Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

Strategic Control of Attention to Objects and Locations

Adam S. Greenberg and Leon Gmeindl
Journal of Neuroscience 16 January 2008, 28 (3) 564-565; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4386-07.2008
Adam S. Greenberg
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Leon Gmeindl
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Humans have a highly developed expertise to selectively process sensory input that is relevant to their goals, an ability known as attention. Visual attention to spatial locations modulates the activity of retinotopically organized neurons within visual cortex. However, attention can also be allocated selectively to relevant objects, even when they are superimposed with irrelevant objects. Object-based attention yields a behavioral and neural advantage when discriminating between parts of the same object versus parts of different objects. This same-object advantage is found even when distances between relevant parts are equated. Because locations and objects are more or less relevant depending on one's current goals, it is important to examine how and when processing resources are allocated to these different types of information. In their recent The Journal of Neuroscience paper, Martinez et al. (2007) provide a window into the temporal profiles and the functional neuroanatomical correlates of space-based and object-based attention. They measured event-related potentials (ERPs), two components of which index attention: the P1 component (a positive deflection 80–130 ms after stimulus presentation) and the N1 component (a negative deflection 150–200 ms after stimulus presentation).

Subjects fixated the center of a display containing either a whole square [intact-object (IO) condition] or a square fragmented into four rectangles [fragmented-object (FO) condition] [Martinez et al. (2007), their Fig. 1 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/27/30/7963/F1)]. A central arrow cued one of the four outermost corners of the display throughout each 20 s block. A target (convex) or nontarget (concave) corner offset appeared for 100 ms at a randomly chosen outermost corner every 400–600 ms, with a target/nontarget ratio of 1:4. Importantly, subjects were instructed to respond only to convex offsets that appeared at the cued location.

Subjects correctly discriminated >90% of cued targets, suggesting that they successfully attended to the cued location when stimuli appeared there. Additionally, neither accuracy nor reaction time differed by object configuration (presumably because subjects were instructed not to respond to uncued stimuli). A small false alarm rate did not vary by condition (A. Martinez, personal communication).

Martinez et al. (2007) predicted a same-object neural advantage such that uncued stimuli would evoke larger neural responses in the IO condition than in the FO condition, because only in the IO condition did uncued stimuli occur on an object having a cued corner. This prediction was borne out in the N1 component: cued, uncued IO, and uncued FO stimuli evoked the largest, medium, and smallest amplitudes, respectively. The amplitude difference between IO and FO conditions indicates that, even when it is irrelevant, object information can guide the allocation of processing resources. Meanwhile, the amplitude difference between cued and uncued stimuli reflects a concurrent space-based attentional modulation. However, the earlier P1 component indicated a purely space-based modulation: cued stimuli evoked the largest amplitude, but there was no effect of object configuration [Martinez et al. (2007), their Fig. 3 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/27/30/7963/F3)]. Accordingly, Martinez et al. (2007) concluded that “spatially mediated selection began earlier than the object-selective effects.”

N1-component source analysis localized a combined object-based and space-based attentional modulation of neural activity to the lateral occipital complex (LOC) contralateral to the offset-stimulus hemifield. In a separate fMRI experiment, the authors found that a region near LOC was the only area reflecting both space-based and object-based attentional modulation in their paradigm [Martinez et al. (2007), their Fig. 5 (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/27/30/7963/F5)]. These are intriguing findings because LOC is thought to be one of the first points in the ventral visual processing stream where object representations are formed; thus, it is a prime candidate for the confluence of space-based and object-based selection mechanisms. However, as mentioned above, the ERP analysis indicated that space-based attentional modulation (which might have been driven by a source outside of LOC) began before object-based modulation in LOC.

The difference between ERP amplitudes to cued and uncued stimuli observed in both the P1 and N1 components reflects a space-based allocation of attention. Unlike most object-based attention paradigms, however, subjects were instructed not to respond to stimuli appearing at uncued locations. As a result, subjects should have been especially motivated to ignore stimuli appearing at any uncued location and, thus, to focus attention very narrowly at the cued location. Nevertheless, the difference in ERP amplitude between IO and FO conditions reflects a persistent same-object neural advantage.

The prevailing view of object-based selection (Vecera and Farah, 1994) is that when attention is directed to a location on an object, all points on that object gain attentional priority. However, the data of Martinez et al. (2007) are also consistent with an alternative hypothesis that subjects have flexible, albeit limited, control over the degree to which object information in the visual environment is permitted to guide attention. For instance, one may be able to bias neural resources toward the processing of locations over objects. In the task used by Martinez et al. (2007), because only one location was relevant in each trial block, subjects may have suppressed object processing while attending to the cued location. This account is consistent with the proposal that P1 indexes visual suppression (Luck, 1995) and would explain both the space-based attentional modulation and the lack of object-based attentional modulation in LOC immediately after stimulus presentation.

However, a failure to prevent the influence of objects on attentional allocation could arise from salient changes in object information (e.g., when a new object appears) and/or from momentary loss of attentional control [e.g., when attention is captured by a sudden onset (Yantis and Hillstrom, 1994)]. In Martinez et al.'s (2007) study, attention may have been captured when a stimulus appeared at an uncued location because the subject anticipated a sudden-offset target (possibly consistent with the observation of false alarms). This momentary loss of attentional control could have provided an opportunity for object processing to subsequently receive neural resources that were previously withheld. It is also possible that because the particular target and nontarget stimuli used in this study changed the shape of the object on which they appeared, a new object representation might have been formed after corner offset. This object representation could have modulated the top-down, space-based attentional bias toward the cued location. Moreover, the same-object neural advantage may have arisen during the N1 time period because the newly formed object representation encompassed the cued location in the IO condition only. Conversely, in the FO condition, the newly formed object representation did not overlap the cued location, resulting in an attenuation of the N1 component. Thus, the combined effects of voluntary space-based attention and failed suppression of object processing might underlie the N1 modulations observed in LOC.

The particular hypothesis outlined above is predicated on our general notion of voluntary control over the influence of object representations on visual attention. The delayed object-based attentional modulation of LOC activity in the face of persistent objects suggests that humans can filter out objects, or isolate object representations from space-based attentional selection processes, when those objects are not relevant to current goals. Consistent with this possibility, recent studies have shown that 100% valid cues indicating the location of a target appearing within an object eliminate the same-object behavioral advantage (Shomstein and Yantis, 2002; Greenberg et al., 2006). Future studies will contribute to a better understanding of how attention is flexibly allocated to objects and locations as a function of one's goals, motivation, and environment.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short reviews of a recent paper in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to mimic the journal clubs that exist in your own departments or institutions. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to either Adam S. Greenberg or Leon Gmeindl at the above address. agreenb{at}jhu.edu or gmeindl{at}jhu.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Greenberg AS,
    2. Serences JT,
    3. Yantis S
    (2006) Object-based attention does not automatically spread throughout an object [abstract] J Vis 6:594.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Luck SJ
    (1995) Multiple mechanisms of visual-spatial attention: recent evidence from human electrophysiology. Behav Brain Res 71:113–123.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Martinez A,
    2. Ramanathan DS,
    3. Foxe JJ,
    4. Javitt DC,
    5. Hillyard SA
    (2007) The role of spatial attention in the selection of real and illusory objects. J Neurosci 27:7963–7973.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Shomstein S,
    2. Yantis S
    (2002) Object-based attention: sensory modulation or priority setting? Percept Psychophys 64:41–51.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Vecera SP,
    2. Farah MJ
    (1994) Does visual attention select objects or locations? J Exp Psychol Gen 123:146–160.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Yantis S,
    2. Hillstrom AP
    (1994) Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20:95–107.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 28 (3)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 28, Issue 3
16 Jan 2008
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Strategic Control of Attention to Objects and Locations
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Strategic Control of Attention to Objects and Locations
Adam S. Greenberg, Leon Gmeindl
Journal of Neuroscience 16 January 2008, 28 (3) 564-565; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4386-07.2008

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Strategic Control of Attention to Objects and Locations
Adam S. Greenberg, Leon Gmeindl
Journal of Neuroscience 16 January 2008, 28 (3) 564-565; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4386-07.2008
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Objects from Motion: Moving beyond Static Images with Object Kinematograms
  • Toward an Interpersonal Neuroscience in Technologically Assisted (Virtual) Interactions
  • Complex Interactions between Distinct Theta Oscillatory Patterns during Sleep Deprivation
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.