–20 h, trained with a single appetitive conditioning session, and were either returned to food vials for 48 h (group A) or for 24 h and then subsequently food deprived for the next 24 h (group B). Both groups were trained, stored, and tested for 48 h appetitive LTM at 25°C.
Discussion
One conditioning session forms appetitive LTM
A single 2 min training session pairing odor with sucrose forms appetitive memory that lasts for days. We cautiously use the term “session” rather than “trial” because, although the conditioned odor stimulus is continuously presented for 2 min, we do not know how often the flies sample the sugar unconditioned stimulus. One session of the established aversive training paradigm presents 12 shocks at 5 s intervals overlapping with 1-min-long odor exposure (Tully and Quinn, 1985), and therefore neither protocol is strictly “single-trial” learning. Nevertheless, our results present a profound difference between the training protocol requirements to form aversive and appetitive LTM in flies. Formation of aversive LTM requires 5–10 training sessions with rest intervals (Tully et al., 1994), whereas a single 2 min session is sufficient to form robust protein synthesis-dependent appetitive LTM. Appetitive LTM is disrupted by CXM feeding, inhibition of CREB-dependent transcription (Tully et al., 1994; Yin et al., 1994), and the crammer (Comas et al., 2004) and tequila (Didelot et al., 2006) genes, which suggests that it is bona fide LTM. Furthermore, these data indicate some mechanistic parallel between aversive and appetitive LTM.
Appetitive conditioning forms more distributed memory traces in the brain (Thum et al., 2007) and more efficiently forms LTM than aversive conditioning. We speculate that these properties of appetitive memory result from the ethological relevance of feeding and the salience of sucrose reinforcement. Furthermore, the salience is likely to be enhanced in hungry flies because they are motivated to seek food. There are a few other reports of single-trial training forming LTM. With the notable exception of fear conditioning in rodents (Fanselow, 1980), most involve feeding behavior and the gustatory pathway. In conditioned taste aversion experiments, rodents develop a long-lasting avoidance of a novel tastant after a single exposure of the tastant and delayed drug-induced malaise (Garcia et al., 1955). Similarly, pond snails develop long-lasting conditioned taste aversion if carrot juice is paired with salt exposure (Sugai et al., 2007), and 1-d-old chicks develop LTM to avoid pecking a colored bead if that bead was tainted with a bitter tasting compound when first presented (Lossner and Rose, 1983). There are also examples in which single-trial conditioning forms appetitive LTM. Rats deficient in thiamine can be trained to prefer non-nutritious saccharin-flavored water by pairing it with delayed an intramuscular thiamine injection (Garcia et al., 1967). Pond snails form appetitive LTM for the odorant/tastant amylacetate after a single trial of appetitive conditioning pairing it with sucrose (Fulton et al., 2005). Last, a single trial of appetitive conditioning in honeybees forms robust day-long memory that, surprisingly, does not require new protein synthesis after training (Menzel, 2001). Therefore, it is possible that the innate importance of food-seeking behavior and memory makes it particularly prone to fast consolidation to LTM.
Fast consolidation to LTM
The single training session appetitive LTM assay provides a unique advantage for the study of memory consolidation because one can manipulate the brain immediately after training during the initial period of memory formation. In contrast, 10 cycles of aversive spaced training takes 150 min to complete (Tully et al., 1994), and therefore one cannot perturb neural processing during this period without also interfering with acquisition. Using cold-shock anesthesia, we found that appetitive memory is quickly, and perhaps entirely, consolidated to anesthesia-resistant forms within 2 h after training.
Previous work in flies suggests that cold shock-resistant memory can be broken into two independent components, ARM that depends on the rsh gene and is resistant to CXM and LTM that is unaffected by rsh and is sensitive to CXM (Tully et al., 1994; Isabel et al., 2004). Feeding flies CXM disrupted appetitive LTM and produced a statistically significant defect 6 h after training, suggesting that protein synthesis-dependent LTM guides behavior at that time. Although the effect of CXM feeding is estimated to inhibit only 50% of global protein synthesis (Tully et al., 1994) and has to be partial, these data are consistent with the notion that consolidated memory before 6 h might be ARM. However, whereas aversive LTM requires protein synthesis and is not affected by rsh (Tully et al., 1994), appetitive LTM requires new protein synthesis and rsh, suggesting appetitive LTM and rsh-dependent appetitive memory do not represent separable memory phases. This result highlights a potentially major mechanistic difference between aversive and appetitive LTM, and that the relationship between ARM and LTM is worth revisiting. Unfortunately, the recent cloning of rsh does not provide any mechanistic insight because its primary sequence does not contain any known functional domains (Folkers et al., 2006).
Our data reveal a slight discrepancy in the notion that rsh, dCreb-dependent transcription and new protein synthesis are all necessary components of appetitive LTM. Cold-shock anesthesia indicates that appetitive memory consolidation is nearly complete 2 h after training and rsh mutant flies display defective performance 3 h after training, but neither dCreb2-b nor CXM feeding produced a significant difference in memory performance 3 h after training. We speculate that expression of early forms of appetitive LTM (E-LTM) depend on rsh and that because radish protein immunolocalized to neuropil (Folkers et al., 2006), radish might function in a synaptic tagging process that marks the relevant synapses for capture of dCreb2-dependent transcripts. This idea provides a plausible reason why radish is required both for E-LTM and for later appetitive LTM (L-LTM), whereas dCreb2-b only interferes with L-LTM. Similarly, we posit that CXM feeding blocks the translation of mRNAs that are direct and indirect targets of CREB and that are necessary for L-LTM. Similar models have been proposed based on work in rodents and Aplysia (Frey and Morris, 1997; Martin et al., 1997; Casadio et al., 1999; Barco et al., 2002; Si et al., 2003).
A neural circuit perspective
We previously determined that stable olfactory memory (MTM) observed 3 h after aversive and appetitive training requires the sequential involvement of different MB neuron subsets. MB α′β′ neurons are required during and after training to acquire and stabilize memory (Krashes et al., 2007), whereas MB αβ neuron output is only required to retrieve the memory (Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Krashes et al., 2007). Stable aversive and appetitive MTM also requires the action of MB-innervating DPM neurons during the first hour after training (Keene et al., 2004, 2006; Yu et al., 2005; Krashes et al., 2007). Similarly timed manipulation of these distinct neural circuit elements strongly impairs appetitive LTM, suggesting a tight mechanistic link between appetitive MTM and LTM.
Finding that consolidation of appetitive memory to a protein synthesis-dependent form requires the DPM–MB neural circuitry and that retrieval requires MB αβ neuron output is consistent with the idea that consolidated memory is represented in MB αβ neurons themselves. Several studies have now reported that MB neuron output is required to retrieve olfactory memory (Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Krashes et al., 2007), and a few have indicated that MB αβ neurons are particularly important to retrieve aversive and appetitive MTM (McGuire et al., 2001; Krashes et al., 2007) or aversive LTM (Isabel et al., 2004). A recent live-imaging study provided additional evidence that consolidated aversive LTM is represented in MB αβ neurons (Yu et al., 2006). Flies that had been space trained with odor and shock exhibited enhanced odor-evoked Ca2+ signals in the vertical α branch of MB αβ neurons 9–24 h after conditioning. The development of this memory “trace” was disrupted by CXM administration, by mutations in the amnesiac gene, and by expressing a transgenic dCreb2-b in MB αβ neurons. Furthermore, expression of the dCreb2-b transgene in MB αβ neurons also impaired aversive LTM behavior. These data are highly consistent with our findings described here for appetitive LTM after a single training session and therefore indicate that there are common mechanistic components to aversive and appetitive LTM. It is also worth noting that radish is strongly expressed in MB αβ neurons (Folkers et al., 2006). Therefore, this collection of findings provides strong evidence that consolidated aversive and appetitive LTM involves MB αβ neurons.
Our results do not support the recently proposed idea that LTM consolidation involves transfer from MB to EB (Wu et al., 2007). Although we used an appetitive memory assay, we found that Feb170;uas-shi ts1 flies have a pronounced locomotor defect and therefore these flies are not suitable for memory analysis. Furthermore, Ruslan GAL4 (Wu et al., 2007) and c305a (Krashes et al., 2007) express in EB ring neurons, but blocking these neurons does not affect appetitive LTM retrieval. Our data are instead consistent with the notion that the transfer of the MB lobe requirement within the first few hours after training may be the fly equivalent of systems consolidation.
Motivational control of appetitive memory retrieval
Our data clearly demonstrate that flies have to be hungry to effectively retrieve appetitive memory. Feeding them ad libitum after training suppressed memory performance, but restarving them restored memory performance. We propose this apparent context dependence of appetitive memory retrieval reflects a motivational state to seek food and therefore predict it is regulated by neuromodulatory systems that signal hunger (Melcher et al., 2007).
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant MH09883 to S.W. We thank Motojiro Yoshihara and Jena Pitman for critical comments on this manuscript and Jerry Yin, Thomas Preat, Ron Davis, and Josh Dubnau for fly strains.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Scott Waddell, Department of Neurobiology, University of Massachusetts Medical School, 364 Plantation Street, Worcester, MA 01605. scott.waddell{at}umassmed.edu
References
- Barco et al., 2002.↵
- Barco et al., 2003.↵
- Benzer, 1967.↵
- Carew et al., 1972.↵
- Casadio et al., 1999.↵
- Castellucci et al., 1989.↵
- Comas et al., 2004.↵
- Dahanukar et al., 2007.↵
- Daniels, 1971.↵
- Davis and Squire, 1984.↵
- Didelot et al., 2006.↵
- Dubnau et al., 2001.↵
- Dubnau et al., 2003.↵
- Ebbinghaus, 1885.↵
- Erber, 1976.↵
- Fanselow, 1980.↵
- Folkers et al., 1993.↵
- Folkers et al., 2006.↵
- Frey and Morris, 1997.↵
- Frost et al., 1985.↵
- Fulton et al., 2005.↵
- Garcia et al., 1955.↵
- Garcia et al., 1967.↵
- Ge et al., 2004.↵
- Hanesch et al., 1989.↵
- Isabel et al., 2004.↵
- Jaffe, 1980.↵
- Jiao et al., 2007.↵
- Keene et al., 2004.↵
- Keene et al., 2006.↵
- Kim et al., 2007.↵
- Kitamoto, 2001.↵
- Krashes et al., 2007.↵
- Lonze and Ginty, 2002.↵
- Lossner and Rose, 1983.↵
- Marella et al., 2006.↵
- Martin et al., 1997.↵
- McGaugh, 1966.↵
- McGuire et al., 2001.↵
- Melcher et al., 2007.↵
- Menzel, 2001.↵
- Mery and Kawecki, 2005.↵
- Mizumori et al., 1985.↵
- Montarolo et al., 1986.↵
- Perazzona et al., 2004.↵
- Quinn and Dudai, 1976.↵
- Renn et al., 1999.↵
- Rose and Jork, 1987.↵
- Schafe and LeDoux, 2000.↵
- Schwaerzel et al., 2003.↵
- Schwaerzel et al., 2007.↵
- Si et al., 2003.↵
- Slone et al., 2007.↵
- Sonderegger and Patthy, 2007.↵
- Strauss and Heisenberg, 1993.↵
- Sugai et al., 2007.↵
- Tempel et al., 1983.↵
- Thorne et al., 2004.↵
- Thum et al., 2007.↵
- Tully and Quinn, 1985.↵
- Tully et al., 1994.↵
- Waddell et al., 2000.↵
- Wang et al., 2004.↵
- Wu et al., 2007.↵
- Yin et al., 1994.↵
- Yu et al., 2005.↵
- Yu et al., 2006.↵
- Zars et al., 2000.↵