Abstract
Prism adaptation does not only induce short-term sensorimotor plasticity, but also longer-term reorganization in the neural representation of space. We used event-related fMRI to study dynamic changes in brain activity during both early and prolonged exposure to visual prisms. Participants performed a pointing task before, during, and after prism exposure. Measures of trial-by-trial pointing errors and corrections allowed parametric analyses of brain activity as a function of performance. We show that during the earliest phase of prism exposure, anterior intraparietal sulcus was primarily implicated in error detection, whereas parieto-occipital sulcus was implicated in error correction. Cerebellum activity showed progressive increases during prism exposure, in accordance with a key role for spatial realignment. This time course further suggests that the cerebellum might promote neural changes in superior temporal cortex, which was selectively activated during the later phase of prism exposure and could mediate the effects of prism adaptation on cognitive spatial representations.
Introduction
Sensorimotor plasticity can be induced experimentally in healthy subjects using optical prisms that produce a misalignment between the perceived location of visual and proprioceptive information. Under prisms, initial pointing movements are typically deviated toward the virtual (perceived) target position. These errors constitute the direct effect of prisms. Their correction is thought to depend on a short-latency “strategic” process that is activated by deviations from intended plans (error feedback; Redding and Wallace, 1996). In general, terminal errors are successfully corrected after a series of 10–15 trials only. Behavioral research suggests that the terminal error on a given trial may be used to correct movement on the next trial (Rossetti et al., 1993). However, further pointing movements are necessary to obtain a sustained realignment of visual and proprioceptive spatial coordinates. This is a “true” adaptation (Weiner et al., 1983) that can be demonstrated by a compensatory shift opposite to the prism deviation when subjects are asked to point straight-ahead after removal of prisms (so-called after-effects) (Redding et al., 2005).
The neural substrates underlying prism adaptation are of great importance to understand sensorimotor plasticity. Furthermore, improvement of spatial neglect after prism adaptation in stroke patients (Rossetti et al., 1998) and generalization of adaptation effects to other visuo-motor (Colent et al., 2000; Michel et al., 2003; Michel et al., 2007) or non-visuomotor tasks (Striemer et al., 2006) in healthy subjects have generated a major theoretical interest because they suggest that visuomotor adaptation might modulate higher-level processes involved in spatial cognition.
A recent model (Pisella et al., 2006b) proposed that prism adaptation might critically depend on the cerebellum, in keeping with the lack of adaptation observed in patients with cerebellar lesions (Weiner et al., 1983; Pisella et al., 2005), whereas the subsequent cognitive effects might involve the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) via bottom-up signals generated by the cerebellum. An important role for PPC is also suggested by functional neuroimaging (Clower et al., 1996). In this pioneer study, PET scans were performed in healthy volunteers while prismatic deviation was reversed every five trials (left vs right), and revealed selective activation in PPC during pointing with prisms (vs without prisms), independent of the shift direction. However, because deviation was repeatedly changed, this study captured only the strategic short-term correction of visuo-motor errors before any sustained realignment of sensorimotor coordinates could be established. Another preliminary PET study on pointing with versus without prisms reported activation in the lateral cerebellum, plus ventrolateral thalamus and prefrontal cortex (Zeffiro, 1995). Sensori-motor plasticity has also been investigated using visuo-motor rotation paradigms (Inoue et al., 1997; Imamizu et al., 2000; Graydon et al., 2005; Seidler et al., 2006; Girgenrath et al., 2008), but the large magnitude of visual rotation (≥90°) in these studies may involve high-level strategic control processes that are distinct from pure spatial realignment mechanisms concerned by prism displacement (Redding et al., 2005). Although one study (Diedrichsen et al., 2005) used a small rotation (25°, closer to prismatic shift), the direction was regularly reversed (clockwise or counterclockwise) so that the slow spatial realignment component of adaptation could not develop.
Here we examine the neural bases of sustained prism exposure with the aims to capture both the early strategic component of prism adaptation and the more progressive realignment process. We used an event-related fMRI design allowing a dynamic investigation of neural activity over the time course of prism exposure and adaptation.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
We examined 14 healthy volunteers, 7 women and 7 men. Three subjects had to be excluded from the analyses because of technical problems during acquisition (two failed to correctly hear auditory instructions and behavioral responses of the third one were missing). All tested subjects (age range 19–28, mean 23) were right handed as assessed by the Edinburgh inventory (Oldfield, 1971). Eight of these subjects were right eye dominant and three left eye dominant as determined by standard methods. All participants had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. They all gave informed consent according to the local ethical committee regulation.
Methods.
Our paradigm included all three classical phases of prism adaptation: (1) preexposure baseline; (2) active exposure to prismatic deviation over a prolonged period; (3) postexposure compensation or “de-adaptation” (Redding et al., 2005). Participants had to point to visual targets with their right hand, across four consecutive fMRI runs: either without prism (runs 1 and 4), or with prism (runs 2 and 3) that shifted their vision 10° leftward (Fig. 1). Event-related fMRI responses were obtained for each pointing trial in each condition over the whole course of adaptation (see Experimental procedures). By comparing pointing during different runs, we could distinguish the different phases of prismatic adaptation, including short-term strategic corrections and “realignment,” as well as any activity related to the de-adaptation phase.
Participants were installed in the MRI scanner with a particular set-up allowing a slight anterior flexion of the head position to allow a direct vision of the target display in front of the scanner bore, with a very strict, although comfortable, fixation of head and shoulders to prevent systematic pointing-related movements (see Fig. 1b). Prismatic goggles were specifically made for this experiment using a point-to-point prismatic lens (Optiquepeter.com). The main advantage of these prisms, compared with Fresnel prisms, is the absence of any optical deformation of the environment perceived by the subject, which is thought to favor low-level sensori-motor spatial realignment (true adaptation) instead of higher-order strategic control. They were placed in front of the participant's eyes and attached to their forehead and to the headcoil. They were composed of a MRI compatible cardboard frame and two transparent glasses: one neutral (without any deviation) and one prismatic (producing a 10° leftward shift of the visual wide-field). Position of the prism on the goggles was randomly assigned across participants (7 subjects had the prism over the right eye, 4 over the left eye). In addition, an opaque patch was put in front of the goggles' frame to allow monocular occlusion of either the right or left eye. This patch was controlled by a mechanical device such that it could be moved from one eye to the other during scanning without moving the goggles or the head. The patch was placed over the eye with the prism during runs 1 and 4 (i.e., neutral vision conditions), and over the nonprismatic neutral glass during runs 2 and 3 (i.e., prism exposure conditions).
The experiment began with two brief training sessions of pointing without prisms, outside and inside the scanner. Then participants performed 4 fMRI runs (∼7 min for run 1 and 4 and ∼6 min for run 2 and 3). Runs 1 and 4 (neutral vision) consisted of 4 blocks of pointing in alternation with 4 blocks of clicking.
The pointing task required subjects to reach (with the tip of their right index) to one of two possible targets, located on the left or right side of a touch-sensitive display in front of the scanner bore. The targets were two red points horizontally aligned on either side of the display (∼10° from center) and remained visible throughout the task. On each trial, the target to reach was indicated by an auditory command (the word “left” or “right”) through earphones, in a random sequence (3 left, 3 right in each block). To ensure optimal adaptation, subjects were trained to make pointing movements as fast and accurately as possible in a single movement, with a starting hand position on their umbilicus, occluded from sight; and then to return to this position immediately after touching the target. In the clicking task, subjects were required to press (with their right index) on either the right or left button of a response-pad, fixed on their umbilicus. Instructions were identical to the pointing task, with the same auditory commands (“left” or “right”) given by earphones in a random order (3 clicks on either side in each block).
Each task was repeated four times in each of these runs (1 and 4), resulting in 24 pointing trials (12 to the left target, 12 to the right) and 24 clicking trials (12 on the left key, 12 on the right) per run. Runs 2 and 3 (prism exposure) consisted of 4 blocks of pointing trials, alternating with short rest periods without pointing. Pointing trials were exactly identical to the other runs (see above) except for the presence of the prism. Each block included 3 right and 3 left targets, in random order. At the end of prism exposure (run 3), two adaptation test trials were performed, in which the goggles were changed to the neutral position (with the patch over the prism), and subjects were instructed to look and then point to one of the visual targets with their eyes closed. These instructions were given by earphones, without interrupting fMRI acquisition, with the following sequence: “Look at the left (or right) target; close your eyes; and now point.” Participants made one pointing movement to the left target, and one to the right, in random order.
Pointing accuracy was recorded on each trial by comparing the end-position of the movement relative to the target position on the visual display. The latter consisted of a touch-sensitive surface, made with a thin soft plastic board overlaying a PC keyboard adapted for MRI compatibility. Individual keys were not visible to avoid any visual cues other than the two color targets. Pointing errors were measured in terms of the distance between the keys touched at the end of the movement and the key corresponding to the correct target position. This error was subsequently transformed in visual angle, with a deviation of 1 key (2 cm) corresponding to ∼2° (given a distance between the subject's eyes and visual display of ∼55 cm). A more precise spatial discrimination of errors was obtained by recording all keys simultaneously pressed by the subject, and by calculating the final end-position as the average key coordinates (which yielded a resolution of 0.5° in the horizontal axis given the lateral shift of keys between rows). The size of terminal pointing errors allowed us to derive different behavioral indices for each pointing trial, used as independent predictor in our parametric analyses of fMRI data (see below). The error size index was determined as the absolute magnitude of terminal error on a given trial (i.e., with negative to positive values for leftward to rightward deviations, respectively). The error correction index was determined as the amplitude of pointing adjustment occurring between the preceding trial and the current trial (error correction on trial n = amplitude of the terminal error on trial n − 1 minus the terminal error on trial n). Error correction was set to zero for the first trial of each pointing block given that no correction could occur in this case.
MRI scanning.
MRI data were acquired in a 1.5-T whole-body INTERA system (Philips Medical Systems), using the standard head-coil configuration. For each participant, structural images were acquired with a 3-D-GRE T1-weighted sequence (FOV = 250 mm, TR/TE/Flip = 15 ms/5.0 ms/30°; matrix = 256 × 256, slice thickness = 1.25 mm) and functional images with a GRE EPI sequence (TR/TE/Flip = 2500 ms/40 ms/80°, FOV = 250 mm, matrix = 128 × 128). Each functional image comprised 30 contiguous 4.5 mm axial slices (2 × 2 mm in plane), oriented parallel to the inferior edge of the occipital and temporal lobes, and covering the whole brain from cerebellum to parietal cortex. For the main experiment, a total of 636 functional images were acquired in four scanning runs, each separated by a very brief pause, with an effective repetition time of 2.5 s.
Data analysis.
Images were processed and analyzed using SPM2 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience). All functional volumes were realigned to the first volume (Friston et al., 1995a), spatially normalized to a standard template with a resampled voxel size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm, and smoothed using a Gaussian Kernel with a full width at half maximum (FWHM) of 8 mm. To remove low-frequency drifts from the data, high-pass temporal filtering with a cutoff of 128 s was applied. Statistical analysis was performed using the general linear model (Friston et al., 1995b) for event-related designs as implemented in SPM2. In our main analysis, each pointing and clicking trial was modeled by convolving a δ function at the trial onset (i.e., auditory command) with a canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF). Error size and error correction measured on each pointing trial were included in the same design matrix as additional parametric regressors, also convolved with the HRF. Because these two regressors were effectively orthogonalized in SPM analysis, we could obtain a measure of their independent contribution to the neural responses on a trial-by-trial basis in each subject. The orthogonality between error and correction parameters was confirmed by computing the absolute cosine values of their angle for each participant: mean 0.0676 (SD = 0.0244) and 0.0778 (SD = 0.0406) for runs 2 and 3, respectively (values close to zero indicate orthogonality, whereas values close to 1 indicate colinearity), corresponding to nonsignificant correlation coefficients between these two parametric regressors (r < 1, p > 0.27 for both runs). In addition, residual effects of head motion were corrected by including the six estimated motion parameters for each subject as regressors of no interest in the design matrix.
The general linear model was then used to generate parameter estimates of activity at each voxel, for each regressor and each participant. Contrasts images were calculated by applying appropriate linear contrasts to the parameters estimates for the regressors of each event. These contrast images were then entered into one-sample t tests across the 11 participants separately to perform random-effects group analyses. All statistical analyses were performed on a voxelwise basis across the whole brain. We report results at p < 0.001 uncorrected for multiple comparisons and cluster size >10. Location of activations in the cerebral cortex was determined on the average brain template of the 11 subjects, according to the stereotaxic coordinate system of the standard MNI template; location of activations in the brainstem and cerebellum was determined according to Schmahmann's atlas (Schmahmann et al., 1999).
In a complementary SPM analysis, to estimate the time course of activity on a trial-by-trial basis, we modeled individual pointing trials during prism exposure (runs 2 and 3) as separate events in a new design-matrix, including again motion parameters of no interest, and then applied the general linear model to generate new parameter estimates of activity at each voxel, for each regressor and each participant. These parameters were pooled for left and right targets and averaged for pairs of successive trials to smooth the time course curves. Beta values (i.e., parameter estimates of neural activity corresponding to each regressor in the general linear model) obtained with this analysis were extracted and averaged from clusters of three specific regions-of-interest (ROIs) that were previously identified in our main SPM analysis (antIPS, POS, and cerebellum).
Results
Behavioral performance
Pointing accuracy was determined by the distance between the target and terminal location on the touch-pad. We compared the different pointing conditions by ANOVA on the mean terminal deviation using adaptation phase (runs 1–4) as factor. As can be seen in Figure 1c, the main effect of phase was highly significant [F(3,33) = 39.04; p < 0.0001], with Scheffé post hoc tests showing a difference between run 2 and run 4 relative to run 1 (both p < 0.0002). There was no significant difference between run 3 and run 1, confirming that terminal position returned to baseline values (correct target location) during run 3.
To assess the time course of error correction, we also performed trial-by-trial one-sample t tests comparing the average end-position of each pointing (left and right trials together) with the real position of the target (reaching the latter corresponding to zero deviation). This showed no significant deviation during run 1. During run 2, the maximum pointing deviation was observed on the first trial (mean deviation = 1.7° to the left from the real target position). The end-position of the first 12 trials plus the 1eighth trial were significantly different from the real target position, whereas the last 6 trials of run 2 and all subsequent trials of run 3 were not different from the real target position. In run 4, the end-position was again significantly different from correct position for all trials except the last two, demonstrating a sustained compensatory effect after prism exposure.
In addition, a direct measure of adaptation strength was obtained at the end of prism exposure by test trials performed without prism, but eyes closed (see Materials and Methods) (Fig. 1c). These test trials confirmed a reliable sustained adaptation, with highly significant rightward shift (mean deviation = 2.5° opposite to the optical deviation) between end-position and real target position (i.e., clear after-effects).
Brain imaging results
Control of visuo-motor actions: pointing > clicking
We first identified brain regions implicated in the visual guidance of motor actions by contrasting all pointing trials made without prisms, relative to all clicking trials performed in the same runs (pointing run 1 + pointing run 4 > clicking run 1 + clicking run 4). Thus, any general cognitive processes activated by instructions, target selection, and/or motor decisions were canceled out in this contrast. This analysis identified a network of regions implicated in motor and proprioceptive control, including the contralateral primary motor cortex, overlapping with an area associated with elbow movements (Alkadhi et al., 2002); cerebellum (vermis and both hemispheres); bilateral supplementary motor area (SMA); and contralateral somatosensory cortex (Table 1). Activations were also observed in superior parietal regions implicated in visuo-motor action (Buneo and Andersen, 2006) including the posterior parieto-occipital sulcus (POS) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS). These regions were used to define a functional mask (at p < 0.05) of regions of interest (ROIs), specifically associated with visuo-motor control, which we could then test for changes during prism adaptation in subsequent analyses (see below).
Sustained exposure to prism deviation: prism > neutral
Brain regions recruited during the whole period of pointing under exposure to prismatic displacement were identified by contrasting all pointing trials in runs with prisms, relative to the first run without prism (i.e., [pointing runs 2 + 3] > [pointing run 1] ×2). This revealed selective activation in both the right and left superior temporal gyrus (STG) extending to the posterior-superior temporal sulci (STS) (Table 2, Fig. 2). When using an inclusive mask of ROIs previously defined as specific to visuomotor actions (i.e., pointing > clicking, see above), no significant overlap was found with the activated loci in STG/STS. This result argues against a role of these areas for the direct visuo-motor command of pointing, but rather suggests a contribution to higher nonmotor function that can be modified by prism adaptation; see review in Michel (2006). Moreover, when comparing activation in these temporal areas across the two successive prism runs, both the left and right STG/STS showed a further increase for pointing during run 3 compared with run 2 (Fig. 2) (T >4.93, p < 0.001 for both sides). This result further supports the hypothesis that activation within the STG/STS is not directly associated with sensorimotor plasticity but rather reflects higher-order processes induced by prism adaptation.
Exposure to prism deviation: early versus late phase
To determine brain areas more specifically implicated during the early stage of prism exposure, we contrasted pointing trials performed during the second versus the third fMRI runs (pointing in run 2 > pointing in run 3, all executed in presence of the exact same prismatic displacement) (Fig. 1). This comparison identified a network of areas including the right cerebellum hemisphere (lobule 4 and 5), the right superior parietal lobule (SPL) extending to the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and the left posterior parietal cortex extending to IPS and inferior parietal lobule (IPL) (Table 2, Fig. 3). Among these regions, only activation in the right cerebellum overlapped with the ROI mask previously defined as specific to visuomotor actions (pointing > clicking), suggesting a role in both the visuo-motor control of pointing and the early exposure to prism deviation. Unlike STG/STS, none of these parietal and cerebellar regions showed any progressive increases in activation during pointing in run 3 relative to run 2, but a significant reduction instead.
We corroborated these data by performing an additional parametric analysis based on the magnitude of terminal pointing errors, which progressively decreased over successive trials during prism exposure (runs 2 and 3). This analysis tested for any voxels across the whole brain in which event-related activity during pointing showed a linear parametric modulation as a function of deviation magnitude, therefore allowing us to identify regions in which activity covaried either positively or negatively with error magnitude on each individual trial (see Materials and Methods) (Table 2, Fig. 4a). A significant positive correlation (i.e., voxels with activity increasing in proportion to the magnitude of deviation) across all successive trials under prism (runs 2 and 3) was found in the left anterior intraparietal sulcus (antIPS). Another activation was observed in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (x, y, z = −2, 30, 22, p < 0.001, but with a cluster size below our threshold of 10 voxels). However, a significant negative correlation (i.e., voxels with activity decreasing in proportion to the magnitude of deviation) across all successive trials in runs 2 and 3 was found in the left parieto-occipital sulcus (POS). This cluster was located on the supero-anterior bank of the sulcus. When comparing these regions with the ROI mask previously defined by specific visuo-motor activity (pointing > clicking), only the left POS (negative covariation) showed a significant overlap, suggesting a direct role in visuo-motor control.
Because the correlation between parietal activation and error size during adaptation might reflect neural processes engaged by either detection or correction of errors (both being greater during early than late stages of prism exposure), our parametric analysis also included an index of error correction, defined as the difference in error size on trial n relative to the preceding trial n − 1 (see Materials and Methods), in addition to the absolute error size index used above. For both antIPS and POS clusters identified above, we compared the values of these two different parametric regressors, estimating the modulation of activity during pointing as a function of either error magnitude or correction magnitude on the current trial (as determined by separate parametric factors in a single SPM analysis). This comparison revealed that activity in left POS was primarily modulated by error correction on the current trial, more than by error size; whereas conversely, activity in left antIPS was modulated by error size more than by correction (Fig. 4b). In other words, a modulation of POS by behavioral performance was better accounted for by error correction than by error size on a given trial; and conversely a modulation of antIPS by performance was better accounted for by error size than by error correction. ANOVA on the parametric estimates obtained for each modulation type (error size and correction) and each region confirmed a highly significant interaction between modulation type and region [F(1,20) = 21.35; p < 0.001]. These data provide further evidence that the positive covariation of antIPS activity with pointing errors during prism exposure (Fig. 4a) is related to the detection of deviation, whereas the negative covariation of POS with pointing errors is directly related to the correction of deviation.
Trial-by-trial analysis of adaptation
Given the dynamic nature of adaptation, we performed a separate analysis to calculate parameter estimates of cerebral activity on a trial-by-trial basis, and thus determine more precisely the time course of changes in specific regions of interest (ROIs). In this complementary analysis, individual pointing trials during prism exposure in runs 2 and 3 (averaged across pairs of left and right targets) were modeled by separate regressors in SPM (see Materials and Methods). Parameter estimates were then extracted for these successive trials from ROIs in the left antIPS and left POS (which were defined by our previous parametric analysis on error size), as well as in the right cerebellum (which was activated in the first run of prism exposure, see above). The time course of activation in these three regions is plotted in Figure 5, and shows differential activation across the successive trials performed during the first run of prism exposure (while error and correction of the error took place). During the first 12 trials, antIPS progressively decreased its activity and POS progressively increased activity, before both regions reached a relative plateau. Whereas activity in POS then remained relatively stable between trials 12–18, antIPS showed further fluctuations until trials 18–20 before declining more completely. The right cerebellum showed a more progressive increase over trials, reaching a later peak around trial 16, and then gradually decreased during the last 6 trials. The time course of these activations were parallel to the pattern of deviation and correction in pointing performance over the same trials, because the greatest improvement in accuracy took place from trial 1–12 and returned to a steady baseline performance after trial 18 (Fig. 5) (see Behavioral results).
De-adaptation
Finally, we tested for brain regions involved in the de-adaptation process (after-effects), after removal of the prism after successful adaptation (Table 2). By contrasting pointing trials without prism in the last run relative to the first run (run 4 > run 1), we found selective increases in the left inferior parietal lobule extending to the antIPS, close to areas activated during early versus late phase of prism exposure (Fig. 3d) as well as those showing a positive correlation with pointing errors in parametric analysis above (Fig. 4a). This is consistent with the marked deviation errors observed behaviorally during initial pointing trials in the de-adaptation run (Fig. 3e).
Discussion
Our study reveals for the first time the neural substrates of prism adaptation in healthy subjects, by tracking dynamic changes in behavior and brain activity over the course of both sustained prism exposure and de-adaptation. Several distinct areas were activated, reflecting specific processes involved in prism adaptation, from initial pointing errors to successful spatial realignment, as well as possible influences on spatial cognition.
We should note that our paradigm implied an alternation of monocular vision across pointing conditions (one eye with prisms, runs 1 and 4; vs the other eye without prisms, runs 2 and 3). However, only one contrast (Prism > Neutral) was potentially affected by this bias, and the bilateral and symmetrical activation (in superior temporal cortex) revealed by this comparison is unlikely to be explained by monocular alternation.
Posterior parietal cortex
In posterior parietal cortex (PPC), the left parieto-occipital sulcus (POS) and left anterior intraparietal sulcus (antIPS) showed a distinct profile of activation suggesting separate roles during prism exposure. We found that activity in POS was related to both reaching and correction magnitude on the current trial. Thus, POS activity was low in the first prism trials (with large terminal errors), and then progressively increased until errors became reduced (∼10 trials). This region was already reported during reaching (Connolly et al., 2003; Medendorp et al., 2003) or visuo-motor tracking (Diedrichsen et al., 2005). Moreover, parieto-occipital lesions typically produce optic ataxia, an inability to point to visual targets in peripheral vision (Karnath and Perenin, 2005; Pisella et al., 2006a). Together, these data indicate that POS might be responsible for adjusting movement plans based on new visual inputs after displacement by prism, and thus underlie successful correction during the early phase of prism exposure. A neighboring region was also selectively activated in a delayed mental rotation task (Suchan et al., 2002). This result supports the hypothesis that POS may compute the correction for the next trial through a deliberate mental translation (Rossetti et al., 1993), and hence contribute to the strategic component of prism adaptation.
In contrast, the left antIPS was activated in direct proportion to pointing deviation, as demonstrated by a positive parametric correlation between the trial-by-trial error size and activity in this region; whereas there was no correlation with the magnitude of correction on the current trial (unlike POS). These data suggest a crucial role for this region in the error detection process, consistent with a previous PET study reporting similar activation during a reaching task with jumping targets (Desmurget et al., 2001). Our results are also consistent with PET results showing a selective activation of IPL during pointing while prisms were reversed (left to right) every five trials (Clower et al., 1996). However, the latter design was likely to induce a state of continuous error production (similar to the early prism exposure and final de-adaptation phases of our study). Our new data suggest that this activation may not underlie true adaptation per se (i.e., realignment of visual-proprioceptive coordinates), but only the error detection process necessary for the initial strategic corrections. This interpretation is also compatible with a case study showing impairment in error correction during prism exposure, but preserved adaptation, after bilateral parietal lesion (Pisella et al., 2004).
Finally, an activation in right intraparietal cortex was also observed during the early exposure phase, when comparing the first versus second runs of pointing with prisms. Given the major role of right PPC in visuo-spatial processing and attention (Mesulam, 1999), this finding may further argue for a visuo-spatial component in the initial error signal generated by prism exposure.
Cerebellum
Activation of lobules IV and V in the right cerebellum was also greater in the first run of prism exposure, relative to the second run, and overlapped with visuo-motor activity associated with pointing (relative to clicking). This is highly consistent with an implication of this region in visually directed movements (Stein, 1986; Bursztyn et al., 2006) and eye–hand coordination (Miall et al., 2000). A similar activation was found in the late stage of a visual rotation tracking task (Imamizu et al., 2000), and attributed to the acquisition of a new internal model after visual perturbation. The cerebellum involvement in prism adaptation is further supported by lesion-studies in both monkey (Baizer et al., 1999) and man (Weiner et al., 1983; Martin et al., 1996; Pisella et al., 2005). Interestingly, our trial-by-trial analysis revealed that cerebellum activity increased progressively during the early phase of prism exposure, and remained high for a longer period than the initial rapid improvement in pointing accuracy.
Although our trial-by-trial analysis is essentially descriptive and must be taken with caution because of inherent variability of fMRI signal, this dynamic profile in key regions of the cerebellum (and parietal cortex) shows a remarkable convergence between behavioral and neural data.
We suggest that the cerebellum is particularly involved in the realignment process that is necessary to re-establish a correct spatial mapping among visuo-motor and sensorimotor coordinates systems, so as to adapt to the optical shift. This process, which may result in the acquisition of a new internal model, is slower than the initial (more strategic) error correction and requires more pointing movements to become established (Redding et al., 2005). The relatively prolonged increases in cerebellar activity during prism exposure (until a stable correction of pointing errors) seem consistent with a critical function providing bottom-up signals to cortical areas responsible for the cognitive effects of prism adaptation.
Superior temporal cortex
Another major finding of our study was the selective and bilateral activation of the superior temporal sulcus (STS), extending into the superior temporal gyrus, associated with sustained prism exposure. STS increases arose only during the later stage, when pointing deviation was fully compensated and errors completely abolished. Such activity was further enhanced in the second relative to the first run with prisms, but unrelated to trial-by-trial adjustment of performance.
This result suggests an important role for STS/STG in the slow realignment process and longer-lasting changes induced by prisms. Whereas a previous study of medial-temporal lobectomy patients showed preserved prism adaptation (Canavan et al., 1990), our results provide the first direct evidence for a major role of lateral temporal regions in long-term effects of prism adaptation that might underlie changes in spatial cognition. Studies in monkeys have shown that STS and posterior-superior temporal regions receive multimodal inputs and make important contributions to cross-modal integration (Bruce et al., 1981). Thus, STS/STG is ideally suited to link multiple sensorimotor signals in a common spatial register.
These data provide new support for an implication of the superior temporal lobe in spatial cognition (Karnath et al., 2001), an issue that has been intensely debated in recent years (Halligan et al., 2003). Lesion data suggest that right temporal damage may be significantly more frequent in patients with left spatial neglect than in patients without neglect (Karnath et al., 2004), with a maximal lesion overlap being remarkably close to the right STS activation found in our study (x, y, z = 64–24 8 vs 70–20 4, respectively). We therefore conclude that the progressive increase of STS activity during prism exposure might reflect changes in a cross-modal or supramodal system that could subserve the effects of prism adaptation on spatial cognition, although other target areas may potentially also contribute (Striemer et al., 2008).
Implications for theories of visuomotor adaptation and spatial cognition
By identifying the neural networks underlying different components of prism adaptation and their dynamical time course, our results provide new insights on the mechanisms by which visuo-motor plasticity may interact with spatial cognition. Based on our results, we propose that the initial short-term strategic processes implicate error detection signals in anterior IPS and error correction plans in POS; whereas the slower realignment processes implicate the cerebellum at which different spatial maps for eye and hand sensorimotor coordinates can be integrated and realigned. In contrast, the more delayed activity observed within STS/STG could mediate the cognitive effects of prism adaptation. The latter activation might be modulated by sustained activity in the cerebellum. Hence, our results support the hypothesis that prism adaptation can modulate cortical areas implicated in spatial cognition through a bottom-up signal generated in the cerebellum (Rode et al., 2003; Pisella et al., 2006b), albeit a key role of temporal cortex was previously not suspected. Anatomical connections between the temporal cortex and the cerebellum remain poorly known but might participate to cognitive functions associated with cerebellum (Ramnani, 2006).
How sensorimotor plasticity induced by prism exposure can improve spatial neglect remains largely unknown. Our results add new support to the hypothesis that such therapeutic effects may operate by recruiting or inhibiting intact cortical areas (Pisella et al., 2006b). Among these areas, the temporal cortex of the intact left hemisphere may be responsible for improving spatial output subsequent to prism adaptation. This hypothesis is consistent with a recent PET study in neglect patients (Luauté et al., 2006), in which changes in left temporal cortex were correlated with neglect improvement after prism adaptation. Furthermore, delayed activation in temporal areas, as observed here, may contribute to consolidate these new spatial representations and explain why the improvement of spatial neglect increased after a delay of 2 h after prism exposure (Rossetti et al., 1998).
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by grants from Inserm and La Société Française de Médecine Physique et de Réadaptation to J.L., and Swiss National Science Foundation to P.V. (No. 3200B0-114014).
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Jacques Luauté, Service de Rééducation Neurologique, Mouvement et Handicap, Hospices Civils de Lyon, Hôpital Henry Gabrielle, 69230 Saint Genis Laval, France. jacques.luaute{at}chu-lyon.fr