Abstract
Neurons within the spinal cord can support several forms of plasticity, including response–outcome (instrumental) learning. After a complete spinal transection, experimental subjects are capable of learning to hold the hindlimb in a flexed position (response) if shock (outcome) is delivered to the tibialis anterior muscle when the limb is extended. This response-contingent shock produces a robust learning that is mediated by ionotropic glutamate receptors (iGluRs). Exposure to nociceptive stimuli that are independent of limb position (e.g., uncontrollable shock; peripheral inflammation) produces a long-term (>24 h) inhibition of spinal learning. This inhibition of plasticity in spinal learning is itself a form of plasticity that requires iGluR activation and protein synthesis. Plasticity of plasticity (metaplasticity) in the CNS has been linked to group I metabotropic glutamate receptors (subtypes mGluR1 and mGluR5) and activation of protein kinase C (PKC). The present study explores the role of mGluRs and PKC in the metaplastic inhibition of spinal cord learning using a combination of behavioral, pharmacological, and biochemical techniques. Activation of group I mGluRs was found to be both necessary and sufficient for metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning. PKC was activated by stimuli that inhibit spinal learning, and inhibiting PKC activity restored the capacity for spinal learning. Finally, a PKC inhibitor blocked the metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning produced by a group I mGluR agonist. The data strongly suggest that group I mGluRs control metaplasticity of spinal learning through a PKC-dependent mechanism, providing a potential therapeutic target for promoting use-dependent plasticity after spinal cord injury.
Introduction
Long-term plasticity in the CNS manifests at multiple levels, from molecular changes at individual synapses to alterations in learning and behavior. The ionotropic glutamate receptors (iGluRs) AMPA receptor (AMPAR) and NMDA receptor (NMDAR) play an essential role in plasticity. Signaling through these receptors can produce lasting changes in synaptic efficacy resulting in long-term potentiation (LTP) or long-term depression (LTD) of postsynaptic potentials (Malinow and Malenka, 2002). The threshold for inducing synaptic plasticity is, itself, subject to modulation. Certain forms of stimulation can shift the threshold for plasticity so that stimuli that would normally induce LTP come to induce LTD, or have no effect. This plasticity of plasticity or “metaplasticity” (Abraham and Bear, 1996; Abraham and Tate, 1997) is associated with trafficking of ionotropic glutamate receptors (Hellier et al., 2007). iGluR function can be affected by group I metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluR1 and mGluR5) and activation of downstream protein kinase C (PKC). Direct pharmacological manipulation of these signaling systems affects metaplasticity within the CNS.
Most studies of CNS plasticity and metaplasticity have focused on the hippocampus. However, these phenomena are observed throughout the CNS, including the spinal cord (Parker and Grillner, 1999; Bevan and Parker, 2004; Grau et al., 2006). Comparatively little is known about the mechanisms of spinal metaplasticity, despite evidence that recovery of function after spinal cord injury requires concurrent augmentation of adaptive plasticity (e.g., reacquisition of stepping) and diminution of maladaptive plasticity (e.g., plasticity within pain pathways). Here, we explore the pharmacological and biochemical mechanisms of spinal metaplasticity using a simple spinal learning paradigm as the primary behavioral measure (Grau et al., 1998). The task requires that spinally transected rats learn to maintain the hindlimb in a flexed position to terminate shock delivered to the same leg [response-contingent shock (Buerger and Fennessy, 1970)]. With this simple preparation, it can be shown that stimulation alters the capacity for spinal learning in a bidirectional manner that is reminiscent of hippocampal metaplasticity (Grau et al., 2006). Training with response-contingent shock on one limb facilitates learning on the contralateral limb (Crown and Grau, 2001). Conversely, if nociceptive stimulation is delivered in a manner that is independent of leg position (e.g., uncontrollable shock or peripheral inflammation), the spinal cord demonstrates a lasting inhibition of learning (Grau et al., 1998; Ferguson et al., 2006; Hook and Grau, 2007; Hook et al., 2008). Elsewhere, we found that this bidirectional spinal plasticity correlates with spinal levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Gómez-Pinilla et al., 2007), interacts with locomotor plasticity (Bigbee et al., 2007), and affects recovery of function after spinal cord injury (Grau et al., 2004; Hook et al., 2007).
The present study tests whether group I mGluRs and downstream activation of PKC regulate the capacity for spinal learning. Using a combination of behavioral, pharmacological, and biochemical methods, we found that mGluR1, mGluR5, and PKC are necessary for metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning. Group I mGluR activation was sufficient to generate a long-term inhibition of spinal learning and did so through a PKC-dependent mechanism.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
Male Sprague Dawley rats obtained from Harlan served as subjects. The rats were 100–120 d of age and weighed between 400 and 460 g. Subjects were individually housed, maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle, and given ad libitum access to food and water. All experiments were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health (NIH) standards for the care and use of laboratory animals (NIH publication no. 80-23) and were approved by the University Laboratory Animal Care Committee at Texas A&M University.
Surgery and intrathecal cannulization.
Surgery and intrathecal cannulization were performed using procedures described in detail previously (Grau et al., 1998; Ferguson et al., 2003, 2006). Briefly, subjects were pretreated with an intraperitoneal injection of warm 0.9% saline (2.5 ml) and atropine (40 mg/kg) followed by anesthesia with pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, i.p.). Subjects' backs were shaved, cleaned with iodine, and a longitudinal incision was made extending from about cervical vertebra 6 to thoracic vertebra 4 (T4). The tissue in front of T2 was cleared away, and the spinal cord was transected using a cautery. The void produced by the transection was filled with Oxycel (Parke-Davis) and a segment of polyethylene tubing (25 cm; PE-10) fitted with 0.23 mm (diameter) stainless-steel wire (SWGX-090; Small Parts) was inserted 9 cm caudally into the subarachnoid space between the dura and the spinal cord. The exposed end of tubing was secured to the adjacent tissue using cyanoacrylate. The guide wire was then gently pulled from the tubing and the wound was closed using Michel clips (Fine Science Tools).
After spinalization, subjects were hydrated with an intraperitoneal injection of 2.5 ml of warm 0.9% saline and placed in temperature-controlled environment (∼25.5°C). To prevent injury to the hindlimbs during recovery, the rear legs of spinalized animals were maintained in a normal flexed position by two pieces of porous tape (Orthaletic; width, 1.3 cm) gently wrapped once around the rat's body. Experiments were begun 24 h after transection. Previous work has indicated that comparable spinal learning is observed in acute and chronic spinal preparations, suggesting that spinal shock only has a minimal effect on this form of spinal learning (Grau et al., 1998; Bigbee et al., 2007).
Spinal transections were confirmed by (1) inspecting the cord during the operation to ensure that no spared fibers bridged the transection site and that the rostral and caudal stumps of the cord completely retracted, (2) observing the behavior of the subjects after recovery to ensure that they exhibited paralysis below the level of the forepaws and did not vocalize to leg shock, and (3) examining the spinal cord postmortem in a randomly selected subset of the subjects.
Shock-induced inhibition of spinal learning.
The procedures used to generate a deficit in instrumental learning/adaptive plasticity in the spinal cord have been described in detail previously (Grau et al., 1998; Crown and Grau, 2001; Crown et al., 2002a; Ferguson et al., 2003, 2006; Joynes et al., 2003; Joynes and Grau, 2004; Patton et al., 2004). Briefly, a shock electrode constructed from a modified fuse clip was coated with electrode paste and attached to the tail with a length of Orthaletic tape. Leads from the fuse clip were attached to a BRS/LVE shock generator (model SG-903) and 6 min of intermittent uncontrollable shock (80 ms duration; 1.5 mA AC; mean interstimulus interval of 2 s) was delivered. This procedure has been found to generate reliable metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning that lasts at least 48 h (Crown et al., 2002b). This metaplastic inhibition occurs on both legs and is observed if uncontrollable shock is applied to the hindlimb or the tail.
Spinal cord learning procedures.
Instrumental testing with controllable leg shock in spinalized animals was conducted using an apparatus similar to that used in previous studies (Grau et al., 1998). Briefly, rats were loosely restrained in Plexiglas tubes [20.0 cm (length) × 7.0 cm (internal diameter)]. Two slots [6.0 cm (length) × 1.7 cm (width)] were cut in the sides and base of the tube, allowing both hindlegs to hang freely. Shock was delivered using a BRS/LVE shock generator (model SG-903). Leg shock was applied by attaching one lead from the shock generator to a wire inserted through the skin over the tibia 1.5 cm above the ankle. The other lead was attached to a 2.5 cm stainless-steel pin that was inserted 0.4 cm into the tibialis anterior muscles 1.7 cm above the other electrode. Leg position was monitored using a contact electrode constructed from a 7.0 cm, 0.018“ stainless-steel rod that was taped to the plantar surface of the subject's foot using cloth tape (Orthaletic; 1.3 cm; Johnson & Johnson). A heat shrink covering (2.5 cm) covered the end of the rod and electrically insulated this circuit from the rat. A fine wire [0.01 mm2 (36 AWG)] was attached to the end of the rod, extending from the rear of the foot to a digital input monitored by a Macintosh computer. A plastic rectangular dish [11.5 (w) × 19 (l) × 5 (d)] containing a NaCl solution was placed ∼7.5 cm below the restraining tube, submerging the tip of the contact electrode. A drop of soap was added to the solution to reduce surface tension. A ground wire was connected to a 1 mm stainless-steel rod that was placed in the solution. When the contact electrode attached to the rat's paw contacted the solution, it completed a circuit monitored by the computer. The state of this circuit was sampled at 30 Hz.
Flexion force was measured before instrumental testing using a strain gauge connected to the foot by a monofilament plastic line (“4 lb test” Stren; DuPont). The strain gauge had previously been calibrated by determining the relationship between voltage and force in newtons. The data revealed a linear relationship that allowed us to convert voltage to force. Shock intensity was adjusted to produce a flexion force of a fixed value.
Behavioral measures.
The spinal learning apparatus provides three outputs: time in solution, flexion number, and flexion duration. The computer recorded the amount of time that the contact electrode was in contact with the solution (time in solution). Whenever the electrode left the solution, the number of flexion responses was increased by 1 (flexion number). To observe learning across trials, the training session was divided into 30, 1 min training bins. From time in solution and flexion number, we derived flexion duration using the following equation: flexion durationi = (60 s − time in solutioni)/(flexion numberi + 1), where i was the current training bin. Previous work has demonstrated that flexion duration is uniquely sensitive to response–outcome (instrumental) learning (Grau et al., 1998), whereas flexion number and time in solution can change in the absence of instrumental learning (Church et al., 1976). For this reason, flexion duration was used as the primary assay of spinal plasticity. Flexion number was evaluated as a measure of motor performance and is displayed in the supplemental material (available at www.jneurosci.org).
Drug experiments.
Intrathecal drugs were delivered using a 10 μl Hamilton syringe that was attached to the exposed end of the intrathecal cannulas. Controlled delivery of drug over several minutes was achieved using a syringe pump (model 11; Harvard Apparatus). For the group I mGluR experiments, drugs were purchased from Tocris, and subjects were given one of four doses of drug dissolved in 3 μl of vehicle. The vehicle was 0.9% saline for the mGluR5 antagonist 2-methyl-6-(phenylethylnyl)pyridine (MPEP) and the general group I mGluR agonist 3,5-dihydroxyphenylglycine (DHPG). The mGluR1 antagonist 7-(hydroxy-imino)cycloproa[b]chromen-1a-carboxylate ethyl ester (CPCCOEt) was dissolved in 100% dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO). The PKC inhibitor bisindolylmaleimide (BIM) (Sigma-Aldrich) was delivered in 10 μl of 100% DMSO and the PKC inhibitor chelerythrine (Calbiochem) was delivered in 10% DMSO in 0.9% saline. Because DMSO has been shown to have electrophysiological effects and effects on NMDA-dependent activation of the spinal locomotor networks (Tsvyetlynska et al., 2005), we were concerned that the DMSO vehicle per se could impact the test results. To examine whether vehicle treatment impacted the results, we merged the vehicle data from all experiments. This resulted in a large dataset containing 114 subjects. Even with this relatively high sample size, ANOVA failed to find any significant effect of DMSO versus 0.9% saline (p > 0.05). Analysis of effect size and power calculations revealed that DMSO does not have an effect that can be resolved using reasonable sample sizes for animal research (η2 = 0.007; 1 − β = 0.77). Together, these results suggest that, for all intents and purposes, DMSO does not affect spinal learning in this preparation.
Protein kinase C activity assay.
PKC activity was measured in tissue homogenate containing spinal segments L4–S2 from shocked and unshocked rats (N = 24). Previous work has isolated spinal learning to these segments (Liu et al., 2005). At 10 min, 1 h, and 24 h after the shock stimulus, tissue was flash frozen in liquid nitrogen and homogenized in ice-cold lysis buffer consisting of 50 mm Tris-HCl, pH 7.5, 150 mm NaCl, 1% NP-40, 0.5% sodium deoxycholate, supplemented with a protease inhibitor mixture (Sigma-Aldrich; P8340; 1:100) and sodium orthovanadate (Na3VO), a phosphatase inhibitor. Lysates were incubated on ice for 30 min and cleared by centrifugation at 30,000 × g, 4°C for 30 min. The protein concentration in cord lysates was determined using the micro-BCA protein assay (Pierce). Kinase activity was measured using the StressXpress Nonradioactive PKC Kinase Assay kit (no. EKS-420; Assay Designs). Given the sensitivity of the assay and the small amount of total tissue collected, our initial analyses examined activity in the total protein fraction. Briefly, 2 μg of cord lysate was mixed with dilution buffer and incubated on a microtiter plate for 1 h at room temperature in the presence of ATP. A proprietary PKC substrate (Assay Designs) was precoated on the wells of the microtiter plate. PKC standards were diluted and incubated in the same manner to generate a standard curve for quantification of activity. Next, the microtiter plate was incubated with a phosphospecific IgG antibody to label all the phosphorylated sites resulting from active PKC in the samples. Plates were incubated with anti-rabbit IgG: HRP-conjugated antibody, and colorimetric reactions were initiated by adding ABTS substrate solution. Colorimetric reactions were terminated after 6 min, and absorbance was read on an ELISA microplate reader at 415 nm (BioTek ELX808). Fresh lysis buffer (without cord lysate) was included in the assay as a blank, and cord lysate without ATP served as the negative control. All samples were assayed in triplicate and the ELISA was performed two times. Analyses of the standard concentrations confirmed that optical density was linearly related to concentration (r = 0.96; p < 0.001) within the range of interest. All analyses were conducted on the mean concentration obtained for each subject averaged across the two assays (n = 4 subjects/group).
Statistics.
The experimental designs were conceived a priori and analyzed as balanced, factorial mixed designs using SPSS 16 GLM repeated measures (for additional details, see supplemental Table 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). ANOVA and trend analysis (by polynomial contrast) were used to determine whether the impact of experimental treatments was better represented by linear relationships or higher polynomials when there was a continuous independent variable (e.g., dose or time). ANCOVA was used as a control in rare cases in which ANOVA detected differences on baseline response performance. Group differences were evaluated using one-way ANOVA and Duncan's post hoc tests when appropriate. The reported sample sizes (n) reflect the number of subjects per group for every factorial combination of the between-subjects factors. Sample sizes for each experiment were determined by power analysis on pilot data. In all analyses, significance was evaluated at p < 0.05 and power at 1 − β ≥ 0.8. In all graphical representation, the group means were derived by averaging over time and then averaging across groups. Error bars reflect the between-groups SEM.
Results
All results are reported using flexion duration as the outcome. Previous work has demonstrated that this measure is highly sensitive to spinal cord learning and metaplastic inhibition (Grau et al., 1998, 2006). Here, as in previous studies, a failure to learn was accompanied by an increase in response number (Grau et al., 2006). This is important because it suggests that the failure to learn does not reflect a performance deficit. Because some readers may wish to examine the relationship between flexion duration and performance, the response number data have been made available in the supplemental material (available at www.jneurosci.org).
Group I mGluRs are necessary for metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning
The two group I mGluRs subtypes, mGluR1 and mGluR5, have similar roles in hippocampal plasticity (Gubellini et al., 2003); however, there are reports of divergent effects on some forms of spinal plasticity (El Manira et al., 2002; Mills et al., 2002; Kettunen et al., 2003). Given these observations, it is difficult to predict whether mGluR1 and mGluR5 would have the same or different roles in metaplasticity of spinal learning. We addressed this issue by independently testing the impact of both a mGluR1 and mGluR5 antagonist. For both experiments, drug was delivered intrathecally before metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning with uncontrollable shock. All subjects were tested 24 h later, a time point that is associated with a robust spinal learning deficit under these stimulus conditions (Crown et al., 2002b).
Impact of a mGluR1 antagonist
To test whether mGluR1 is necessary for inhibiting spinal learning, we used the noncompetitive antagonist CPCCOEt at doses that are known to affect spinal nociceptive plasticity (Neugebauer et al., 1999). Immediately after drug administration, subjects were given 6 min of uncontrollable shock (0 or 1.5 mA) to the tail in a balanced, factorial design (n = 6), and spinal learning was evaluated 24 h later.
To determine whether drug treatment affected the baseline capacity to make a response, we evaluated the shock intensity needed to generate a comparable (0.4 N) flexion force across groups and the duration of the first flexion response at the start of testing (n = 6 subjects/group). The mean shock intensity ranged from 0.66 (±0.07) to 0.76 (±0.06) mA and the mean duration of the initial response (+SE) ranged from 0.14 (±0.01) to 0.16 (±0.01) s. Independent ANOVAs failed to reveal significant main effects or interactions on either measure, indicating that the capacity to make a response was not significantly different across groups, all F values <2.84, p > 0.05
The impact of the mGluR1 antagonist CPCCOEt on metaplasticity of spinal learning is depicted in Figure 1A–E. Vehicle-treated subjects learned to maintain the leg in a flexed position, increasing flexion duration over time, whereas uncontrollable shock produced a metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning (Fig. 1A). The mGluR1 antagonist blocked induction of this learning deficit in a dose-dependent manner (Fig. 1A–E). Confirming these observations, an ANOVA on flexion duration revealed a significant main effect of shock (F(1,40) = 26.67; p < 0.0001). The interactions for shock by dose (F(3,40) = 3.21; p < 0.05) and dose by time (F(87,1160) = 1.31; p < 0.05) also reached significance. No other terms reached significance (all F values <1.30; p > 0.05).
Impact of a mGluR5 antagonist
To test the role of mGluR5, we delivered the specific antagonist MPEP (Tocris) to spinalized subjects (n = 8) before uncontrollable shock. Immediately after drug administration, subjects were given 6 min of uncontrollable shock (0 or 1.5 mA) to the tail in a balanced, factorial design. Twenty-four hours later, subjects were prepared as described in Materials and Methods, and spinal learning was evaluated.
To evaluate whether drug treatment affected the capacity to perform the target response, we analyzed the duration of the initial response and the shock intensity necessary to induce a 0.4 N change in flexion force. The duration of the initial response (+SE) ranged from 0.14 (±0.01) to 0.18 (±0.02) s and the shock intensity necessary to elicit a 0.4 N flexion force (+SE) ranged from 0.57 (±0.06) to 0.67 (±0.04) mA. Independent ANOVAs failed to reveal any significant main effects or interactions (all F values <4.0; p > 0.05).
The impact of MPEP on flexion duration is depicted in Figure 1F–J. Saline-treated, unshocked animals demonstrated normal spinal learning (Fig. 1F). Animals that were given saline and uncontrollable shock had impaired learning, demonstrating shock-induced metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning (Fig. 1F). MPEP blocked the shock-induced learning deficit in a dose-dependent manner, restoring the capacity for spinal learning to the level of the unshocked group (Fig. 1F–J). Confirming these observations, an ANOVA on flexion duration revealed a significant main effect of shock (F(1,56) = 4.57; p < 0.05). Both the main effect of dose and the shock by dose interaction reached significance (both F(3,56) > 2.76; p < 0.05). In addition, the main effect of time (F(29,1624) = 13.30; p < 0.001) reached significance. No other main effects or interactions were significant (all F values <1.32; p > 0.05).
Antagonism of mGluR1 but not mGluR5 facilitates acquisition of spinal learning
The observation that mGluR1 and mGluR5 antagonists restore spinal learning suggests that exposure to uncontrollable stimulation engages a group I mGluR-mediated process that inhibits spinal learning. To further test this hypothesis, we asked whether an antagonist would foster acquisition of the learned response. Subjects were given either CPCCOEt or vehicle immediately followed by spinal training. Trend analysis was used to statistically evaluate the slope of acquisition over time. As shown in Figure 2A, CPCCOEt fostered acquisition of the learned response. As in previous studies (Gómez-Pinilla et al., 2007), facilitation was most evident in the first 5 min, before asymptotic performance. An ANOVA revealed a significant effect of time (F(29,406) = 15.31; p < 0.0001). Although neither the main effect of drug treatment, nor the time by drug interaction were significant (both F values <1.28; p > 0.05), polynomial contrast revealed a significant linear component for the time by drug interaction over the full training interval (F(1,406) = 10.34). Neither the quadratic nor cubic contrasts were significant (both F values <1.53). The significant linear component indicates that the acquisition curve for the CPCCOEt group had a steeper overall slope than the vehicle-treated control. To test whether this steeper slope was driven by step function-like changes in learning in individual subjects, we performed a t test on the time until the first 60 s flexion duration (Gallistel et al., 2004). This analysis failed to yield a significant effect, suggesting that the significant increase in the slope of the learning curves in CPCCOEt subjects was attributable to increases in slope of individual learning curves. Graphical analysis confirmed this (data not shown). MPEP, however, did not have a significant effect on spinal learning (Fig. 2B) (all p > 0.05). Together, the mGluR antagonist findings indicate that mGluR1 plays a direct role in acquisition of spinal cord learning, whereas both mGluR1 and mGluR5 play a role in metaplastic inhibition of spinal cord learning. This suggests that acquisition of spinal learning involves pathways that are differentially engaged by mGluR1 and mGluR5, whereas metaplastic inhibition involves common pathways. PKC activation is one well established common pathway activated by both mGluR1 and mGluR5 (for review, see Neugebauer, 2002) providing a potential target for restoring spinal cord plasticity.
Activation of group I mGluRs generates lasting inhibition of spinal learning
The antagonist experiments indicate that group I mGluRs are necessary for metaplastic inhibition of spinal cord learning. To test whether engaging these receptors is sufficient to inhibit spinal learning, we used the agonist DHPG. This compound has been shown in hippocampal culture to induce internalization of iGluRs and LTD, suggesting that it can have lasting effects on cellular plasticity. If mGluRs underlie metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning, DHPG should induce a lasting effect that mirrors the established time course of shock-induced inhibition (Crown et al., 2002b). We therefore delivered DHPG at a range of doses (n = 16) and tested spinal learning 24 h after.
To determine whether DHPG affected response vigor, we analyzed the shock intensity necessary to elicit a 0.4 N change in flexion force and the duration of the initial response. The mean shock intensity ranged from 0.53 (±0.04) to 0.73 (±0.08) mA and the mean duration of the initial response (+SE) ranged from 0.14 (±0.01) to 0.17 (+0.01) s. Independent ANOVAs failed to reveal any significant main effects or interactions on either outcome (all F values <2.76; p > 0.05).
The impact of drug treatment on spinal learning is illustrated in Figure 3. Saline-treated subjects learned to maintain the leg in a flexed position over time (Fig. 3A). DHPG delivered 24 h before testing impaired spinal instrumental learning (Fig. 3A,B). Confirming these effects, an ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of drug (F(3,56) = 3.02; p < 0.05). A Duncan's post hoc analysis on the main effect of drug revealed that the vehicle-treated group exhibited superior performance (longer flexion durations) relative to the DHPG-treated groups (p < 0.05). No other main effects or interactions reached significance (all p > 0.05).
PKC activation by stimulus conditions that inhibit spinal learning
The pharmacological data provide links between group I mGluRs and metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning. The most striking finding was that a single bolus of DHPG produced a lasting deficit in spinal learning that was apparent 24 h later. To our knowledge, this is the first drug manipulation identified that directly inhibits spinal learning for a lasting interval. This suggests that group I mGluRs engage cellular changes that produce lasting alterations of spinal plasticity. Group I mGluRs are known to activate a G-protein-linked cascade involving PKC (Hermans and Challiss, 2001). To test whether PKC activation is associated with inhibition of spinal learning, we examined whether uncontrollable shock increases PKC activation using an ELISA. Subjects (N = 24) received uncontrollable shock or nothing and the L4–S2 tissue was collected 10 min, 1 h, or 24 h later. Previous work has shown that instrumental learning occurs within the L4–S2 segments (Crown et al., 2002b; Liu et al., 2005).
Shock treatment increased PKC activity in the lumbar-sacral cord (Fig. 4). The main effect of shock was significant (F(1,18) = 6.84; p < 0.05). Neither the main effect of time nor its interaction with shock treatment reached statistical significance (both F values <1.27; p > 0.05). Orthogonal planned comparisons of the shocked and unshocked groups at each time point revealed a robust effect at 1 h (F(1,6) = 8.77; p < 0.05) but not at 10 min or 24 h (both F values <2.54; p > 0.05).
PKC activity is necessary for metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning
The finding of PKC activation after uncontrollable shock suggests that PKC may play a role in metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning. To directly test this hypothesis, we delivered two structurally distinct PKC inhibitors and tested for restoration of spinal cord learning. We first used the staurosporine-derivative BIM, GF 109203X, at a range of doses that have been shown to inhibit PKC (Herbert et al., 1990; Toullec et al., 1991) and affect pain behavior when administered intrathecally (Yashpal et al., 1995; Hua et al., 1999; Heinke and Sandkühler, 2005). To confirm PKC specificity, we performed an independent experiment with the structurally distinct PKC inhibitor chelerythrine at a similarly effective dose (Hua et al., 1999). The capacity for spinal learning was tested 24 h later.
To determine whether drug treatment affected baseline behavioral reactivity, we examined both the shock intensity needed to elicit a 0.4 N flexion force and the duration of the first flexion response. Neither BIM nor chelerythrine affected performance on either measure (all F values <3.0; p > 0.05).
Unshocked controls exhibited a progressive increase in flexion duration indicative of instrumental learning. Rats that received the vehicle before shock treatment exhibited poor learning (Fig. 5A,E). Treatment with either BIM (Fig. 5A–D) or chelerythrine (Fig. 5E–G) attenuated this shock-induced learning deficit. ANOVA on the BIM data confirmed that the linear component of the drug by shock interaction was significant (F(1,48) = 5.28; p < 0.05). There was also a significant effect of time (F(29,1392) = 19.32; p < 0.0001). No other variable approached statistical significance (all F values <1.69; p > 0.05). Post hoc comparisons of the group means using Duncan's multiple range test confirmed that vehicle-treated shocked subjects differed from the unshocked controls (both vehicle and BIM, 0.0073 nmol) and the shocked group that received 0.023 nmol of BIM. No other differences were significant (all p > 0.05).
ANOVA on the chelerythrine data revealed a significant linear effect of time (F(1,28) = 19.97; p < 0.001). There was also a significant drug by shock interaction (F(1,28) = 17.20; p < 0.001). Post hoc comparisons with Duncan's multiple range test revealed that the vehicle-treated, shocked subjects had significantly lower performance than all other groups.
To test for significant differences between the two inhibitors, we merged the datasets from the BIM and chelerythrine experiments and then used ANOVA to test for a differential effect of the PKC inhibitors. There were no significant differences between the two drugs (all p > 0.05), suggesting that the different PKC inhibitors were similarly effective.
The findings provide strong support for a role of PKC in metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning. We found similar effects using both BIM and chelerythrine, two mechanistically distinct, selective PKC inhibitors (Herbert et al., 1990; Toullec et al., 1991) that have well documented behavioral and biochemical effects when administered in the spinal cord (Yashpal et al., 1995; Hua et al., 1999; Granados-Soto et al., 2000; Guo et al., 2004). However, this does not exclude the possibility that other kinases may also contribute to these effects, because off-target drug effects or reduced PKC activity could affect signaling across several interlinked pathways.
PKC activation is not necessary for spinal learning
Group I mGluR antagonism selectively blocked the long-term consequences of uncontrollable stimulation, but did not impair learning. Having demonstrated that PKC inhibition also attenuates the consequences of uncontrollable stimulation, we tested its effect on spinal learning. We delivered BIM (0.023 nmol) and assessed spinal learning 15 min later. Vehicle-treated rats exhibited a progressive increase in flexion duration over the 30 min of training (Fig. 6). BIM did not significantly alter the learning over time or the group means (31.8 ± 9.6 for BIM vs 30.7 ± 10.6 for vehicle) (all p > 0.9). This suggests that PKC is involved in metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning but has little effect on the rate of spinal learning itself.
Group I mGluRs inhibit spinal learning through PKC activation
Given that group I mGluRs and PKC were both implicated in metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning, we tested whether mGluR-induced inhibition of spinal learning depends on PKC. Previous studies suggest that DHPG activates PKC in a BIM-reversible manner (Camodeca et al., 1999; Fundytus et al., 2001; Giles et al., 2007). To test whether metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning by DHPG (Fig. 3) depends on PKC, we delivered BIM 15 min before DHPG and assayed spinal learning 24 later.
Spinally transected rats received BIM (0.023 nmol) or its vehicle (100% DMSO), followed by DHPG (100 nmol) or its vehicle (0.9% saline). Baseline flexion vigor, and instrumental learning, was then assessed as described above. Subjects that received two vehicle injections or BIM plus saline were able to acquire the instrumental response (Fig. 7A). Treatment with DMSO plus DHPG produced a marked learning deficit (Fig. 7B). This finding provided an independent replication of the earlier data in which a deficit was observed after administration of DHPG. Pretreatment with the PKC inhibitor BIM blocked the detrimental effects of DHPG (Fig. 7B,C). An ANOVA on the flexion duration data yielded a significant trials by BIM by DHPG three-way interaction (p < 0.05). Post hoc comparisons (Duncan's) confirmed that the DHPG-treated group had significantly lower performance than the other three (p < 0.05). No other differences were significant.
Together with the previous experiments, the findings strongly suggest that metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning involves group I mGluR activation and engagement of intracellular pathways involving PKC.
Discussion
The present findings demonstrate that group I mGluR and PKC activity alter the requirements for future synaptic modification in the spinal cord. When exposed to uncontrollable shock, the spinal cord develops a lasting impairment in behavioral plasticity (Grau et al., 1998; Crown and Grau, 2001; Crown et al., 2002b; Ferguson et al., 2003, 2006; Joynes and Grau, 2004; Patton et al., 2004; Washburn et al., 2007). Pharmacological activation of mGluRs mimicked the effects of uncontrollable shock, whereas mGluR antagonism at the time of training facilitated learning. On stimulation, mGluRs act through intracellular signaling pathways to elevate PKC activity (Pisani et al., 1997; Ugolini et al., 1997; Skeberdis et al., 2001). Uncontrollable shock increased PKC activity in the spinal cord, and the long-term effects of uncontrollable shock were blocked by group I mGluR antagonists and a PKC inhibitor. In parallel, the mGluR–PKC pathway was implicated in pharmacologically induced metaplasticity because spinal cord neurons exposed to an mGluR agonist and a PKC inhibitor together maintained intact learning ability.
DHPG is the first drug demonstrated to produce a lasting impairment of spinal learning. Manipulation of other spinal receptors, including iGluRs, serotonergic, noradrenergic, GABAergic, and opioid systems, has only transient effects (for review, see Grau et al., 2006). Long-lasting synaptic modifications in the CNS commonly involve potentiation or depression of iGluR responses through posttranslational receptor modification and trafficking. Group I mGluR agonists produce long-lasting PKC-dependent potentiation of NMDAR and AMPAR responses in spinal cord motoneurons (Ugolini et al., 1997). In the dorsal horn of transverse spinal slices, group I mGluRs signal through PKC to increase tyrosine phosphorylation of NMDAR subunit NR2B, which is associated with increased channel conductance and calcium influx (Ali and Salter, 2001; Guo et al., 2004). NMDAR potentiation through mGluR–PKC signaling is observed in hippocampal preparations and Xenopus oocyte systems as well (Aniksztejn et al., 1992; Skeberdis et al., 2001). The correspondence between the present findings and these electrophysiological and biochemical data provides avenues for additional investigation; however, predictions are not straightforward, because the specific neuronal circuits underlying flexion response learning are not known. mGluR activation can lead to simultaneous potentiation and depression in distinct parts of the spinal circuit as well as changes in cellular excitability and network properties (Jones and Headley, 1995; Zhong et al., 2000; Marchetti et al., 2003; Clem et al., 2008), although not all of these effects involve PKC (Heinke and Sandkühler, 2005).
A hypothetical postsynaptic mechanism for metaplasticity of spinal learning
Based on the hypothesis that mGluRs control metaplasticity of spinal learning by altering NMDAR function, we can propose a cellular/molecular model that provides numerous targets for restoring adaptive spinal plasticity (Fig. 8). According to the model, response-contingent shock yields a patterned, highly regulated release of glutamate that activates NMDARs (Joynes et al., 2004) and downstream signaling pathways (Fig. 8A). Spinal learning performance correlates with the mRNA levels of calcium/calmodulin-dependent kinase II (CaMKII) and cAMP response element binding protein (CREB), and BDNF facilitates spinal learning (Baumbauer et al., 2007b; Gómez-Pinilla et al., 2007). These molecules are linked to NMDAR-mediated long-term potentiation (Yin and Tully, 1996; Poncer et al., 2002; Bramham and Messaoudi, 2005), suggesting a mechanism for spinal learning with response-contingent shock.
NMDARs detect coincident presynaptic and postsynaptic activity and allow calcium influx eventually leading to altered synaptic strength. Stimulation above or below a certain threshold produces potentiation or depression, respectively, by generating different patterns of postsynaptic calcium influx (Abraham and Bear, 1996). If, as suggested above, NMDAR conductances are altered by increased PKC phosphorylation after DHPG or uncontrollable shock, the threshold for synaptic modification may shift downward (Fig. 8B). Spinal cord synapses would lose the ability to encode more subtle stimulation patterns and adapt to the imposed response contingency. Blockade of these changes with antagonists of either mGluRs (Fig. 1) or NMDAR (Ferguson et al., 2006) protects learning capacity by preventing this inhibitory metaplasticity.
Links to spinal nociception
Spinal mGluRs are involved in nociceptive processing (Coderre and Melzack, 1992; Fisher and Coderre, 1996a,b; Stanfa and Dickenson, 1998; Neugebauer et al., 1999; Bordi and Ugolini, 2000; Karim et al., 2001; Fisher et al., 2002; Fundytus et al., 2002; Mills et al., 2002; Zhang et al., 2002). Activation of group I mGluRs induces spontaneous nociceptive behaviors in rats (Fisher and Coderre, 1996a). Moreover, group I mGluR antagonists reduce nociceptive responses in a number of paradigms including sciatic nerve ligation (Fisher et al., 2002), inflammation by carrageenan/kaolin, formalin (Stanfa and Dickenson, 1998; Karim et al., 2001; Zhang et al., 2002), and intradermal capsaicin (Neugebauer et al., 1999).
The present findings suggest that spinal mGluRs not only affect nociceptive processing but also modify the capacity for spinal learning. This supports other work linking metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning with nociceptive plasticity. Inhibition of spinal learning can be induced by incision injury or intradermal carrageenan and uncontrollable shock produces a tactile hyperreactivity (Ferguson et al., 2006; Young et al., 2007). In addition, inhibition of spinal learning involves receptor systems known to modulate pain, including neurokinin (Baumbauer et al., 2007a), κ-opioid (Joynes and Grau, 2004; Washburn et al., 2008), GABAA (Ferguson et al., 2003), and 5-HT receptors (Crown and Grau, 2005). Removing descending inhibition with a dorsolateral funiculus transection increases nociceptive reactivity (Eide and Hole, 1993; Liu et al., 1997) and renders spinal learning vulnerable to metaplastic inhibition (Crown and Grau, 2005). This suggests that descending inhibitory tracts passing through the dorsolateral funiculus help to maintain the balance of spinal plasticity, preventing both plasticity within pain pathways and metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning.
Plasticity in NMDAR-mediated synaptic transmission provides a framework for understanding the relationship between nociception and metaplastic inhibition. Both mGluR and PKC have been linked to the enhancement of nociceptive reactivity observed after peripheral inflammation and the sensitization of spinal nociceptive pathways (Woolf, 1983; Coderre and Melzack, 1985, 1992; Fisher and Coderre, 1996b; Fundytus et al., 2002). Enhanced nociception in models of neuropathic pain is attributed to an increased contribution of the NMDAR to sensory processing in the spinal cord (Willis, 2001). Phosphorylation of the NR2B subunit increases channel open probability and raises the overall excitability of the system. Because uncontrollable stimulation also enhances nociceptive reactivity, it is possible that these treatments induce overlapping mechanisms. Thus, modified NMDAR currents could both enhance immediate responses to stimulation and alter the stimulus threshold for spinal plasticity, negatively impacting the ability to produce new adaptive responses.
Implications for management of metaplasticity after spinal cord injury
Damage to descending inhibitory tracts after spinal cord injury yields dysregulation of spinal plasticity, rendering spinal learning vulnerable to metaplastic inhibition (Crown and Grau, 2005). Alterations in spinal metaplasticity could have profound effects on recovery of function by altering the ability of the spinal cord to regain proper function (Edgerton et al., 2001; Hook and Grau, 2007; Courtine et al., 2008). Recent findings indicate that uncontrollable shock undermines recovery of function after spinal contusion injury (Grau et al., 2004; Hook et al., 2007). Attempts to control nociceptive transmission with an opioid yielded little benefit and may have enhanced histopathological damage (Hook et al., 2007).
There is variability in the rate and level to which patients recover after spinal cord injury. The factors dictating recovery have yet to be fully elucidated. One critical variable may be metaplastic modulation of glutamatergic plasticity. Glutamate is widely thought to exacerbate cell death and associated functional losses after spinal cord injury (for review, see Beattie, 2004; Park et al., 2004). Recent data indicate that glutamate-mediated cell death may involve plasticity and trafficking of iGluRs (Hermann et al., 2001; Beattie et al., 2002; Ogoshi et al., 2005; Stellwagen et al., 2005; Ferguson et al., 2008). Moreover, the group I mGluRs have been implicated in both neuropathic pain and functional losses after spinal cord injury (Mukhin et al., 1996; Agrawal et al., 1998; Mills and Hulsebosch, 2002; Mills et al., 2002). The precise role of spinal metaplasticity in these effects remains a topic for additional study.
Conclusion and future directions
One advantage of a simple spinal learning task is that it provides a high-throughput model for screening experimental therapeutics that alter spinal metaplasticity. To the extent that metaplasticity plays a role in recovery of function after spinal cord injury, this could prove to be a powerful tool for the field of spinal cord injury research (Grau et al., 2006). Using a simple spinal learning task, the present findings suggest that group I mGluRs generate metaplastic inhibition of spinal learning through a PKC-dependent mechanism. Based on these observations, we hypothesize that controlling mGluR induction of PKC should facilitate adaptive plasticity in the spinal cord across a broad range of injury conditions. This could yield functional benefits after spinal cord injury by promoting use-dependent plasticity.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by National Institute of Health Grants NS041548 [National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS)] and HD058412 (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development) to J.W.G. A.R.F. is supported by NINDS Grant F32NS053059. We thank Todd C. Sacktor for advice about PKC inhibitors.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Adam R. Ferguson, Brain and Spinal Injury Center, University of California, San Francisco, 1001 Potrero Avenue, Building 1, Room 101, San Francisco, CA 94110. adam.ferguson{at}ucsf.edu