Abstract
Recent research on Alzheimer's disease (AD) has shown that cognitive and memory decline in this disease is accompanied by disrupted changes in the coordination of large-scale brain functional networks. However, alterations in coordinated patterns of structural brain networks in AD are still poorly understood. Here, we used cortical thickness measurement from magnetic resonance imaging to investigate large-scale structural brain networks in 92 AD patients and 97 normal controls. Brain networks were constructed by thresholding cortical thickness correlation matrices of 54 regions and analyzed using graph theoretical approaches. Compared with controls, AD patients showed decreased cortical thickness intercorrelations between the bilateral parietal regions and increased intercorrelations in several selective regions involving the lateral temporal and parietal cortex as well as the cingulate and medial frontal cortex regions. Specially, AD patients showed abnormal small-world architecture in the structural cortical networks (increased clustering and shortest paths linking individual regions), implying a less optimal topological organization in AD. Moreover, AD patients were associated with reduced nodal centrality predominantly in the temporal and parietal heteromodal association cortex regions and increased nodal centrality in the occipital cortex regions. Finally, the brain networks of AD were about equally as robust to random failures as those of controls, but more vulnerable against targeted attacks, presumably because of the effects of pathological topological organization. Our findings suggest that the coordinated patterns of cortical morphology are widely altered in AD patients, thus providing structural evidence for disrupted integrity in large-scale brain networks that underlie cognition. This work has implications for our understanding of how functional deficits in patients are associated with their underlying structural (morphological) basis.
Introduction
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a progressive, neurodegenerative disease characterized by the decline of cognitive and memory functions. Current theories of AD have posited that the decline arises from alterations in functional integration of distributed brain systems or from structural disconnection between regions because of white matter damage (for review, see Delbeuck et al., 2003). Evidence from previous neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies has shown AD-associated abnormalities not only in the functional connection of several specific brain regions, involving the prefrontal (Horwitz et al., 1987; Grady et al., 2001, 2003), hippocampus (Celone et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2006), cingulate (Greicius et al., 2004), and visual regions (Horwitz et al., 1995; Bokde et al., 2006), but also in the functional integration of the entire brain networks (Stam et al., 2006, 2007). There is also growing evidence to suggest AD-related abnormalities in the structural integrity of white matter such as corpus callosum, superior longitudinal fasciculus, and cingulum (Rose et al., 2000; Medina et al., 2006). In the current study, we focus on a specific issue that remains to be addressed relevant to alterations in the coordinated patterns of large-scale structural brain networks that are constructed using brain morphological features from structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).
Recent studies have suggested that there are coordinated variations in brain morphology (e.g., volume and thickness of gray matter) between regions of functionally or anatomically connected systems, such as the frontoparietal (Wright et al., 1999), frontotemporal (Lerch et al., 2006), visual system (Andrews et al., 1997), and symmetrically interhemispheric regions (Mechelli et al., 2005). The notion of morphological coordination has been widely used to study correlated evolution in mammalian brain structures (Barton and Harvey, 2000; Clark et al., 2001) or infer structural connectivity between brain regions in humans (Bullmore et al., 1998; McAlonan et al., 2005; Lerch et al., 2006; Bohbot et al., 2007). The advances in the brain morphological studies have provided a new avenue of research toward large-scale topological organization of the entire human cortex. In our previous study, using graph theoretical network analysis (GRETNA), we demonstrated that coordinated variations in the thickness of the human cerebral cortex at a macroscale (regional level) is neither regular nor random but “small world” in nature (He et al., 2007b).
The small world, characterized by a high degree of clustering and short path length linking individual network nodes (Watts and Strogatz, 1998), is an attractive model for the description of complex brain networks because it not only supports both specialized and integrated information processing (Sporns et al., 2004) but also minimizes wiring costs while maximizing the efficiency of information propagation (Kaiser and Hilgetag, 2006; Achard and Bullmore, 2007). Moreover, it provides a quantitative insight into relevant network parameters that have profound effects on the dynamic performances of a network such as the speed of information transfer and robustness against pathological attacks by disease (Bassett and Bullmore, 2006). Recently, using electroencephalogram (EEG) recording, Stam et al. (2007) have reported the loss of small-world characteristics in functional brain networks in AD, implicating disruptive system integrity associated with specific cognitive states caused by the disease. Yet no studies reported AD-related changes in coordinated patterns of large-scale structural brain networks.
Here, we hypothesize that AD patients would show alterations in the coordination of large-scale structural brain networks, using interregional correlation of cortical thickness as a particular metric of the structural basis underlying brain dynamics. We expected that the abnormalities in the patients with AD would be associated with alterations in cortical thickness correlations, small-world parameters, nodal centrality, and network robustness because all reflect the precise coordination of cortical morphology in the brain.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
One hundred and ninety-eight right-handed subjects were selected from the Open Access Series of Imaging Studies (OASIS) database (http://www.oasis-brains.org) (Marcus et al., 2007). Data from nine subjects (one healthy elder adult and eight AD subjects) were excluded from the analysis because of failure of imaging processing. The included subjects were classified into healthy elders (n = 97) and early-stage AD patients (n = 92) by using the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scale (Morris, 1993; Morris et al., 2001). Sixty-four of the 92 AD patients who had a CDR score of 0.5 were assigned to the very mild category, whereas the other 28 who had a CDR score of 1 were assigned to the mild category. The AD patients (female/male, 54:38) ranged in age from 62 to 96 (mean age, 76.65; SD, 7.13). The healthy controls (female/male, 71:26) ranged in age from 60 to 94 (mean age, 75.93; SD, 9.03). Cognitive function of all subjects was evaluated using the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975). The AD and control groups had an average MMSE score of 24.38 (range, 14–30) and 28.95 (range, 25–30), respectively. For the details of clinical and demographic data for all subjects, see Marcus et al. (2007).
Image acquisition
For each subject, three to four individual T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) images were acquired on a 1.5 T Vision scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) within a single session. Head movement was minimized by cushioning and a thermoplastic face mask. The images were motion corrected and averaged to create a single image with high contrast-to-noise. The MP-RAGE parameters were empirically optimized for gray–white contrast (repetition time, 9.7 ms; echo time, 4.0 ms; inversion time, 20 ms; delay time, 200 ms; flip angle, 10°; orientation, sagittal; resolution, 256 × 256 matrix; slices, 128; thickness, 1.25 mm).
Measurements of cortical thickness
Our methods to automatically measure cortical thickness have been described previously (MacDonald et al., 2000; Kim et al., 2005). Briefly, the native MRI were first registered into stereotaxic space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) using a linear transformation (Collins et al., 1994). Simultaneously, the images were corrected for nonuniformity artifacts using the N3 algorithms (Sled et al., 1998). The registered and corrected images were further segmented into gray matter, white matter, CSF, and background using an advanced neural net classifier (Zijdenbos et al., 2002). The inner and outer gray matter surfaces with a total of 81,920 polygons (40,962 vertices) each were then automatically extracted from each magnetic resonance (MR) volume using the constrained Laplacian-based automated segmentation with proximities (CLASP) algorithm (MacDonald et al., 2000; Kim et al., 2005). Cortical thickness was thus defined as the distance between linked vertices on the inner and outer surfaces (Lerch and Evans, 2005). Two representative cortical thickness maps are shown in Figure 1A. The CLASP algorithm has been validated using both manual measurements (Kabani et al., 2001) and simulation approaches (Lerch and Evans, 2005; Lee et al., 2006) and applied to recent AD studies (Lerch et al., 2005; Singh et al., 2006).
Construction of structural cortical networks
We developed a procedure to construct structural cortical networks based on cortical thickness measurements from MRI (He et al., 2007b). First, individual cortical thickness maps were parcellated using the automated nonlinear image matching and anatomical labeling (ANIMAL) package (Collins et al., 1995; Robbins et al., 2004). By registering each subject's MR images to a presegmented volumetric template using nonlinear deformations, the labels of brain regions were transformed to the cortical surface by assigning the value of the voxel label to each vertex on the surface. An average cortical parcellation with 27 regions for each hemisphere was generated by finding the anatomical label with the highest occurrence at each vertex (see Fig. 1B; supplemental Table 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Cortical thickness for each region was measured as the average thickness of all vertices defined as belonging to that region. The interregional correlation matrix Cij (i, j = 1, 2,… N, here N = 54) of each group (see Fig. 1C, left for the control group and right for the AD group) was then obtained by calculating the partial correlation coefficients across individuals between the cortical thicknesses of every pair of regions. The partial correlations between any two cortical regions represent their conditional dependences by partialling out the effects of the other 52 regions defined in the ANIMAL template (Horwitz et al., 1987; Salvador et al., 2005). Before the correlation analysis, a linear regression was performed at every cortical region to remove the effects of age and gender; the resulting residuals were used to substitute for the raw cortical thickness values. Finally, the partial correlation matrix of each group was thresholded into a binarized matrix Aij = [aij], where aij was 1 if the absolute value of the correlation Cij between regions i and j was larger than a given correlation threshold and 0 otherwise. These binary matrices capture the underlying topological organization of the human structural cortical networks. Notably, it is also possible to use the continuous weighting (e.g., true correlation values) between regions to study the network properties (Barrat et al., 2004; Jiang et al., 2004), but it would lead to complicated statistical descriptions in the following network analysis. The current study therefore confined itself to a simpler binary graph theoretical analysis.
Graph theoretical approaches
Threshold selection.
The network (graph) G was represented by a binarized matrix Aij with N nodes and K edges, where nodes and edges indicate cortical regions and undirected links corresponding to its nonzero elements, respectively. It was noted that, when the same correlation threshold was applied to the correlation matrices of the AD and control groups, the resulting graphs would comprise different number of edges because of the discrepancies in the low-level correlations (see Fig. 1C, left vs right). Thus, between-group difference in network parameters would not purely reflect the alterations in the topological organization. To control this effect, we thresholded the correlation matrix Cij of each group into a binarized matrix with a fixed sparsity S (see Fig. 1D, left for the control group and right for the AD group) defined as the total number of edges K in a graph divided by the maximum possible number of edges N(N − 1)/2. Setting a sparsity-specific threshold ensures that the graphs of both groups have the same number of edges or wiring cost (Achard and Bullmore, 2007; Stam et al., 2007). However, because there is currently no definitive way to select a single threshold, we therefore thresholded each correlation matrix repeatedly over a wide range of sparisty (6% ≤ S≤ 40%) and then estimated the properties of the resulting graphs at each threshold value. This enabled us to compare the small-world parameters between the two groups as a function of sparsity independently of the precise selection of threshold. The range of sparsity was chosen here to allow small-world network properties to be properly estimated and the number of spurious edges in each network minimized as indicated in previous studies (Achard and Bullmore, 2007; He et al., 2007b).
Small-world analysis.
Small-world measures of a network (clustering coefficient, Cp, and characteristic path length, Lp) were originally proposed by Watts and Strogatz (1998). Briefly, the Cp is the average of the clustering coefficients over all nodes in a network, where the clustering coefficient Ci of a node i is defined as the number of existing connections among the neighbors of the node divided by all their possible connections. Cp quantifies the extent of local cliquishness or local efficiency of information transfer of a network (Watts and Strogatz, 1998; Latora and Marchiori, 2001). The Lp of a network is the average minimum number of connections that link any two nodes of the network. However, this original definition of Lp is problematic in networks that comprise more than one component because there exist nodal pairs that have no connecting path. To avoid this problem, Lp was measured here by using a “harmonic mean” distance between pairs proposed by Newman (2003) (i.e., the reciprocal of the average of the reciprocals). Lp quantifies the ability of parallel information propagation or global efficiency (in terms of 1/Lp) of a network (Latora and Marchiori, 2001). A real network would be considered small world if it meets the following criteria: γ = Cpreal/Cprand ≻ 1 and λ = Lpreal/Lprand ∼ 1 (Watts and Strogatz, 1998), where the Cprand and Lprand are the mean clustering coefficient and characteristic path length of matched random networks that preserve the same number of nodes, edges, and degree (the degree ki of a node i is the number of connections to that node) distribution as the real network (Maslov and Sneppen, 2002; Sporns and Zwi, 2004).
Nodal centrality.
In this study, we also investigated nodal characteristics of the cortical networks. To address this issue, we considered the “betweenness centrality” of the nodes in the networks. The betweenness Bi of a node i is defined as the number of shortest paths between any two nodes that run through node i (Freeman, 1977). We measured the normalized betweenness as bi = Bi/〈B〉, where 〈B〉 was the average betweenness of the network. bi is a global centrality measure that captures the influence of a node over information flow between other nodes in the network and it was calculated here by using the MatlabBGL package (http://www.stanford.edu/∼dgleich/programs/matlab_bgl/). The hubs of the cortical networks are the regions with high values of bi. Group differences in bi reflect the effects of the disease on the global roles of regions in the cortical networks.
Network robustness.
Network robustness, characterized by the degree of tolerance against random failures and targeted attacks, is usually associated with the stability of a complex network. In the present study, we investigated the robustness (tolerance) of the cortical networks by the removals of nodes and edges (Albert et al., 2000; Kaiser and Hilgetag, 2004; Achard et al., 2006). To address the nodal failure tolerance, we first randomly removed one node from the networks and then measured the changes in the size of the largest connected component. After this, we continued selecting and removing additional nodes from the networks at random and recomputed this measure. This procedure was also applied to the edge failure tolerance assessment in which the links in the networks were randomly selected and removed. To evaluate the attack tolerance, we repeated the above processes but removed the nodes and links in decreasing order of their betweenness, respectively [the betweenness of an edge is the number of shortest paths between pairs of other nodes that pass through the edge (Girvan and Newman, 2002); links with high edge betweenness values can be regarded as the pivotal links of a network]. The simulation procedures were performed for the networks of both control and AD groups, and a comparison of the results was further investigated.
Statistical analysis
Correlation differences.
To test whether interregional correlation of cortical thickness was significantly different between the AD patients and controls, correlation coefficients obtained above were further converted into z values by using Fisher's r-to-z transform. This transformation generated values that were approximately normally distributed. A Z statistic was then used to compare these transformed z values to determine the significance of the between-group differences in correlations (Cohen and Cohen, 1983). To adjust for the multiple comparisons, a false discovery rate (FDR) procedure was performed at a q value of 0.05 (Genovese et al., 2002).
Topological parameters differences.
To determine statistical significance of the differences in network parameters between groups, a nonparametric permutation test method was applied (Bullmore et al., 1999). First, Cp, Lp, and bi of the networks at a given sparsity were computed separately for the AD and control groups. To test the null hypothesis that the observed group differences could occur by chance, we then randomly reallocated each subject's set of regional cortical thickness measures to one or the other of the two groups and recomputed the partial correlation matrix for each randomized group. We then obtained corresponding binarized matrix using the same sparsity threshold as in the real brain networks. Next, we calculated the network parameters for each randomized group and obtained their differences between the randomized groups. This randomization procedure was repeated 1000 times and the 95 percentile points of each distribution were used as the critical values for a one-tailed test of the null hypothesis with a probability of type I error of 0.05. The procedure was repeated at every sparsity threshold value of the brain networks. In the present study, we also assessed between-group differences in the areas under the Cp and Lp curves.
Results
Interregional correlations of cortical thickness
The interregional cortical thickness correlation matrices of the normal controls and AD groups are shown in Figure 1C. The correlation patterns of both groups were complex but had some common features such as strong interhemispheric intercorrelations between bilaterally homologous regions that were consistent with previous brain morphological studies using cortical thickness (He et al., 2007b) and gray matter density (Mechelli et al., 2005) in normal subjects. Statistical analysis further revealed significant between-group correlation differences (p < 0.05, FDR-corrected) in various pairs of cortical regions (Table 1). For instance, decreased interhemispheric positive correlations between the bilateral postcentral gyrus (PoCG) and between the bilateral superior parietal lobe (SPL) were observed in the AD patients compared with the controls. In addition, AD patients were also found to show increased positive correlations in various pairs of cortical regions involved in the lateral parietal, temporal, and frontal cortices [supramarginal gyrus (SMG), inferior temporal gyrus (ITG), superior temporal gyrus (STG), and middle frontal gyrus (MFG)] as well as the medial frontal gyrus (MdFG) and cingulate (CING) regions. Additionally, we also noted several abnormal negative correlations in the AD patients (Table 1).
A flowchart for the construction of structural cortical networks. A, Two representative cortical thickness maps (left for a control subject and right for an AD subject) were obtained from anatomical MRI by computational neuroanatomy. The color bar indicating the range of thickness is shown on the right. B, The entire cerebral cortex was segmented into 54 cortical areas that were displayed on the average cortex (left for the lateral surface and right for the medial surface), each color representing an individual region. C, The correlation matrices were obtained by calculating partial correlations between regional thickness across subjects within each group (left for the control group and right for the AD group). The color bar indicating the partial correlation coefficient between regions is shown on the top. D, The correlation matrices of C were thresholded into the binarized matrices (left for the control group and right for the AD group) by a sparsity threshold of 13%. Such a threshold ensures that the networks of both of the groups have the same number of nodes and links (i.e., the two networks have the same wiring cost). NC, Normal controls. For details, see Materials and Methods.
Abnormal cortical correlations in AD patients compared with controls
Small-world structural cortical networks
Small-world model
Previous studies have demonstrated the small-world topology in large-scale structural brain networks in humans (Hagmann et al., 2007; He et al., 2007b) and nonhuman primates (Hilgetag et al., 2000; Sporns and Zwi, 2004). In a small-world network, the shortest path length between any pair of nodes is approximately equivalent to a comparable random network, but the nodes of the network have greater local interconnectivity than a random network (Watts and Strogatz, 1998). In the current study, we also examined the small-world attributes of cortical networks in the normal elder subjects and AD patients. As expected, both networks (Fig. 2, left for the control group and right for the AD group) demonstrated small-world architectures as they had an almost identical path length (λ ≈ 1, black lines) but were more locally clustered (γ > 1, gray lines) over a wide range of sparsity (6% ≤ S ≤ 40%) in comparison with the matched random networks, consistent with the previous studies. Using computational modeling simulation approaches, Sporns et al. (2000) suggest the emergence of small-world topology when networks are evolved for high complexity of dynamic behavior defined as an optimal balance between local specialization and global integration. Our findings thus provide additional support for the hypothesis that the human brain has evolved into a complex but efficient neural architecture to maximize the power of information processing (Sporns et al., 2004; Kaiser and Hilgetag, 2006).
Small-world properties of structural cortical networks. The graphs show the changes in the γ (Cpreal/Cprand, gray lines) and λ (Lpreal/Lprand, black lines) in the structural networks of both the control (left panel) and AD (right panel) groups as a function of sparisty thresholds. At a wide range of sparsity, both networks have γ > 1 (i.e., the real networks show high clustering compared with 1000 rewiring random networks) and λ ∼ 1 (i.e., the real networks show approximately equivalent path length compared with 1000 rewiring random networks), which implies prominent small-world properties (see Materials and Methods). Note that, as the values of sparsity thresholds increase, the γ values decrease rapidly, but the λ values only change slightly. The black arrows point to a range of sparsity in which the small-world properties are estimable because the average degrees of networks are larger than log(N) (N is the number of node regions) (Watts and Strogatz 1998; Achard et al., 2006; He et al., 2007b). NC, Normal controls.
AD-related alterations
One of the key questions we posed was whether AD patients with cognitive impairment would be associated with alterations of small-world parameters in the cortical networks. As shown in Figure 3, both clustering coefficient, Cp (Fig. 3A, inset), and characteristic path length, Lp (Fig. 3B, inset), in the AD networks were larger than those of the controls over a wide range of sparsity. Statistical analysis further revealed significant differences (p < 0.05) in the Cp values at 12% < S < 15% and 27% < S < 40% (Fig. 3A, arrows) and Lp values at 12% < S < 14% and 22% < S < 34% (Fig. 3B, arrows). Moreover, AD patients demonstrated significantly larger area under the Cp (p = 0.03) (Fig. 4A) and Lp (p = 0.05) (Fig. 4B) curves. These results imply that the small-world parameters were significantly altered in the structural cortical networks of AD patients. Our finding of AD-related increases in paths was consistent with a recent brain functional network study in AD using EEG measurement (Stam et al. 2007). However, the previous study did not detect significant changes in clustering coefficients in AD patients. The discrepancies could be attributable to different network modality (functional vs structural), network size (21 vs 54), and population size (28 vs 189) applied in two studies. Nonetheless, all evidence points to that AD patients are probably related to the loss of small-world characteristics in the large-scale brain systems.
Between-group differences in clustering coefficient (Cp) and path length (Lp) as a function of sparsity. A, The graph shows the differences (red circles) in the Cp between the controls and AD patients as a function of sparsity thresholds. The gray lines represent the mean values (open circles) and 95% confidence intervals of the between-group differences obtained 1000 permutation tests at each sparsity value. The arrows indicate significant (p < 0.05) difference in Cp between the two groups. Note that AD patients (dotted lines) show larger Cp values in the brain networks than controls (solid lines) over a wide range of thresholds (inset). B, The graph shows the differences (red circles) in the Lp between the controls and AD patients as a function of sparsity thresholds. The gray lines represent the mean values (open circles) and 95% confidence intervals of the between-group differences obtained 1000 permutation tests at each sparsity value. The arrows indicate significant (p < 0.05) difference in Lp between the two groups. Note that AD patients (dotted lines) show larger Lp values in the brain networks than controls (solid lines) over a wide range of thresholds (inset). NC, Normal controls.
Between-group differences in areas under the clustering coefficient (CpAUC) and path length (LpAUC) curves. A, The graph shows the differences (black square) in the CpAUC between the controls and AD patients. The black lines represent the mean values (open circles) and 95% confidence intervals of the between-group differences obtained 1000 permutation tests. Note that the AD patients show larger CpAUC values in the brain networks compared with the controls (p = 0.03). B, The graph shows the differences (black square) in the LpAUC between the controls and AD patients. The black lines represent the mean values (open circles) and 95% confidence intervals of the between-group differences obtained 1000 permutation tests. Note that the AD patients show larger LpAUC values in the brain networks compared with the controls (p = 0.05). For the Cp and Lp curves of the controls and AD groups, see the insets of Figure 3. NC, Normal controls.
Regional nodal characteristics
To investigate the nodal characteristics, the cortical networks were constructed at a specific sparsity threshold of 13%. This threshold ensures that all regions are included in the cortical networks while minimizing the number of false-positive paths (Fig. 5). Such a constraint might optimize interregional correlation strengths and therefore be biologically plausible (Bassett et al., 2006).
The size of the largest connected component of structural cortical networks. The graph shows the largest component size of the networks in the control (gray line) and AD group (black line) as a function of sparsity threshold. As the threshold increases, the largest component sizes of both groups tend to increase. The arrow indicates that the lowest sparsity threshold (13%) in which both of the networks included all connected nodes (i.e., 54 regions) defined in the brain template.
Hub regions
To identify the hub regions, we examined normalized nodal betweenness centrality, bi, of each cortical region in both networks (see Materials and Methods). In the control group, 11 regions including 8 heteromodal or unimodal association cortex regions, 2 paralimbic cortex regions, and 1 primary motor cortex region were identified as the hubs because of large values in bi [bi > 1.5 (i.e., the betweenness value of a node is >1.5 times the average betweenness of the network)] (Table 2; supplemental Fig. 1A, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). In the AD group, 14 regions including 9 heteromodal or unimodal association cortex regions and 5 paralimbic cortex regions were identified as the hubs (Table 3; supplemental Fig. 1B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). These identified hubs were predominately located in regions of heteromodal and unimodal association cortex [SMG, STG, middle temporal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, and lingual gyrus (LG)] (Tables 2, 3) receiving convergent inputs from multiple other cortical regions (Mesulam, 1998), suggesting their pivotal roles in the human structural cortical networks. The findings are in accordance with several previous studies in which these recently evolved association cortex regions have been identified as critical nodes in both structural and functional brain networks in humans (Achard et al., 2006; He et al., 2007b) and nonhuman primates (Sporns and Zwi, 2004; Honey et al., 2007).
Regions showing high betweenness in cortical networks of normal subjects
Regions showing high betweenness in cortical networks of AD patients
AD-related alterations
We next examined AD-related changes in the betweenness centrality of cortical regions. Compared with the controls, the AD patients showed significant centrality decreases in the heteromodal regions of the lateral temporal and parietal cortex [STG and angular gyrus (ANG)] and increases in the unimodal association cortex [LG and lateral occipitotemporal gyrus (LOTG)] and paralimbic regions (CING) (Fig. 6A, Table 4). The involved regions were highlighted in the topographic maps of the networks for both the control (Fig. 6B, left) and AD (Fig. 6B, right) groups. Additionally, we also noted that the betweenness values of the bilateral cortical regions were highly correlated in the normal subjects (Pearson's r = 0.50, p = 0.007) but not in the AD patients (Pearson's r = 0.13, p = 0.52). Additional statistical analysis revealed a decreased trend in correlation coefficient of the patients (Z score = 1.47; p = 0.07) (supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). The result was consistent with previous neuroimaging studies showing increased structural and functional asymmetry in AD patients (Grady et al., 1986; Haxby et al., 1990). Together, our findings suggest that the roles of regions in managing information flows over the cortical networks were profoundly affected in AD patients.
AD-related changes in nodal betweenness centrality. A, The graph shows the differences (green square) in normalized betweenness bi for each region between the controls and AD patients. The gray circles and bar lines represent the mean values and 95% confidence intervals of the between-group differences obtained from 1000 permutation tests, respectively. Significant decreases in bi in the AD patients were found in the right ANG (a), left ANG (b), and right STG (c), and significant increases in the left LOTG (d), left LING (g), and right cingulate (h). B, Regions showing significant AD-related changes in bi were mapped to anatomical space in the controls (left) and AD (right) groups. Regions showing AD-related decreases are colored in cyan and regions showing AD-related increases are colored in red. The black lines represent the links of the networks. Note that these results were obtained from the brain networks with a sparsity of 13%. NC, Normal controls. For the abbreviations of regions, see supplemental Table 1 (available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).
Regions showing AD-related changes in the betweenness centrality
Reduced network robustness in AD patients
Figure 7 illustrates the network robustness of both the control and AD groups in response to the random failures and targeted attack. Although the structural cortical network of AD patients was approximately as robust to the random failures as that of controls, it was considerably more vulnerable to targeted attacks. We found that the size of the largest connected component was reduced more noticeably in the network of AD patients than in the controls when at least 10% of the most central nodes and links were attacked (Fig. 7, right). Furthermore, when 25% of the most central nodes were attacked in both networks, the size of the largest component reduced 40% in the network of AD patients (Fig. 7A, right arrow); whereas in contrast, the network of controls only reduced 25% (Fig. 7A, left arrow). Similarly, whereas 25% of the most central links were attacked, the size of the largest component reduced 20% in the network of AD patients (Fig. 7B, right arrow) but it remained nearly unchanged in the control group (Fig. 7B, left arrow). These results suggest that the structural cortical networks of AD patients exhibited topological vulnerability to targeted attack as a consequence of pathological organization.
Topological robustness in structural cortical networks. The graphs show the relative size of the largest connected component as a function of the fraction of removed nodes (A) and links (B) by random failures (left) or targeted attacks (right). The brain network in the AD patients (black line) was approximately as robust as that in the controls (gray line) in response to random failures. However, it displayed remarkably reduced stability against targeted attack compared with the control network. Note that both brain networks used here had a same sparsity value of 13%. NC, Normal controls.
Discussion
The present study, for the first time, demonstrates AD-related changes in the coordination of large-scale structural brain networks by using cortical thickness data from MRI. Our main findings are as follows: (1) that the observed data revealed AD-related abnormalities in morphological correlations among selective subsets of cortical regions, (2) that the global topological organization of cortical networks in AD patients were disrupted as indicated by altered small-world parameters, (3) that the regional characteristic (centrality) was profoundly affected in AD patients, and (4) that the structural cortical network of AD showed reduced topological stability while against targeted attack. Together, these findings strongly suggest that the widely distributed cortical networks are altered in AD patients, thus providing structural (morphological) evidence to support the concept of AD that the core aspects of the pathophysiology of this disease are associated with disruptive alterations in the coordination of large-scale brain networks that underlie high-level cognition.
Neurobiological basis of correlative variation in regional cortical thickness
The thickness of the cerebral cortex represents the size, density, and arrangement of cells (Parent and Carpenter, 1995). In this study, interregional correlations in cortical thickness were used to construct structural brain networks. Previous work from our laboratory has demonstrated that interregional coordinated variations in cortical thickness are approximately associated with the known neuroanatomical pathways in the human brain (Lerch et al., 2006; He et al., 2007b). Other morphological covariations (e.g., gray matter volume) were also reported in regions of functionally or anatomically connected systems (Andrews et al., 1997; Mechelli et al., 2005; Bohbot et al., 2007). Although the precise neurobiological mechanism behind them remains unclear, it was argued (Mechelli et al., 2005) that the morphological coordination in circumscribed regions may result from the mutually trophic effects (Ferrer et al., 1995) or environment-related plasticity (Maguire et al., 2000). Recent studies have also demonstrated disrupted coordination of the brain morphology in various neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia (Bullmore et al., 1998; Wright et al., 1999; Mitelman et al., 2005), autism (McAlonan et al., 2005), and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Pujol et al., 2004). All these processes are the likely driving forces behind alterations in correlative patterns of cortical thickness in AD.
Abnormal interregional correlations of cortical thickness in AD
We observed several abnormal interregional cortical thickness correlations in AD patients (Table 1). Decreased positive correlations were found between the bilateral parietal regions (PoCG and SPL). Studies in monkeys (Seltzer and Pandya, 1983) and humans (Huang et al., 2005) have demonstrated that the bilateral parietal regions are connected through the midbody (PoCG) and posterior body and splenium (SPL) of the corpus callosum (CC). Several electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies found that the AD patients had (1) impaired functional couplings between bilateral homologous regions (Horwitz et al., 1987; Wada et al., 1998), and (2) reduced size or integration of white matter in the midbody, posterior body, and splenium of the CC (Pantel et al., 1999; Sydykova et al., 2007). Thus, our findings are compatible with the previous studies of AD. The decreases could be attributed to lack of mutually trophic influences of anatomical connectivity (CC) between the regions.
Interestingly, we found that AD patient exhibited increased positive correlations among several “default” (Raichle et al., 2001) regions [e.g., medial frontal and parietal (MPFC and CING), and lateral parietal and temporal (SMG, ITG, and STG) regions]. It has been suggested that these regions may be directly connected. Several recent resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) studies have shown that these regions are temporally coherent in intrinsic or spontaneous brain activity in both humans (Greicius et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005) and monkeys (Vincent et al., 2007). Moreover, these “default” regions have also been found to be mainly involved in episodic memory process (Greicius et al., 2003) and show AD-related breakdown of brain activities, such as the amyloid deposition, metabolic and spontaneous activity disruption (Greicius et al., 2004; Buckner et al., 2005; Celone et al., 2006). In addition, these regions were also found to show focal thinning in AD patients (supplemental Fig. 3 and supplemental Table 4, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). One could thus speculate that increased correlations of cortical thickness among the “default” regions may reflect a correlative cortical shrinking because of shared vulnerability to an insult, as preferentially affected by the pathological process of this disease.
We also noted abnormal negative correlations in the AD patients. Although the phenomenon of negative morphological correlations has been observed in normal subjects (Mechelli et al., 2005; Bohbot et al., 2007), and patients with schizophrenia (Mitelman et al., 2005) and autism (McAlonan et al., 2005), it remains unclear as to what they precisely reflect. One hypothesis is that the abnormalities are related to weakened or strengthened interregional inhibitory trophic relationship because of a direct influence between the regions or an indirect modulation from a third party, leading to a reorganization of brain systems (McAlonan et al., 2005; Mitelman et al., 2005).
Altered small-world configurations in structural cortical networks in AD
We also found significant small-world alterations (i.e., increased path length and clustering coefficient in the structural networks of AD patients). Short paths in brain networks assure effective integrity or rapid transfers of information between and across remotely regions that are believed to constitute the basis of cognitive processes (Sporns and Zwi, 2004). The AD-related increases in paths might thus reflect disrupted neuronal integrations among distant regions that were associated with impaired cognitive functions (measured with MMSE scores). In this study, AD patients were also found to show increased clustering in the cortical networks, suggesting a stronger local specialization. Given that the small-world model reflects an optimal balance between local specialization and global integration as described previously, the longer paths combined with higher clustering in the AD networks indicate a disturbance of the normal balance and make their networks more in favor of a regular configuration. It has been suggested that regular lattices have reduced signal propagation speed and synchronizability compared with small-world networks (Strogatz, 2001). Therefore, the AD-related changes in small-world parameters reported here may reflect a less optimal topological organization, thus providing implications for the understanding of the relationship between network topology and neuropathological state of disease.
Altered nodal centrality in structural cortical networks in AD
The AD networks were also found to show significantly decreased nodal centrality in several heteromodal association cortex regions (e.g., STG and ANG). Evidence from monkeys has demonstrated that STG and ANG have wide anatomical connections to the temporolimbic and neocortical association areas located in the prefrontal and parietal cortices (Seltzer and Pandya, 1994; Pandya, 1995) and the anterior temporal and prefrontal regions (Mesulam, 1998), respectively. Functional neuroimaging studies in humans have also suggested that these regions tend to have long-distance connections (Achard et al., 2006) and are preferentially affected at the onset of AD before sensory or motor regions (for review, see Delbeuck et al., 2003). Gomez-Isla et al. (1997) reported AD-related neuronal loss in the STG that paralleled the chronological evolution of dementia and correlated with the neurofibrillary tangles formation. Other researchers also observed AD-related reduction of functional connectivity in the regions (Bokde et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2006). Thus, we speculated that these regional abnormalities may cause a segregation of different brain systems and yield a disruptive integration of large-scale brain networks. However, we also observed AD-related increases in nodal centrality in several unimodal association cortex regions (e.g., LG and LOTG). Previous studies have suggested that these regions usually retain their functional capacity in the early stages of AD (Grady et al., 1988; Mentis et al., 1996). There is also additional evidence for AD-related increases in activation (Backman et al., 2000; Prvulovic et al., 2002; He et al., 2007a) and functional connectivity (Horwitz et al., 1995; Grady et al., 2003) in these regions, which has been characterized as compensatory recruitment of cognitive resources to maintain task performance. One could therefore speculate those increased regional centrality in AD might represent a compensatory process for the reduced centrality in other regions described previously. Overall, our results demonstrated AD-related changes in the nodal ability to manage information flow of cortical networks.
Topological vulnerability in structural cortical networks in AD
Recent studies have demonstrated that small-world brain networks with embedded hubs exhibit surprising resilience to random failures and targeted attacks (Kaiser and Hilgetag, 2004; Achard et al., 2006). Assuming that dynamic behavior of a network is strongly associated with its fundamental topological organization, it seems reasonable to suppose that the alterations in network parameters would reflect the disruptions in the general performance of the network such as stability and robustness. This hypothesis was supported by our findings that the AD networks were extremely vulnerable to targeted attacks on its pivotal nodes and links compared with controls. The reduced topological stability might be attributed to pathological cortical organization in AD such as the aberrant cortical couplings, small-world architecture, and nodal centrality shown previously.
Relationships between structural and functional brain networks
A question of interest is whether the current findings of AD-related alterations in structural cortical networks can be related to functional deficits in patients. In the mammalian neocortex, Honey et al. (2007) have found that the spontaneous neuronal dynamics can be structured at multiple temporal scales, suggesting a tight association between structural and functional networks. In the human brain, we recently demonstrated that structural cortical networks (He et al., 2007b) share many common topological features (e.g., small-world topology and hub nodes) with spontaneous low-frequency (<0.08 Hz) functional networks derived from fMRI data (Achard et al., 2006). One could therefore speculate that the AD-related alterations in structural networks shown here are likely to underlie functional impairments associated with this disease. Together, the present study strongly suggests that coordinated changes in structural features (cortical thickness) and their internal topologies are biologically meaningful, thus potentially opening up a new window into our understanding of cortical organization in AD.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by Human Brain Project Grant PO1MHO52176-11 (International Consortium for Brain Mapping; principal investigator, Dr. John Mazziotta), Canadian Institutes of Health Research Grant MOP-34996, and the Killam Foundation. Y.H. was supported by a Jeanne Timmins Costello Postdoctoral Fellowship of the Montreal Neurological Institute. We are grateful to Drs. Randy Buckner and Daniel S. Marcus for providing the Open Access Series of Imaging Studies dataset (http://www.oasis-brains.org) for this project.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Alan Evans, McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurological Institute, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 2B4. alan.evans{at}mcgill.ca