Abstract
Many principal neurons undergo an early developmental switch from GluR2-lacking to GluR2-containing synaptic glutamate receptors. We tested the generality and timing of the GluR2 switch in excitatory neurons of rat somatosensory cortex. Previous studies show that the switch occurs between postnatal day 14 (P14) and P16 in layer 5 pyramidal neurons. We show, using sensitivity to intracellular spermine, that a similar switch occurs between P12 and P14 in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells and between P7 and P8 in layer 4 stellate cells. The presence of GluR2-lacking receptors in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells before P12 was confirmed by demonstrating sensitivity to blockade by 1-naphthyl-acetyl-spermine and large single-channel conductances. GluR2 and the postsynaptic protein PSD95 show progressive colocalization in tissue from P10, P14, and P24 rats, mirroring electrophysiological developments. To distinguish whether changes in GluR2 expression or targeting underlie the switch, we characterized dendritic AMPA receptor responses using focal photolysis of caged glutamate. Contrary to synaptic responses, dendritic responses at all ages studied (P6–P40) were characteristic of GluR2-containing receptors. In addition, dendritically and synaptically evoked responses showed a corresponding decrease in NMDA/AMPA ratios in pyramidal cells, suggesting parallel mechanisms that regulate neuronal calcium levels. These data suggest that the GluR2 switch results from changes in AMPA receptor targeting during early postnatal development, and that rather than following the laminar sequence of cortical development, it proceeds sequentially from layer 4 to layer 2/3 and finally to layer 5b.
Introduction
AMPA receptors are the main mediators of fast excitatory neurotransmission in the CNS (Dingledine et al., 1999). They are homotetrameric or heterotetrameric assemblies of GluR1 through GluR4 subunits (or, synonymously, GluRA-D) (Keinanen et al., 1990; Mano and Teichberg, 1998; Rosenmund et al., 1998). AMPA receptors lacking GluR2 subunits, as well as kainate receptors, are Ca2+ permeable (Hollmann et al., 1991) and are modulated by polyamines. Intracellular polyamines preferably bind to receptors at depolarized membrane potentials and, when bound, block the channel pore. This results in inwardly rectifying current/voltage (I/V) relationships (Verdoorn et al., 1991; Kamboj et al., 1995; Koh et al., 1995). AMPA receptors containing one or more GluR2 subunits are Ca2+ impermeable and are not modulated by polyamines (Washburn et al., 1997). Receptor interacting proteins can be secondary determinants of polyamine modulation; one example is the association of stargazin with GluR2-lacking receptors, which attenuates polyamine block (Soto et al., 2007). Regulation of synaptic strength involving distinct populations of AMPA receptors is central to the expression of various forms of plasticity (Hayashi et al., 2000; Plant et al., 2006). Phosphorylation and other posttranslational modifications of the receptor subunits themselves or their interacting proteins play important roles in synaptic targeting (Matsuda et al., 1999; Chetkovich et al., 2002; Esteban et al., 2003; Mauceri et al., 2004; Liu and Cull-Candy, 2005; Guire et al., 2008).
Layer 5 neocortical pyramidal neurons in rats undergo a developmental switch around P15 in which immature, Ca2+ permeable, GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors are replaced in synapses by mature GluR2-containing receptors (Kumar et al., 2002; Shin et al., 2005). We investigated whether the GluR2 switch also occurs in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells and layer 4 stellate cells (i.e., whether it is a general property of principal neocortical neurons). We found that the AMPA receptor subunit switch in layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons precedes that in layer 5b neurons, whereas layer 4 stellate cells express mainly GluR2-containing AMPA receptors already at P8. Thus, timing of the GluR2 switch does not correspond to the ontogenetic age of principal neurons. We also show that inwardly rectifying AMPA receptors are absent from extrasynaptic sites of pyramidal cells. In addition, NMDA/AMPA ratios were more than twofold higher at extrasynaptic than at synaptic sites, and the ratios at both sites decreased during development. Expression of GluR2-containing AMPA receptors and downregulation of the NMDA/AMPA ratios would work in concert to progressively limit synaptically driven calcium influx during neocortical development.
Materials and Methods
Slice/tissue preparation.
All experiments were performed according to protocols approved by the Stanford Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Sprague Dawley rats aged postnatal day 6 (P6) to P50 were deeply anesthetized using 50 mg/kg sodium pentobarbital, and brains were removed and immediately transferred to ice-cold sucrose solution containing (in mm) 234 sucrose, 11 glucose, 24 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 MgSO4, and 0.5 CaCl2, equilibrated with a 95% O2/5% CO2 mixture. Coronal neocortical slices (400 μm) were sectioned on a VT1000S Vibratome (Leica) at 4°C in sucrose solution and transferred into a holding chamber filled with artificial CSF (ACSF; in mm: 126 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH3PO4, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, and 10 glucose, pH 7.4, equilibrated with 95% O2/5% CO2). After a recovery period of 1 h at 32°C, the holding chamber containing the slices was removed from the water bath and allowed to cool to room temperature.
Electrophysiology.
Slices were transferred to a recording chamber and constantly superfused with oxygenated ACSF at a rate of ∼2 ml/min. All experiments were conducted at room temperature, and all cells recorded were located in primary somatosensory cortex in trunk and limb areas. The location of the six layers of the primary somatosensory cortex was determined from an overview image at 5× magnification, and neurons were visualized at 63× magnification using differential contrast optics with an Axioskop 2 FS microscope (Carl Zeiss). Pyramidal cells were identified based on their large size, tear-shaped morphology, and thick apical dendrite. In addition, cells were routinely filled with 0.3% w/v biocytin for further verification of their identity. Whole-cell recordings in voltage-clamp mode were obtained using borosilicate glass electrodes with a tip resistance of 2–4 MΩ. The pipette solution contained (in mm) 130 Cs-gluconate, 8 CsCl, 10 HEPES, 4 EGTA, and 0.01 [N-(2,6-dimethylphenylcarbamoylmethyl) triethylammonium bromide] (QX314), pH 7.3 adjusted with CsOH (290 mOsm) for all experiments except recordings of spontaneous EPSCs, in which we used (in mm) 120 K-gluconate, 11 KCl, 1 MgCl2, 1 CaCl2, 10 HEPES, and 10 EGTA, pH 7.3, adjusted with KOH. For measurements of I/V relationships and paired-pulse ratios (PPRs), 50 μm spermine was included in the pipette solution. Unless indicated otherwise, NMDA receptor-mediated conductances were blocked by bath application of either 100 μm dl-AP-5 (Tocris) or 50 μm d-AP-5 (Ascent Scientific); GABAA receptor-mediated conductances were blocked using 50 μm picrotoxin (Tocris). Only recordings in which the series resistance was <25 MΩ were included in the data analysis, and membrane potentials were corrected for a liquid junction potential of 20 mV (all voltages given subsequently include liquid junction potential correction). A concentric bipolar electrode (75 μm diameter; CB-XRC75; Frederick Haer) was positioned on-column in layer 4 for electrical stimulation of layer 2/3 neurons, in layer 2/3 or upper layer 4 for electrical stimulation of layer 5 neurons, and in layer 4 or 5a for layer 4 neurons. Synaptic activity was evoked by delivering constant current pulses of 100–500 μA for 100 μs at intervals between 5 and 20 s. Stimulation intensities were selected to correspond to ∼2× threshold for a detectable response. Signals were amplified with a Multiclamp 700A amplifier, sampled at 10 kHz, filtered at 3 kHz, acquired using a Digidata 1320A digitizer, and analyzed using pClamp9 (all from Molecular Devices).
Focal photolysis of caged glutamate.
A pulsed 355 nm UV laser beam (DPSS Lasers) was directed into the back aperture of the 63× microscope objective and was controlled with mirror galvanometers (model 6210; Cambridge Technology) using a locally developed software program. Focal photolysis of 4-methoxy-7-nitroindolinyl (MNI)-caged glutamate (100 μm; Tocris) was triggered by UV light pulses (50–500 μs, unless indicated otherwise). MNI-caged glutamate was supplied in a 30 ml recirculating bath solution. To find a location for photolysis, we scanned areas in a 100–500 μm radius surrounding the soma of the recorded cell until we robustly detected graded, short-latency (<3 ms) “direct dendritic responses” that were abolished by moving the laser beam >10–20 μm laterally. Our experience shows that MNI–glutamate remains stable for up to several hours (Deleuze and Huguenard, 2006; Jin et al., 2006). We typically exchanged the 30 ml recirculating bath solution after 3–4 h or sooner if we detected significant rundown of direct dendritic responses.
I/V relationships were assayed by evoking EPSCs or direct dendritic responses from holding potentials of −60, −45, −30, −15, 0, 15, 30, 45, and 60 mV (after correction for liquid junction potential). At least two, but typically three to five direct dendritic responses and six to eight synaptic responses were averaged, and synaptic failures were excluded. Rectification indices were defined as the ratio of the slope at positive (0–60 mV) and negative (−60 to 0 mV) holding potentials. Slopes were derived by linear regression of maximal response amplitudes. Voltage-dependent unblocking was assayed by measuring PPRs (100 ms interstimulus intervals) of EPSCs at holding potentials of −60 and +50 mV. Voltage-dependent blocking indices were calculated by dividing the PPR at +50 mV by the PPR at −60 mV. Ratios >1 indicated voltage-dependent unblocking.
Pharmacological experiments.
For testing sensitivity to extracellular polyamines, 30 min of spontaneous activity were recorded in voltage-clamp mode at a holding potential of −65 mV in the presence of d-APV and picrotoxin. After 10 min in regular ACSF, the bath solution was changed to ACSF supplemented with 200 μm 1-naphthyl-acteyl-spermine (NAS; Sigma-Aldrich) for 10 min, and finally changed back to regular ACSF for 10 min. The last 5 min of each interval were analyzed, to allow for a 5 min washin and washout of NAS. For control experiments, 30 min of spontaneous activity in regular ACSF was recorded, and equivalent time points were analyzed (i.e., minutes 5–10, 15–20, and 25–30). EPSCs were detected and quantified using locally written software.
Nonstationary fluctuation analysis.
Spontaneous EPSCs recorded during a 10 min period at a holding potential of −65 mV in the presence of d-APV and picrotoxin were identified and aligned to their peak. An average trace was computed, and its scaled decay phase was subtracted from each individual trace. The background variance was calculated from event-free portions of the recording and subtracted. The average background-subtracted variance of these synaptic fluctuations was plotted against the amplitude of the averaged EPSC and fit to a parabolic curve using the following equation: σ2 = iI − I2/N, where i is the single-channel current, I is the peak of the mean current, and N is the number of channels activated at the peak of the mean current (Benke et al., 2001). The single-channel conductance (γ) was calculated using γ = i/V (V = holding potential = −65 mV assuming a reversal potential of 0 mV).
NMDA/AMPA ratios.
Cells were held at membrane potentials of +50 and −60 mV in the presence of QX314 in the internal solution and picrotoxin in the ASCF. EPSCs and direct dendritic responses were evoked as described for I/V relationships, and 6–15 sweeps were averaged. The ratio of the peak amplitudes at +50 mV (typically within 20 ms, predominantly NMDA receptor mediated) and at −60 mV (typically within 5 ms, predominantly AMPA receptor mediated) was calculated.
Immunohistochemistry.
Paraformaldehyde-fixed and cryoprotected tissue was frozen and cut into 40 μm sections on a HM400 cryotome (Microm). Sections were blocked in PBS and 10% normal goat serum for 1 h at room temperature and incubated in a primary antibody overnight at 4°C (rabbit polyclonal anti-GluR2, mouse monoclonal PSD95; 1 μg/ml; Millipore Bioscience Research Reagents). Sections were rinsed twice at room temperature for 5 min and incubated in a solution containing fluorescent secondary antibodies at room temperature for 1 h (2 μg/ml goat anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 488 and 2 μg/ml goat anti-mouse IgG Alexa Fluor 568; Invitrogen). After rinsing twice in PBS for 5 min, sections were mounted on SuperfrostPlus slides (Thermo Fisher Scientific) and coverslipped using Vectashield mounting medium (Vector Laboratories). “Z-stacks” (25 × 25 × 5 μm) of 12 images (at a vertical spacing of 417 nm) were captured on an LSM510 confocal laser-scanning microscope (Carl Zeiss). Locations of z-stack images were chosen randomly. X, Y, and Z coordinates of all GluR2-positive (red) and PSD95-positive (green) objects consisting of three or more contiguous voxels were analyzed using volocity software (Improvision). To calculate the degree of colocalization of GluR2 with PSD95, we identified and counted the number of GluR2-positive objects in 0.05 μm shells around each PSD95-positive object using a customized software. The small volume of the innermost shell precluded a statistically valid determination of colocalization at distances of <0.05 μm in layer 2/3. Particle density was fit to a single-exponential decay function; in layer 2/3, the innermost shell was excluded from the analysis, and the fit was extrapolated to x = 0. Uniformity of antibody penetration and secondary staining was confirmed by demonstrating that density of GluR2- and PSD95-positive particles did not change as a function of depth in the section. To minimize edge effects, GluR2 particle density analysis was restricted to PSD95-positive particles interior to the stack and >1 μm from any edge.
Results
Inwardly rectifying AMPA receptors in immature excitatory cells in layers 2/3, 4, and 5b
We recorded I/V curves from 52 layer 2/3 pyramidal cells, 46 layer 4 stellate cells, and 50 layer 5b pyramidal cells in the primary somatosensory cortex of rats aged P6–P40. Linear I/V curves are indicative of GluR2-containing, calcium-impermeable AMPA receptors, whereas increasing inward rectification is indicative of higher relative proportions of GluR2-lacking, calcium-permeable AMPA receptors. For recordings in layer 5b, the stimulation electrode was placed on-column in layer 2/3 (but occasionally in upper layer 4) to permit a preferential activation of layer 2/3 to layer 5b projections. As reported previously (Kumar et al., 2002; Shin et al., 2005), layer 5 pyramidal cells switched from inwardly rectifying to linear I/V relationships around P15 (rectification index: P8–P14, 0.62 ± 0.04, n = 26; P16–P40, 1.04 ± 0.03, n = 24; p < 0.0001) (Fig. 1A2). For recordings in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells, the stimulation electrode was placed on-column in layer 4. Because layer 2/3 receives the majority of excitatory inputs from layer 4 (Dantzker and Callaway, 2000; Shepherd et al., 2003; Bureau et al., 2004), and descending layer 2/3 pyramidal cell axons usually do not arborize extensively in layer 4 (Petersen and Sakmann, 2001; Larsen and Callaway, 2006), layer 4 stimulation will result mainly in activation of afferents from that layer. The examples in Figure 1A1 and the summary data in Figure 1, A3 and C, show that a similar developmental switch occurred in layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons, but it preceded the switch in layer 5 neurons by ∼2 d (rectification index: P8–P12, 0.59 ± 0.04, n = 15; P13–P40, 1.03 ± 0.02, n = 37; p < 0.0001). In slices from P13 and P14 rats, we found a significant difference in the average rectification indices between layer 2/3 and layer 5b pyramidal cells (layer 2/3: 0.92 ± 0.03, n = 11; layer 5: 0.70 ± 0.05, n = 14; p < 0.01) (Fig. 1A3). Comparisons of rectification indices between layer 2/3 and 5 pyramidal cells in slices from younger (P8–P12) or older (P16–P40) animals showed no significant differences (Fig. 1A3, Table 1). For layer 4 recordings, the stimulation electrode was placed on-column below the recorded cell to activate both intralaminar and thalamocortical axons (Feldmeyer et al., 1999; Thomson et al., 2002). Figure 1B shows example traces from representative layer 4 stellate cells at P6, P9, and P14 and the corresponding I/V curves. We measured rectification indices in 46 layer 4 stellate cells in slices from rats aged P6–P19 by electrical stimulation of adjacent cortical areas (Table 2). Before P8, layer 4 cells had inwardly rectifying I/V curves (rectification index, 0.61 ± 0.05; n = 12) and continued to display slight inward rectification between P8 and P14 (0.81 ± 0.05; n = 24). After P14, they displayed linear I/V relationships (0.98 ± 0.03; n = 9) (Fig. 1C). It should be noted that there was an additional, minor change in rectification indices around P14 in layer 2/3 and layer 4 cells (Fig. 1C).
Inwardly rectifying AMPA receptors in immature principal neurons. A1, A2, Left three panels, Representative examples of EPSCs evoked from holding potentials of −60, −30, 0, 30, and 60 mV in layer 2/3 (A1) and layer 5 (A2) pyramidal cells of animals aged P9, P13/P14, and P17/P18. Recordings were obtained with 50 μm spermine in the patch pipette. Calibration: 20 ms, 10 pA. Right, I/V curves derived from normalized EPSC amplitudes of the traces shown in the left panels (white circles, P9; gray circles, P13–P14; black circles, P17–P18). Layer 2/3 cells at P9 and layer 5 cells at P9 and P13–P14 show inward rectification. Straight lines are added to indicate linear I/V relationships. A3, Summary data for all rectification indices (RIs) measured in layer 2/3 (L2/3; open circles) and layer 5 (L5; filled circles) cells for animals aged P8–P12, P13–P14, and P16–P40. Boxes to the right of individual data points show mean (open square), median (central horizontal line), and 25th and 75th (box) and 5th to 95th (whiskers) percentiles. The switch from inwardly rectifying to linear I/V relationships in layer 2/3 cells precedes the switch in layer 5 cells. **p < 0.001; ***p < 0.0001. B, Same as A1 and A2, but for layer 4 stellate cells from animals aged P6, P9, and P14. Calibration: 20 ms, 10 pA. Left, I/V curves derived from normalized traces for P6 (white circles), P9 (gray circles), and P14 (black circles). The P6 cell shows inward rectification. C, Developmental changes in RIs in principal neurons of layer 4, layer 2/3, and layer 5 in animals aged <P8, P8–P10, P11–P12, P13–P14, and >P14 (layer 2/3 and layer 5 from same dataset as in A3). RIs in all neocortical layers change during early postnatal development from inward rectification to linearity. The greatest RI increases occur around P8 in layer 4 cells, around P12/P13 in layer 2/3 cells, and around P14 in layer 5 cells. In layer 4 and layer 2/3 cells, there is a second, minor increase around P14. *p < 0.01; **p < 0.001; ***p < 0.0001. Numbers at the bottom of columns indicate sample size. Error bars indicate SEM. D1, Paired pulses (100 ms) were delivered from holding potentials of −60 mV (black lines) and +50 mV (gray lines). Raw traces (top) from representative layer 2/3 pyramidal cells of a P8 and a P18 rat are shown. Normalized traces (bottom) show that at P8 the second pulse at +50 mV is relatively larger than at −60 mV, whereas PPRs at P18 do not depend on the holding potential. Calibration: 50 ms, 20 pA. D2, Correlation between rectification index (x-axis) and unblocking index (y-axis) for layer 2/3 (L2/3; gray circles) and layer 5 (L5; black circles) pyramidal cells of P8–P40 rats and layer 4 stellate cells (L4; white circles) of P6–P18 rats. The solid line indicates fits by linear regression [dark gray, layer 2/3; black, layer 5 (largely overlaps with layer 2/3 regression line); light gray, layer 4).
Rectification indices (RIs) for synaptic and dendritic AMPA receptor-mediated responses in layer 2/3 and layer 5 pyramidal cells in slices from rats aged P8–P12, P13–P14, and P16–P40
RIs for synaptic AMPA receptor-mediated responses in layer 4 stellate cells in slices from rats aged P8–P12, P13–P14, and P16–P19
Intracellular polyamines preferentially block GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors at depolarized potentials, and this blockade is partially relieved in an activity-dependent manner (Rozov and Burnashev, 1999; Bagal et al., 2005). We used this phenomenon as further verification of the presence of GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors in immature pyramidal cells. Paired synaptic stimuli (100 ms interstimulus interval) were delivered at +50 and −60 mV, and the quotient (“unblocking index”) of the PPR at positive and negative holding potentials was calculated for most layer 2/3, layer 4, and layer 5b cells in which I/V curves had been recorded (see above). Unblocking indices of >1 (i.e., a greater PPR at positive membrane potentials) is consistent with voltage- and use-dependent relief of polyamine block. Figure 1D1 (top) shows example traces evoked by paired-pulse stimulation in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells of a P8 and a P18 rat at −60 mV (black trace) and +50 mV (gray trace). The normalized traces (Fig. 1D1, bottom) superimposed almost perfectly in the P18 cell. In the P8 cell, on the contrary, the second pulse was relatively larger at +50 mV than at −60 mV. Figure 1D2 shows the unblocking index plotted against the rectification index for layer 2/3 (gray circles; n = 43), layer 4 (white circles; n = 30), and layer 5 (black circles; n = 39) cells shown in Figure 1A3. Unblocking and rectification indices were inversely correlated (solid line), with low (<1) rectification indices (i.e., inward rectification) corresponding to high (>1) unblocking indices.
To further verify the presence of GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors on immature layer 2/3 pyramidal cell synapses, we tested two additional electrophysiological parameters. GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors have larger single-channel conductances than GluR2-containing receptors (Swanson et al., 1997). Single-channel conductances can be estimated from whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings using nonstationary fluctuation analysis (Benke et al., 2001). Briefly, currents during the decay phase of individual EPSCs deviate from the average. These fluctuations represent stochastic single-channel opening and closing events, such that the variance of current fluctuations is proportional to the single-channel conductance. We calculated the average single-channel conductances using isolated spontaneous AMPA receptor-mediated EPSCs from 10 min recordings in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in slices from 11 immature (P9–P11) and 7 more mature (P15 and P16) rats. Figure 2A1 shows representative data from a P9 and a P15 cell with single-channel conductances of 7.07 and 3.94 pS, respectively, and Figure 1A2 shows the summary data. As would be predicted for GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors, neurons from more immature rats had significantly larger average single-channel conductances than neurons from more mature animals (P9–P11: 7.01 ± 0.58 pS, n = 11; P15-P16: 3.58 ± 0.44 pS, n = 7; p < 0.001). These values are similar to those reported previously (Swanson et al., 1997; Benke et al., 1998).
GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors in immature layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons. A, Nonstationary fluctuation analysis to determine single-channel conductance of spontaneous EPSCs in layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons of rats aged P9–P11 and P15–P16. A1, Variance of current during the decay phase of EPSCs recorded in representative cells from a P10 (open circles) and a P15 (gray circles) animal. The P10 cell shows greater initial slope of the parabolic fit, indicating larger single-channel conductance, which is representative of GluR2-lacking receptors. A2, Calculated conductances of P9–P11 (open circles; γ = 7.01 ± 0.59 pS; n = 11) and P15–P16 (gray circles; γ = 3.58 ± 0.44 pS; n = 7) cells. **p < 0.001. B, Blockade of GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors by 200 μm of NAS. B1, Amplitudes of spontaneous EPSCs in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells averaged for a 5 min control period (a), 5 min of NAS or control without NAS (b; after a 5 min initial washin), and 5 min of washout (c). Left, NAS applied to cells from P9–P11 animals; middle, control without NAS applied to the same age group; right, NAS applied to cells from P15–P16 animals. B2, Averaged EPSCs from representative example cells from all conditions shown in B1. B3, Average reduction in spontaneous EPSC amplitude during period b for all three test conditions. P9–P11 with NAS, 19.9 ± 2.8%, n = 6; P9–P11 without NAS, −3.0 ± 1.7%, n = 6; P15–P16 with NAS, −3.1 ± 4.1%, n = 5. *p < 0.01. Error bars indicate SEM.
As a second test, we measured the susceptibility of AMPA receptor-mediated spontaneous EPSCs to blockade by extracellular NAS (bath perfused at a concentration of 200 μm), a synthetic analog of Joro spider toxin. This class of compounds causes open-channel block of GluR2-lacking AMPA and kainate receptors (Koike et al., 1997). Therefore, we expected the amplitude of spontaneous EPSCs in pyramidal cells from immature rats to decrease in the presence of NAS. EPSC amplitudes were quantified during a 5 min baseline period, during the last 5 min of a 10 min application of NAS, and in the last 5 min of a 10 min wash period. NAS susceptibility in six layer 2/3 pyramidal cells of rats aged P9–P11 and five cells of rats aged P15 and P16 was compared. Because we were concerned about possible NAS-independent rundown, we also included a control group (six cells, P9–P11) in which no NAS was applied. Figure 2B1 shows EPSCs amplitudes before, during, and after NAS application (or equivalent control periods) for all cells, and averaged example EPSCs are shown in Figure 2B2. NAS partially blocked EPSCs only in cells from the more immature rats. The average block in P9–P11 cells was 19.9 ± 2.8% (n = 6; p < 0.01), whereas EPSCs in vehicle (NAS free) time-matched experiments were unaffected (3.0 ± 1.7%; n = 6; p > 0.5). EPSCs in cells from P15/P16 were insensitive to NAS (3.1 ± 4.1%; n = 5; p > 0.05) (Fig. 2B3). Blockade by NAS of EPSCs in immature layer 2/3 pyramidal cells is consistent with the presence of GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors.
Additionally, we observed that EPSCs in mature layer 2/3 neurons were significantly slower than in immature cells, whereas those in layer 5 and layer 4 neurons did not change (supplemental Fig. S1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Although this change is concurrent with the AMPA receptor switch, it is likely attributable to different mechanisms, such as changes in AMPA receptor subunit splice variants (Mosbacher et al., 1994; Lambolez et al., 1996; Koike et al., 2000).
Colocalization of GluR2 and PSD95
As an independent measure of the emergence of synaptic GluR2, we assessed colocalization of GluR2 and the postsynaptic glutamatergic marker protein PSD95 in layers 2/3 and 5b in tissue from rats aged P10, P14, and P24 using immunohistochemistry and confocal microscopy. Figure 3 shows representative images of layer 2/3 (Fig. 3A1–A3) and layer 5 (Fig. 3B1–B3) for the three ages; GluR2 immunofluorescence is shown in red, and PSD95 immunofluorescence is shown in green. The predominantly punctate staining of both GluR2 and PSD95 indicates clustering of the antigens. In the neuropil, GluR2 and PSD95 immunofluorescence intensity appears to increase between P10 and P14, but direct comparisons are difficult because antibodies might penetrate differently in tissue of different ages. The images show a larger fraction of colocalizing (yellow) GluR2- and PSD95-positive puncta in P14 (Fig. 3A2) and P24 (Fig. 3A3) sections compared with P10 sections (Fig. 3A1) in layer 2/3. In layer 5b, the degree of colocalization increased between P10 and P14 (Fig. 3B1,B2) and between P14 and P24 (Fig. 3B2,B3).
GluR2/PSD97 colocalization. A, B, Representative confocal images from cortical sections of P10 (A1, B1), P14 (A2, B2), and P24 (A3, B3) rats. Images show 15 × 15 μm sections from layer 2/3 (A) and layer 5 (B) that were stained for PSD95 (green) and GluR2 (red). Increasing numbers of yellow puncta can be seen in sections from older animals. C, Analysis of the number of GluR2-positive puncta (particles/μm3) within 0.1 μm shells of increasing radius from each PSD95-positive object for layer 2/3 (C1) and layer 5 (C2). Error bars indicate SEM. *p < 0.01; **p < 0.001.
To quantify the position of GluR2-positive puncta relative to PSD95-positive puncta, we captured stacks of 12 images (25 × 25 × 5 μm total stack volume) and measured the density of GluR2-positive puncta in 100 nm shells surrounding each PSD95-positive object. If both sets of objects were distributed independently, the density of GluR2-positive puncta would remain constant in each shell. In contrast, increasing densities in the inner shells would indicate colocalization of the two antigens. Figure 3, C1 and C2, shows the results from seven to nine sections from layer 2/3 and layer 5b of P10, P14, and P24 animals. In layer 2/3, there is little colocalization of GluR2 and PSD95 at P10 but higher colocalization at P14 and P24. The density of GluR2-positive puncta was significantly increased at distances up to 300 nm from each PSD95-positive object in P14 and P24 tissue (Fig. 2C1). In layer 5, colocalization was moderate in sections from P10, with a small increase at P14 animals and a sharper increase by P24 (Fig. 3C2). This is in agreement with the physiological results indicating emergence of synaptic GluR2-containing AMPA receptors around P15 in layer 5 and around P12 in layer 2/3. It should be noted, though, that these experiments cannot assign a putative synapse to a particular neuronal type or its layer of origin. Layer 2/3 pyramidal cells receive the majority of synaptic inputs from layer 4 (Dantzker and Callaway, 2000; Shepherd et al., 2003; Bureau et al., 2004). Thus, the putative synapses in layer 2/3 would be a mixed population of intralaminar synapses and synapses of layer 4 afferents onto pyramidal cells, in addition to pyramidal cell-to-interneuron synapses. The synapses in layer 5 are likely more heterogeneous, because layer 5b pyramidal cells receive extensive synaptic inputs from layers 5 and 6 but also from layers 2–5a (Staiger et al., 2000; Schubert et al., 2001; Jin and Huguenard, 2006). Thus, the putative synapses in Figure 3C2 comprise layer 5 intralaminar synapses and afferents from other cortical layers. We found no significant trend in either the absolute numbers or the ratios of GluR2- to PSD95-positive puncta in each analyzed slice for the three age groups (ratio ranged from 1.06 ± 0.03 for layer 2/3 at P14 to 1.31 ± 0.08 for layer 5 at P10; data not shown). This demonstrates that the increasing colocalization reflects a shift in the distribution of the two antigens rather than a relative increase in the expression of one of them.
Characterization of direct responses mediated by dendritic AMPA receptors
Previous reports have suggested that, unlike synaptic AMPA receptors, somatic AMPA receptors in immature layer 5 pyramidal cells are GluR2 lacking (Kumar et al., 2002). Pyramidal cell somata are devoid of glutamatergic synapses (DeFelipe and Farinas, 1992); therefore, somatic AMPA receptors must be extrasynaptic. We wanted to extend these findings to dendritic AMPA receptors, because these can be in very close proximity to glutamatergic synapses and thus may well comprise a different pool than somatic receptors. We used focal photolysis of MNI-caged glutamate to activate dendritic receptors. Dendrites were identified by scanning across areas ∼100–500 μm surrounding the soma of the recorded cell until focal, EPSC-like, short-latency, reproducible responses were evoked. To ensure that direct responses could be unambiguously distinguished from synaptic responses, we uncaged glutamate directly onto the soma of 19 pyramidal cells and recorded action potentials in the cell-attached mode. No action potentials were detected within the first 3 ms of the uncaging stimulus (data not shown). Thus, for analysis of direct responses, we considered only responses that rose within 3 ms of the uncaging stimulus. Figure 4A shows an example of a layer 5 pyramidal cell from a P17 rat. The cell was filled with Alexa455-dextran, which allowed visualization under UV illumination (a different wavelength from that used to uncage MNI–glutamate). The top left trace in Figure 4A shows a direct dendritic response evoked by uncaging onto the apical dendrite ∼200 μm from the soma, and the top right trace shows that this response was abolished by moving the uncaging beam <30 μm laterally. The bottom trace shows a direct response evoked by uncaging onto the soma. It had a larger amplitude and a shorter latency but similar kinetics compared with the dendritic response. Amplitudes of direct dendritic responses were predicted to be graded because they do not depend on stochastic synaptic release, with the amount of uncaged glutamate proportional to the stimulation duration and MNI–glutamate concentration. We assessed whether the kinetics and amplitudes of direct dendritic responses vary with MNI–glutamate concentration and stimulation intensity, potentially as a result of local receptor saturation, desensitization, and/or diffusion of uncaged glutamate. Figure 4B1 shows a representative experiment in which a layer 5 pyramidal cell from a P18 rat was exposed to combinations of three MNI–glutamate concentrations (33, 100, and 300 μm) and four stimulation durations (900, 300, 200, and 100 μs). Decay constants (τ) and 50% rise times of evoked responses did not vary systematically under these conditions (top two panels), but the response amplitude increased with rising MNI–glutamate concentrations and with longer stimulation durations. Figure 4B2 shows example traces at indicated times during all combinations of conditions, demonstrating changing amplitudes (top, raw traces) but similar kinetics (bottom, superimposed normalized traces). Decay constants, 50% rise times, and amplitudes of direct dendritic responses from 10 pyramidal cells (both layers 2/3 and 5; 100 μm MNI–glutamate) are shown in Figure 4C1–C3, respectively, with stimulation durations ranging from 0.2 to 1 ms. Again, decay constants and 50% rise times did not vary systematically, but amplitudes increased with increasing stimulation durations. With even longer uncaging pulse durations (several milliseconds), response kinetics did slow down significantly (possibly because of overlapping stimulation and response times; data not shown). Only stimulation durations between 0.1 and 0.5 ms were used in subsequent experiments.
Characterization of direct dendritic responses. A, Composite image of a patched layer 5 pyramidal cell filled with Alexa455-dextran dye while recording in voltage clamp (patch pipette is to the right of the soma). The apical and two basal dendrites are visible. Circles indicate locations of focal MNI–glutamate uncaging (100 μm in bath solution). A large inward response was elicited by uncaging onto the soma (arrow indicates time of 100 μs laser pulse; bottom trace). A smaller response was obtained by uncaging onto the apical dendrite ∼200 μm from the soma (top left circle and trace; 200 μs laser pulse) but was abolished when the uncaging focus was moved ∼30 μm to the side of the dendrite (top right circle and trace; 200 μs laser pulse). B1, Kinetics of direct responses elicited from one spot on the apical dendrite in a layer 5 pyramidal cell of a P18 rat from a holding potential of −60 mV. MNI–glutamate (Glu) concentrations and stimulation durations are shown on top. The starting MNI–glutamate concentration of 33 μm was followed by washin of 100 and 300 μm MNI–glutamate. Stimulation durations were 900, 300, 100, and 200 μs. The bottom three panels show decay constants (τ), 50% rise times, and maximum amplitudes of responses. Amplitudes are dependent on stimulation durations and MNI–glutamate concentrations, whereas decay and rise times are not. B2, Top, Example traces recorded at time points a through f indicated above the x-axis in C1, representing all six concentration/duration combinations. Bottom, Scaled traces. Calibration: 50 ms, 50 pA. C1–C3, Decay constants (C1), 50% rise times (C2), and amplitudes (C3) of responses evoked from a holding potential of −60 mV at stimulation durations between 100 μs and 1 ms. Responses from 10 layer 2/3 and layer 5 pyramidal cells from P12 and P16 rats evoked by photolysis of 100 μm MNI–glutamate are shown. Amplitudes are approximately proportional to stimulation duration, whereas decay and rise times are unaffected. D, Rectification indices were measured at various distances from the soma in five pyramidal cells to assess the potential contributions of voltage-clamping artifacts and signal attenuation. Gray circles show individually measured rectification indices between 100 and 500 μm from the soma, and black circles show averaged data with 100 μm binning. Error bars indicate SEM.
Pyramidal neurons have extended dendritic arbors, which leads to filtering and attenuation of electrical signals. This could compromise voltage-clamp fidelity and lead to an underestimation of rectification indices, both for direct and for synaptic responses. Figure 4D shows rectification indices measured in mature layer 5 pyramidal cells evoked by direct dendritic stimulation at various distances from the soma (100–500 μm). Gray circles represent results from individual cells, whereas black circles show averaged data for 100 μm bins. Rectification indices remain constant within this range, indicating that voltage-clamping artifacts do not preclude determination of rectification indices for direct dendritic responses, originating within 0.5 mm of the soma and, by extension, for synaptic responses. Together, these results show that amplitudes of direct dendritic responses are suitable for determining I/V relationships.
GluR2-containing AMPA receptors predominate principal cell dendritic responses
We measured rectification indices in apical dendrites by direct stimulation of 65 layer 2/3 and 49 layer 5 pyramidal cells in slices from rats aged P8–P40. Figure 5A1 shows representative traces evoked by uncaging onto the apical dendrites of layer 2/3 and layer 5 pyramidal cells of a P8 and a P19 rat; the corresponding I/V curves are shown in Figure 5A2. Pyramidal cells in both layers and at both ages display slightly outwardly rectifying I/V curves, typical of GluR2-containing receptors (Washburn et al., 1997). Figure 5B shows all rectification indices obtained from layer 2/3 (open circles) and layer 5 (filled circles) cells. No significant differences were seen between immature and mature neurons in either layer or between the two layers (Table 1), although decay kinetics slowed in more mature cells (supplemental Fig. S1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). There was no difference in rectification indices evoked from basal and apical dendrites (P8–P12, layer 2/3: basal, 1.29 ± 0.09, n = 6; apical, 1.13 ± 0.07, n = 11; P8–P12, layer 5: basal, 1.35 ± 0.05, n = 4; apical, 1.26 ± 0.09, n = 9; p > 0.05). We also measured rectification indices in pyramidal cell somata, and in agreement with previous reports (Kumar et al., 2002), they were linear or slightly outwardly rectifying (data not shown). Dendritic rectification indices from layer 4 stellate cells of P6 animals were also slightly outwardly rectifying (rectification index, 1.47 ± 0.12; n = 4; data not shown). Whereas immature synapses of neocortical excitatory neurons express GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors, dendritic AMPA responses are dominated by GluR2-containing receptors.
GluR2-containing extrasynaptic AMPA receptors. A1, Representative direct dendritic responses evoked from holding potentials of −60, −30, 0, 30, and 60 mV in layer 2/3 (L2/3) and layer 5 (L5) pyramidal cells of animals aged P8 and P19. Responses were evoked by focal photolysis of caged glutamate onto a dendritic location at least 100 μm from the soma. Recordings were obtained with 50 μm spermine in the patch pipette. Calibration: 50 ms, 10 pA. A2, I/V curves derived from the traces shown in A1. Direct dendritic responses for layer 2/3 (white circles) and layer 5 (black circles) pyramidal cells in both age groups were normalized to the maximal amplitude at −60 mV. Diagonal lines were added to indicate that I/V curves were slightly outwardly rectifying. The inset shows inwardly rectifying dendritic responses at Vhold of −60, −30, 0, 30, and 60 for dendritic responses of a layer 5 fast spiking interneuron. The corresponding I/V curve is depicted by gray circles. B, Summary data for all dendritic rectification indices measured in layer 2/3 (L2/3; open circles) and layer 5 (L5; filled circles) cells for animals aged P8–P12, P13–P14, and P16–P40. There is no developmental switch in rectification, indicating prevalence of GluR2-containing dendritic AMPA receptors. Error bars indicate SEM.
Using the same glutamate uncaging protocol, we could reliably evoke inwardly rectifying responses from dendrites and somata of layer 5 fast spiking interneurons (dendrite: rectification index, 0.70 ± 0.04, n = 3; soma: rectification index, 0.56 ± 0.07, n = 6; age, P14–P15) (Fig. 5A2, inset). The linear I/V curves from immature pyramidal dendrites are thus neither an experimental artifact nor a universal feature of all neuronal dendrites. Rather, they appear to represent a cell-type-specific and developmentally regulated mechanism of AMPA receptor targeting.
NMDA/AMPA ratios are greater in dendrites than in synapses and decrease during development
To further explore developmental and cell-compartment-specific differences between dendritic and synaptic glutamate receptors, we examined the contribution of AMPA and NMDA receptors to EPSCs and direct dendritic responses (Fig. 6). Synaptic and dendritic responses were evoked in the same cells from membrane potentials of ×60 and +50 mV. At ×60 mV, NMDA receptors are blocked by extracellular Mg2+, but the magnesium block is relieved by depolarization. NMDA receptors activate and inactivate slowly, so that the peak response at +50 mV occurs at a time when AMPA receptor-mediated responses have mostly decayed (∼20 ms after stimulus) (Fig. 6A). In agreement with a recently published report on dendritic spines of layer 2/3 somatosensory cortex neurons (Busetto et al., 2008), we found a significantly higher NMDA/AMPA ratio in dendritic responses compared with EPSCs in cells from animals aged P9–P12 and P17–P18 both in layer 2/3 (Fig. 6B1) (P9–P12, n = 10: dendrite, 6.42 ± 0.97; synapse, 1.31 ± 0.19; p < 0.001; P17–P18, n = 9: dendrite, 2.34 ± 0.55; synapse, 0.97 ± 0.21; p < 0.05, paired t tests) and in layer 5b (Fig. 6B2) (P9–P12, n = 11: dendrite, 5.34 ± 0.99; synapse, 1.04 ± 0.16; p < 0.005; P17–P18, n = 11: dendrite, 2.83 ± 0.58; synapse, 0.54 ± 0.13; p < 0.01, paired t tests). Synaptic and dendritic NMDA/AMPA ratios decreased to similar degrees between P9–P12 and P17–P18 (Fig. 6C).
NMDA/AMPA ratios. A, Representative EPSCs (left) and direct dendritic responses (right) evoked from holding potentials of −60 and +50 mV in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells from animals aged P8 and P17. Traces were scaled to the maximal amplitude of the response at −60 mV (AMPA receptor-mediated response). Responses at +50 mV (NMDA receptor mediated) had slow kinetics and are larger in dendrites than at synapses. B, NMDA/AMPA ratios from layer 2/3 (B1) and layer 5 (B2) immature (P9–P12) and mature (P17–P18) pyramidal cell dendrites (Den.) and synapses (Syn.). Individual data (circles) and summary data (large rectangle; center line, median, 25–75%; small square, mean; whiskers, 5–95%). Synaptic NMDA/AMPA ratios in both age groups are significantly lower than dendritic ratios. C, NMDA/AMPA ratios decline during development, both in dendrites (Den.) and in synapses (Syn.). Synaptic and dendritic NMDA/AMPA ratios normalized to ratios at P9–P12 in layer 2/3 and layer 5 pyramidal cells. The decrease was statistically significant (p < 0.05), except for synapses in layer 2/3 cells. Error bars indicate SEM. *p < 0.01; **p < 0.001.
Discussion
Principal neurons in many regions of the CNS express GluR2-lacking synaptic AMPA receptors early in development and subsequently switch to GluR2-containing AMPA receptors. This has been shown for a number of neurons in rodents, including layer 5 pyramidal cells (Kumar et al., 2002), inner hair cell synapses (Eybalin et al., 2004), calyx of Held synapses (Joshi et al., 2004), and in the hippocampus (Ho et al., 2007). It seems to be a common developmental process that occurs not only in mammals but also in species such as zebrafish (Patten and Ali, 2007), chicken (Migues et al., 2007), and Xenopus (Aizenman et al., 2002). We show that the developmental switch from GluR2-lacking to GluR2-containing receptors also occurs in excitatory layer 2/3 and layer 4 neurons. We probed the presence of GluR2-lacking receptors using I/V curves, voltage-dependent unblocking, susceptibility of spontaneous EPSCs to blockade by extracellular NAS, and single-channel conductances determined by nonstationary fluctuation analysis. All results were consistent with the presence of GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors in immature synapses. We should point out that the immature synapses probed by us, with rectification indices of 0.5–0.6, likely contained a mixed, rather than pure, population of GluR2-lacking and GluR2-contianing receptors, whereas mature synapses predominantly contained GluR2-containing receptors. Kidd and Isaac (1999) have shown that thalamocortical synapses onto developing layer 4 stellate cells in the absence of intracellular spermine have mixed fast, linear AMPA receptor-mediated and slow, inwardly rectifying kainate receptor-mediated responses. We should note that (1) we did not strictly stimulate thalamocortical fibers, such that our stimulation protocol likely stimulated both thalamocortical and intracortical axons, and (2) the inwardly rectifying responses reported here may be, at least partially, kainate receptor mediated. Kainate receptors, like GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors, are calcium permeable and inwardly rectifying in the presence of intracellular polyamines (Kamboj et al., 1995).
Neocortical layers are formed in an inside-out manner, such that excitatory neurons of deep layers are born first and superficial neurons are born, and migrate to their final position, later (for review, see Kriegstein, 2005). However, the timing of layer formation is not reflected in the “canonical neocortical circuit”: thalamic inputs project mainly to layers 4 and 5a, these project primarily to layer 2/3, and layer 2/3 sends a large number of efferents to layer 5b (Angevine and Sidman, 1961; Staiger et al., 2000; Petersen and Sakmann, 2001; Thomson and Bannister, 2003; Wirth and Lüscher, 2004; Douglas and Martin, 2007; Lübke and Feldmeyer, 2007; Petersen, 2007). This is obviously a simplification, especially for layer 5b (there are extensive intralaminar projections, as well as afferents and efferents from other layers and other cortical areas). However, the basic circuit has been well established using laser-scanning photostimulation to map synaptic inputs to different layers (Dantzker and Callaway, 2000; Staiger et al., 2000; Schubert et al., 2001; Shepherd et al., 2003; Bureau et al., 2004), voltage-sensitive dye studies to image the spread of activation across layers (Petersen and Sakmann, 2001; Laaris and Keller, 2002), and paired recordings (Thomson and Bannister, 1998; Reyes and Sakmann, 1999; Feldmeyer et al., 2002; Thomson et al., 2002). The timing of the GluR2 switch (layer 4 → layer 2/3 → layer 5) corresponds to the position within the canonical neocortical circuit rather than the ontogenetic age of the layer. Layer 4 stellate cells, which receive input from thalamic relay nuclei and provide input to layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons, fail to exhibit strong inward rectification after P7, a time when thalamocortical inputs in the somatosensory cortex are already well established (Crair and Malenka, 1995; Isaac et al., 1997; Bannister et al., 2005; Inan et al., 2006). Timing of the GluR2 switch in layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons coincides with the critical period for developmental plasticity in somatosensory cortex (Stern et al., 2001). Not much is known about timing of critical period plasticity in layer 5b, but it is conceivable that it, too, coincides with network maturation and the GluR2 switch. Because we could not strictly stimulate only one set of afferent projections to each layer, and because the switch in layer 5b occurs at the same time regardless of whether callosal, intralaminar, or supragranular stimulation was used (Kumar et al., 2002; this study), we hypothesize that the GluR2 switch occurs at specific times in each cortical layer regardless of specific afferents.
GluR2-containing receptors could be more readily excluded from immature synapses by a specific active targeting mechanism. Alternatively, there could be a passive mechanism in which GluR2 expression slowly increases, creating a delay for GluR2-containing receptors to be trafficked into more and more distal synapses. If GluR2-lacking receptors slowly diffused “out into the dendrites,” the smaller size of layer 2/3 and layer 4 neurons could account for the earlier switch. We believe that our results argue for an active targeting mechanism. First, the linear I/V relationships of dendritic AMPA receptor-mediated responses in immature neurons indicate that GluR2-containing receptors are present at locations up to 0.5 mm from the soma, such that a significant proportion of synaptic AMPA receptors of immature neurons are GluR2 lacking, whereas dendritic receptors in the same cells are not. Second, as shown in Figure 4D, there was no correlation between rectification indices and distance of glutamate release from the soma. Finally, GluR2 and the postsynaptic glutamatergic marker protein PSD95 showed a greater degree of immunohistochemical colocalization in cortical sections from more mature animals, although GluR2-positive puncta are clearly present earlier. In addition, previous reports have consistently shown that trafficking of distinct AMPA receptor subtypes into and out of synapses is tightly regulated and essential in responding to changes in synaptic activity (for review, see Sprengel, 2006; Greger and Esteban, 2007; Isaac et al., 2007).
We demonstrate that focal photolysis of caged glutamate is suitable to probe extrasynaptic receptors in intact neocortical neurons in brain slices because it does not appear to activate significant proportions of synaptic receptors. This might result from several factors, for example the anatomy of the synaptic cleft, which may not allow efficient access to exogenously released glutamate, efficient glial glutamate uptake might occur in the vicinity of synapses, or extrasynaptic receptors might greatly outnumber synaptic receptors.
Extrasynaptic AMPA receptors can serve as a reserve pool for the experience- and activity-dependent modulation of synaptic receptor populations (Adesnik et al., 2005), but there may be more direct roles in regulating dendritic excitability. Extrasynaptic glutamate receptors have been reported to sense and react to extracellular glutamate levels via synaptic spillover (Asztely et al., 1997) and autoexcitation (Nicoll and Jahr, 1982) and to be involved in glia-to-neuron signaling (for review, see (Fellin et al., 2006). AMPA receptors desensitize quickly, have low glutamate affinities compared with NMDA receptors, and are thus unlikely to be major effectors for slowly released glutamate at concentrations much lower than those found in the synaptic cleft. At present, only very few reports indicate a direct physiological role for nonsynaptic AMPA receptors, for example in lateral excitation of mitral cells (Urban and Sakmann, 2002) or in neuron-to-glia signaling between Bergmann glia and climbing fiber and parallel fiber synapses (Clark and Barbour, 1997).
Additionally, we characterized NMDA/AMPA ratios, which display striking differences between dendritic and synaptic compartments. We found a much larger NMDA component in dendrites, supporting the notion that NMDA receptors play a more prominent role in extrasynaptic glutamate signaling (for review, see Vizi and Mike, 2006). We also shown that the NMDA/AMPA ratio decreased by similar degrees in dendrites and synapses during development. This is in agreement with a recently published report using two-photon glutamate uncaging onto dendritic spines of layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons, in which the authors found a decrease in NMDA/AMPA ratios around P12, possibly caused by a decrease in silent (i.e., AMPA receptor-lacking) synapses (Busetto et al., 2008). Again, the timing of this NMDA/AMPA ratio decrease coincides with the GluR2 switch. NMDA receptors and GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors are calcium permeable, suggesting that both developmental switches limit calcium influx into maturing pyramidal cells.
In this study, we have expanded on findings that describe a developmental switch in neocortical principal neurons from GluR2-lacking to GluR2-containing synaptic AMPA receptors. Simultaneously, synaptic and dendritic NMDA/AMPA ratios of pyramidal cells are downregulated. We also validate a method to activate dendritic, extrasynaptic receptors by focal photolysis of caged glutamate and show that dendritic receptors in immature cortical neurons differ from synaptic receptors at similar distances from the soma: dendritic receptors are GluR2 containing, and synaptic receptors are GluR2 lacking. Thus, neocortical synaptic maturation coincides with a shift in AMPA receptor targeting, which proceeds sequentially from layer 4 to layer 2/3 to layer 5b.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS12151 (J.R.H.) and an American Epilepsy Society postdoctoral fellowship (J.B.). We thank Isabel Parada for expert assistance with immunostaining.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. John R. Huguenard, Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, 300 Pasteur Drive, Room M016, Stanford, CA 94305. john.huguenard{at}stanford.edu