Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

The Native Coordinate System of Spatial Attention Is Retinotopic

Julie D. Golomb, Marvin M. Chun and James A. Mazer
Journal of Neuroscience 15 October 2008, 28 (42) 10654-10662; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2525-08.2008
Julie D. Golomb
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Marvin M. Chun
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
James A. Mazer
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site

Figure 1.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Tasks and conditions, experiment 1. A, Example trial for saccade task. While subjects maintained fixation on the white fixation dot, a memory cue appeared briefly at another location. Subjects were instructed to hold this cued location in memory throughout the trial. The fixation dot then moved, and after completion of a saccade to the new fixation location, a probe stimulus (oriented bar) appeared after a variable delay in either the spatiotopic (top), retinotopic (middle), or control (bottom) location of the cue. Subjects made a button press response to indicate probe orientation. A memory test stimulus then appeared, and subjects indicated whether it occupied the same spatiotopic location as the memory cue. B, In the no-saccade task, subjects remained fixated on the original location for an equivalent amount of time, so that probe delays relative to the cue were consistent across tasks. Probes appeared in the spatiotopic/retinotopic (top) or control (bottom) locations. C, In the return-saccade task, subjects held fixation at the second location for 750 ms before making a return saccade back to the first fixation location. Probes appeared at the spatiotopic/retinotopic (top), updated retinotopic (middle; reported in supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), or control (bottom) locations; probe delays were the same as in the saccade task relative to onset of the final fixation. Gray arrows indicating saccades did not actually appear on screen. Stimulus configurations illustrated here represent only one of 108 possible cue–saccade configurations for each task (for additional examples, see supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).

  View article

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 45 (25)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 45, Issue 25
18 Jun 2025
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Figure 1.
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Figure 1.
Journal of Neuroscience Dec 1969,

Citation Manager Formats

Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Figure 1.
Journal of Neuroscience Dec 1969,
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Related Articles

Cited By...

  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.