Abstract
The human brain has the remarkable capacity to alter in response to environmental demands. Training-induced structural brain changes have been demonstrated in the healthy adult human brain. However, no study has yet directly related structural brain changes to behavioral changes in the developing brain, addressing the question of whether structural brain differences seen in adults (comparing experts with matched controls) are a product of “nature” (via biological brain predispositions) or “nurture” (via early training). Long-term instrumental music training is an intense, multisensory, and motor experience and offers an ideal opportunity to study structural brain plasticity in the developing brain in correlation with behavioral changes induced by training. Here we demonstrate structural brain changes after only 15 months of musical training in early childhood, which were correlated with improvements in musically relevant motor and auditory skills. These findings shed light on brain plasticity and suggest that structural brain differences in adult experts (whether musicians or experts in other areas) are likely due to training-induced brain plasticity.
Introduction
Studies comparing adult musicians with matched nonmusicians have revealed structural and functional differences in musically relevant brain regions such as sensorimotor brain areas (Elbert et al., 1995; Hund-Georgiadis and von Cramon, 1999; Schlaug, 2001; Gaser and Schlaug, 2003b), auditory areas (Pantev et al., 1998; Zatorre, 1998; Schneider et al., 2002; Gaab and Schlaug, 2003; Bermudez and Zatorre, 2005; Lappe et al., 2008), and multimodal integration areas (Münte et al., 2001; Sluming et al., 2002, 2007; Gaser and Schlaug, 2003a; Lotze et al., 2003; Bangert and Schlaug, 2006; Zatorre et al., 2007). While some research has investigated functional brain correlates of musical training in childhood (Overy et al., 2004; Koelsch et al., 2005; Fujioka et al., 2006; Shahin et al., 2008), no studies have yet examined structural brain and behavioral changes in the developing brain in response to long-term music training to specifically address the question of whether structural brain differences seen in adults (comparing experts with matched controls) are a product of “nature” or “nurture.”
Such a study could also examine cognitive and behavioral changes in parallel with brain changes in response to music training. There is a widespread view that learning to play a musical instrument in childhood stimulates cognitive development and leads to the enhancement of skills in a variety of extramusical areas, which is commonly referred to as transfer (Bangerter and Heath, 2004). The most commonly observed form of transfer occurs when there is a close resemblance between the training domain and the transfer domain (typically referred to as “near transfer,” e.g., fine motor skills that develop while learning to play a musical instrument lead to increased speed and accuracy in typing). While near-transfer effects are relatively common, it is notoriously difficult to demonstrate “far transfer,” where the resemblance between training and transfer domains is much less obvious (e.g., learning to read and perform with precision from musical rhythm notation and understanding fractions in math). There are some claims for far transfer from instrumental music training in the areas of verbal, spatial, mathematical, and intelligence quotient (IQ) performance (Rauscher et al., 1993, 1997, 1998; Chan et al., 1998; Ho et al., 2003; Schellenberg, 2004; Forgeard et al., 2008), but such findings have also been controversial (Steele et al., 1999).
As part of an ongoing longitudinal study on the effects of music training on brain, behavioral, and cognitive development in young children (Norton et al., 2005; Schlaug et al., 2005), here we investigated structural brain changes in relation to behavioral changes in young children who received 15 months of instrumental musical training relative to a group of children who did not. We used deformation-based morphometry (DBM), an unbiased and automated approach to brain morphology, to search throughout the whole brain on a voxelwise basis for local brain size or shape differences between groups (Collins et al., 1994; Robbins et al., 2004). The DBM technique is useful for measuring morphometric brain changes longitudinally, as in the present study, where the DBM metric of interest, the Jacobian determinant, yields a measure of relative voxel size change over time in terms of voxel expansion (growth) or contraction (shrinkage). To investigate a brain–behavioral relationship, we correlated the brain deformation changes after 15 months with performance changes on behavioral tests.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
We tested two groups of children that were recruited from Boston area public schools and who had no prior formal musical training (see Table 1). The “instrumental” group consisted of 15 children (mean age at start of study 6.32 years old, SD 0.82 years) beginning weekly half-hour private keyboard lessons (outside of the school system), and who continued lessons for a mean interval of 15 months. The “control” group consisted of 16 children (mean age at start of study 5.90 years old, SD 0.54 years) who did not receive any instrumental music training during this 15 month period, but did participate in a weekly 40 min group music class in school consisting of singing and playing with drums and bells. The instrumental and control children were all right handed and matched as closely as possible in gender, age at the start of the study, and socioeconomic status (SES). SES was defined by parental education on a six-point scale, with a score of 1, for children whose parents had some high school education, to a score of 6, for those whose parents had a doctoral degree (see Norton et al., 2005).
At time 1, all children were tested on a series of behavioral tests (described below), and underwent a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan (scan 1). At time 2 (15 months later), all children were retested on the behavioral tests and underwent a second MRI scan (scan 2). The children whose results are reported here are drawn from a slightly larger group of instrumental and control children (see Norton et al., 2005). Here we only report the results from the children who completed both the behavioral tests and MRI scanning at times 1 and 2. We tested the hypothesis that brain and behavioral changes after 15 months should be greater in instrumental than in control children; this time period allows us to compare our results with those of other studies using a similar observation period.
Behavioral tests and MRI scanning.
Children were tested individually at times 1 and 2 on measures of handedness and SES, and on two near-transfer measures: a four-finger motor sequencing test for the left and right hands assessing fine finger motor skills, and a custom-made “melodic and rhythmic discrimination test battery” assessing music listening and discrimination skills. Five far-transfer measures were also administered: the object assembly, block design, and vocabulary subtests of the WISC-III (Wechsler, 1991), the Raven's progressive matrices (colored progressive matrices and standard progressive matrices) (Raven, 1976a,b), and the auditory analysis test (Rosner and Simon, 1971), assessing phonemic awareness. The vocabulary subtest of the WISC was used as a proxy for verbal IQ. For a detailed description of these tests and their administration to this group of children, see Norton et al. (2005) and Forgeard et al. (2008).
The two musically relevant (near transfer) behavioral tests are described in more detail below, since these were the only tests that showed significant between group differences after 15 months (see below, Results). Both of these tests are related to musical activity, but can also be performed by children who do not have any instrumental music training. In the four-finger motor sequencing test, children pressed a particular number sequence (e.g., 5-2-4-3-5) corresponding to fingers 2–5 of their left or right hand on the number keys of a computer keyboard as often, accurately, and fast as possible over a 30 s period. In the “melodic and rhythmic discrimination test battery,” children heard pairs of five-tone musical phrases differing only in melody and pairs of phrases differing only in rhythm. The task was to indicate whether the two musical phrases were the same or different. These musical phrases were designed for this study and have been described in more detail previously (Overy et al., 2004; Norton et al., 2005; Forgeard et al., 2008). The melodic and rhythmic subtest scores were combined to form one single behavioral measure of auditory–musical discrimination. Behavioral “difference scores” measuring the difference in performance on the behavioral tests from time 1 to time 2 were calculated and then correlated with the brain deformation measures.
Anatomical MRI scans were obtained for all children on a 3T General Electric MRI scanner using a T1-weighted, magnetization-prepared gradient-echo volume acquisition with a voxel resolution of 0.93 × 0.93 × 1.5 mm. This research was approved by the ethics committees of the Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of all the children, and the children themselves gave assent to participate in this study.
Brain deformation-based morphometry analyses.
Automated deformation brain analyses were performed on the T1 MRI data for each child (see supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). All MRI scans were first nonuniformity corrected (Sled et al., 1998), and registered to MNI space with a nine-parameter linear transform using mni_autoreg tools (Collins et al., 1994; Robbins et al., 2004). Next, brain deformation measures in terms of the Jacobian determinants (yielding a measure of relative voxel expansion or contraction) were calculated so that we could perform three different statistical analyses. First, to test for any brain deformation differences at baseline (before musical training), for each group, all time 1 MRI scans (0 months) iteratively underwent nonlinear registration toward the previous group average (starting with the linear group average). The Jacobian determinants of the final nonlinear registration were computed and blurred with a 10 mm Gaussian kernel. Statistical analyses were then performed comparing the Jacobian determinant data between groups at baseline, at each voxel. Second, to test for brain deformation differences between groups over time, each child's time 1 scan (at 0 months) was nonlinearly aligned to his or her time 2 scan (15 months later). The resulting displacement field was blurred with a 10 mm Gaussian kernel and the Jacobian determinant of the blurred displacement field was computed. Statistical analyses were then performed comparing the longitudinal Jacobian determinant data between groups, at each voxel. Third, to test for a brain–behavioral relationship, brain deformation differences (Jacobian determinants of scan 2 − scan 1 as above) were regressed on the behavioral difference scores (difference in test performance time 1 to time 2), for each subject, at each voxel. Last we checked for T1-weighted intensity differences between groups. All scans were intensity normalized, each subject's time 1 scan was subtracted from their time 2 scan, and the resulting intensity differences were compared between groups in a linear model.
The general linear model was used in the group statistical analyses with age at time 1, gender, and SES entered as covariates. The results from the group comparison were thresholded using random field theory cluster thresholding (Friston et al., 1994; Worsley et al., 2004), with a p < 0.05 cluster corresponding to at least 904 connected voxels with an uncorrected p < 0.001, or at an a priori cluster threshold of p < 0.1 (at least 240 connected voxels at an uncorrected p < 0.001) for strongly predicted regions that were not significant at the whole-brain threshold. The significant brain deformation differences from the group comparison were then used to define a volume of interest in which to test for brain–behavior correlations with the scores on the motor and auditory–musical tests. The results from this volume of interest were thresholded using the false discovery rate theory (Genovese et al., 2002) at q = 0.05.
Results
Behavioral changes
An initial χ2 analysis showed no significant difference between the instrumental and control groups in gender distribution (p > 0.1). Initial ANOVAs showed no significant difference between the groups in vocabulary scores at baseline (p > 0.1), replicating the results initially reported in Norton et al. (2005). There was a significant difference between groups in SES, with the instrumental group (mean 5.1 points, SD 0.63) having a higher average SES than the control group (mean 4.47 points, SD 0.87). The two groups also differed slightly in age at baseline (time 1), with the instrumental group (mean 6.32 years, SD 0.82) ∼5 months older than the control group (mean 5.90 years, SD 0.54). Although this age difference only approached significance (p = 0.1), we chose to be conservative and covaried age along with SES in our subsequent analyses.
A multiple analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), covarying age and SES, was conducted to determine that there were no preexisting group differences at time 1 on either near- or far-transfer outcomes. Missing values were replaced by the series' mean (2.42% of all values). The MANCOVA revealed no significant overall difference between groups (Wilks' λ = 0.85, F (8,20) = 0.44, p = 0.88). Follow-up univariate tests also indicated that the two groups did not differ significantly on any of the outcomes (all p > 0.1). Furthermore, the groups did not differ significantly in interval length (in months) between baseline (time 1) and time 2 testing (p > 0.1)
To determine whether the instrumental group progressed more than the control group on any of the outcomes between times 1 and 2, another MANCOVA was performed using the behavioral difference scores (performance difference from time 1 to 2) as our dependent variable, and age at baseline and SES as our covariates. Missing values were replaced by the series' mean (for 6.85% of all values). As predicted, there was a significant overall difference in the behavioral difference scores between the two groups (Wilks' λ = 0.50, F (8,20) = 2.55, p = 0.04, partial η2 = 0.51). Univariate tests revealed differences in the two near-transfer outcomes (motor and melody/rhythm tests) but not in any far-transfer outcomes.
On the finger motor sequencing test, the instrumental group significantly outperformed the control group in terms of the right-hand motor performance improvement over time (F (1,27) = 7.25, p = 0.01, partial η2 = 0.21), and the difference between groups approached significance for the left hand (F (1,27) = 3.81, p = 0.06, partial η2 = 0.12). The instrumental group also significantly outperformed the control group in improvement on the custom-made melodic/rhythmic discrimination test battery (F (1,27) = 13.20, p < 0.01, partial η2 = 0.33). No between-group differences in improvement over time (time 1 to 2) were found for the far-transfer measures of block design, vocabulary, object assembly, Raven's progressive matrices, and auditory analysis (all p > 0.1).
Brain deformation changes
With regard to between-group brain differences, we did not see any differences between groups at time 1. In terms of brain deformation changes in typical development that occurred in our controls (n = 15) over the 15 month period, brain deformations were found in frontal, temporal, and parieto-occipital brain areas (supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). In terms of between-group differences between the two time points, instrumental children showed significantly different brain deformation changes over the 15 months (time 2 scan at 15 months minus time 1 scan at 0 months) compared with controls (see Table 2 for all significant results). Instrumental children showed areas of greater relative voxel size than those of controls in motor areas, such as the right precentral gyrus (motor hand area) (Fig. 1 a), and the corpus callosum (fourth and fifth segment/midbody) (Fig. 2 a), that were significant at a whole-brain cluster threshold at p < 0.05, as well as in a right primary auditory region (lateral aspect of Heschl's gyrus) (Fig. 3 a) that was significant at an a priori cluster threshold at p < 0.1. Some significant brain deformation differences were also found outside auditory and motor brain areas. Instrumental children showed areas of greater relative voxel size than those of controls in bilateral frontolateral and frontomesial regions and a left posterior pericingulate region. In comparison, instrumental children showed only one area of lesser relative voxel size than that of controls in the left middle occipital gyrus. Last, no differences in normalized MR intensities were found between the two groups.
Correlations between brain and behavioral changes
Brain deformation changes in motor-related brain areas, including the right precentral gyrus and the corpus callosum, were predicted by left-hand motor test improvement scores. To illustrate the relationship between brain morphometry and behavior, we plotted the longitudinal brain deformation change over 15 months (in terms of relative voxel size) for each child as a function of his or her behavioral difference score on the left-hand motor sequencing test at the most significant (peak) voxel in the right precentral gyrus and the corpus callosum. The relative voxel size significantly increased with increasing left-hand motor improvement score at peak voxels in the right precentral gyrus (Fig. 1 b) and the corpus callosum (Fig. 2 b), but not in the right primary auditory region. Brain deformation changes in the right auditory area (Fig. 3 b) were predicted by improvements on the melodic/rhythmic discrimination test. However, brain deformation changes in the right primary motor region were not predicted by improvements on the melodic/rhythmic discrimination test battery, and brain deformation changes in the right primary auditory region were not predicted by motor improvement scores. No other significant correlations were found between brain deformations and either near or far-transfer behavioral measures.
Discussion
In the present study, we demonstrate regional structural brain plasticity in the developing brain that occurred with only 15 months of instrumental musical training in early childhood. Structural brain changes in motor and auditory areas (of critical importance for instrumental music training) were correlated with behavioral improvements on motor and auditory–musical tests. This study is the first longitudinal investigation to directly correlate brain structure and behavioral changes over time in the developing brain.
The lack of brain and behavioral differences between the instrumental and control children at baseline (before any music training) is consistent with previous findings from a larger sample that included the present subset of children tested here (Norton et al., 2005). It is not possible from these findings to completely rule out that musicians may be born with preexisting biological predictors of musicality or that some children may have a certain genetically determined trajectory of cerebral development that may lead them to more likely continue to practice music relative to other children without this same predisposition. However, our findings do support the view that brain differences seen in adult musicians relative to nonmusicians are more likely to be the product of intensive music training (Norton et al., 2005; Schlaug et al., 2005). Children who played and practiced a musical instrument showed greater improvements in motor ability (as measured by finger dexterity in both left and right hands) and in auditory melodic and rhythmic discrimination skills. Contrary to previous findings, however (Chan et al., 1998; Vaughn, 2000; Ho et al., 2003; Schellenberg, 2004; Rauscher et al., 1997, 2000), children who studied an instrument for 15 months did not show superior progress in visual–spatial and verbal transfer domain outcomes than children who did not receive instrumental training. We propose three reasons why 15 months of instrumental music training may not have been sufficient to result in far transfer: (1) 15 months of instrumental lessons may be too short a period of time (duration explanation); (2) children in our instrumental group may have practiced too little (intensity explanation); or (3) a larger sample may be required to demonstrate far transfer (power explanation).
The brain deformations found over 15 months in our controls (see supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) are consistent with previous findings in normal development that have included similar age ranges (from 5 to 7 years old) (e.g., Sowell et al., 2004). The consistency of the brain deformation found here in our controls with other studies of typical brain development in frontal, temporal, and parieto-occipital brain areas strengthens our conclusions that the brain deformations observed here between instrumental and control children are due to musical training. The present findings of structural brain changes in response to 15 months of instrumental music training are consistent with previous findings of training-induced structural brain differences in adults in various contexts (Draganski et al., 2004; Draganski and May, 2008). More specifically, the brain deformation differences found in primary motor brain regions are consistent with structural brain differences found between adult musicians and nonmusicians in the precentral gyri (Gaser and Schlaug, 2003b) and the corpus callosum (Schlaug et al., 1995; Oztürk et al., 2002; Schmithorst and Wilke, 2002; Lee et al., 2003). Although the right auditory cluster was not significant at a whole-brain level, this result was strongly predicted on the basis of findings of previous structural brain differences in right auditory cortex in adult musicians (Schneider et al., 2002; Gaser and Schlaug, 2003b; Bermudez and Zatorre, 2005). Thus, we report this right primary auditory region at an a priori threshold.
The brain–behavioral correlations found here in motor and auditory brain regions for performance on motor and auditory (melodic/rhythmic) tests show that different motor and auditory behavioral functions (both musically relevant) appear to be driving the group differences in separate predicted brain regions. These results are important from a functional perspective since these brain regions are known to be of critical importance in instrumental music performance and auditory processing. For example, the primary motor area plays a critical role in motor planning, execution, and control of bimanual sequential finger movements as well as motor learning (Karni et al., 1995; Grodd et al., 2001). The correlation found between the brain deformation measures and the motor test at the corpus callosum is consistent with the fact that the peak voxel lies in the fourth and fifth segments of the corpus callosum (Witelson, 1989) (also called midbody), which contains fibers connecting primary sensorimotor cortex (Wahl et al., 2007). Moreover, it has been suggested that intense bimanual motor training of musicians could play an important role in the determination of callosal fiber composition and size (Schlaug et al., 1995). Last, the correlation found between the brain deformation measures and the melody/rhythmic test battery in the right primary auditory region is consistent with functional brain mapping studies that have found activity changes using auditory–musical tests in similar auditory regions (Zatorre et al., 2002).
While structural brain differences were expected in motor and auditory brain areas, unexpected significant brain deformation differences were also found in various frontal areas, the left posterior pericingulate, and a left middle occipital region. However, none of these unexpected deformation changes were correlated with motor or auditory test performance changes. While we do not currently have an interpretation for some of these unexpected brain findings since they did not correlate with the auditory and motor behaviors, the left posterior pericingulate region warrants additional discussion since it showed a highly significant deformation difference. This region lies in the vicinity of Brodmann area 31 in the transition between posterior cingulate and occipital cortex and is involved in the integration of sensory (mostly visual) information and the limbic system. Such integration is involved in learning to read musical notation and relating music to its emotional content. The relative voxel size increases in frontomesial regions also stand out, although no obvious relationship with changes in motor and auditory performance was seen in these regions. Overall, these findings indicate that plasticity can occur in brain regions that control primary functions important for playing a musical instrument, and also in brain regions that might be responsible for the kind of multimodal sensorimotor integration likely to underlie the instrumental learning. None of the unexpected brain deformation differences mentioned above were correlated with behavioral performance changes in any of the far-transfer domains. This may indicate that brain structural changes in association areas and multimodal integration regions may develop before the emergence of significant behavioral/cognitive changes in far-transfer domains.
While we have discussed the functional significance of the brain–behavioral structural changes, the underlying structural properties of the results are not trivial to explain. The brain deformation techniques used here are key to localize brain size/shape changes over time, but are not able to inform us on the microstructural nature of these changes. Overall, instrumental children showed greater relative voxel size expansion than controls over the 15 months, and only one area of voxel size contraction. A voxel expansion or contraction may reflect increased or decreased gray or white matter due to neural reorganization/pruning or increased/decreased brain connectivity. Evidence from animal models investigating the effects of long-term learning and practice of complex motor skills (Anderson et al., 2002) on brain structure may shed light on the structural neural basis of the brain structural changes seen here. Several groups have demonstrated microstructural brain changes as a function of long-term motor learning, including an increased number of synapses and glial cells, increased density of capillaries in primary motor cortex and cerebellum, and new brain cells in the hippocampus after long-term motor training in adult rats (Black et al., 1990; Isaacs et al., 1992; Anderson et al., 1994; Kleim et al., 1996; Kempermann et al., 1997; Anderson et al., 2002). The sum of these microstructural changes could amount to structural differences that are detectable on a macrostructural level, such as those observed in the present study (Anderson et al., 2002; Bangert and Schlaug, 2006). It is possible that the specific and continuous engagement of a unimodal and multimodal sensorimotor network, and the induced changes in this network across a musician's career, may provide the neural basis for some of the sensorimotor and cognitive enhancements attributed to musical training. Future, even higher-resolution morphometric investigations with more direct measures of gray and white matter will be key to developing a better understanding of the underlying nature of the brain deformation differences found here. We also did not find any differences in MR intensities between groups, though using T1-weighted sequences is clearly a limitation in this regard. Future studies should examine quantitative sequences, such as diffusion tensor imaging, magnetization transfer, etc., in more detail to see whether microstructural changes can be captured separately from the volumetric differences described herein. Last, we wish to point out that one of the potential confounds of deformation-based morphometry is that the deformation procedure can sometimes result in changes being propagated to regions distant from their actual origin. Given that the present results were predicted based on the functional literature, we feel it is unlikely that such propagation accounts for the results presented in this manuscript. In the future, converging results from additional structural and functional analyses metrics will serve to strengthen our conclusions.
In summary, our findings show for the first time that musical training over only 15 months in early childhood leads to structural brain changes that diverge from typical brain development. Regional training-induced structural brain changes were found in musically relevant regions that were driven by musically relevant behavioral tests. The fact there were no structural brain differences found between groups before the onset of musical training indicates that the differential development of these brain regions is induced by instrumental practice rather by than preexisting biological predictors of musicality. These results provide new evidence for training-induced structural brain plasticity in early childhood. These findings of structural plasticity in the young brain suggest that long-term intervention programs can facilitate neuroplasticity in children. Such an intervention could be of particular relevance to children with developmental disorders and to adults with neurological diseases.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (BCS0518837), the Dana Foundation, and the NAMM Foundation. We thank our previous research assistants and postdoctoral fellows (K. Cronin, L. Forbes, L. Blake, C. Alexander, M. Rosam, K. Brumm, A. Norton, L. Zhu, U. Iyengar, and K. Overy) for test preparation, behavioral testing, and imaging data collection and the participating children and their families for their cooperation in taking part in our experiments.
- Correspondence should be addressed to either Krista L. Hyde or Gottfried Schlaug at the above addresses. krista.hyde{at}mail.mcgill.ca or gschlaug{at}bidmc.harvard.edu
References
- Anderson et al., 1994.↵
- Anderson et al., 2002.↵
- Bangert and Schlaug, 2006.↵
- Bangerter and Heath, 2004.↵
- Bermudez and Zatorre, 2005.↵
- Black et al., 1990.↵
- Chan et al., 1998.↵
- Collins et al., 1994.↵
- Draganski and May, 2008.↵
- Draganski et al., 2004.↵
- Elbert et al., 1995.↵
- Forgeard et al., 2008.↵
- Friston et al., 1994.↵
- Fujioka et al., 2006.↵
- Gaab and Schlaug, 2003.↵
- Gaser and Schlaug, 2003a.↵
- Gaser and Schlaug, 2003b.↵
- Genovese et al., 2002.↵
- Grodd et al., 2001.↵
- Ho et al., 2003.↵
- Hund-Georgiadis and von Cramon, 1999.↵
- Isaacs et al., 1992.↵
- Karni et al., 1995.↵
- Kempermann et al., 1997.↵
- Kleim et al., 1996.↵
- Koelsch et al., 2005.↵
- Lappe et al., 2008.↵
- Lee et al., 2003.↵
- Lotze et al., 2003.↵
- Münte et al., 2001.↵
- Norton et al., 2005.↵
- Overy et al., 2004.↵
- Oztürk et al., 2002.↵
- Pantev et al., 1998.↵
- Rauscher et al., 1993.↵
- Rauscher et al., 1997.↵
- Rauscher et al., 1998.↵
- Raven, 1976a.↵
- Raven, 1976b.↵
- Robbins et al., 2004.↵
- Rosner and Simon, 1971.↵
- Schellenberg, 2004.↵
- Schlaug, 2001.↵
- Schlaug et al., 1995.↵
- Schlaug et al., 2005.↵
- Schmithorst and Wilke, 2002.↵
- Schneider et al., 2002.↵
- Shahin et al., 2008.↵
- Sled et al., 1998.↵
- Sluming et al., 2002.↵
- Sluming et al., 2007.↵
- Sowell et al., 2004.↵
- Steele et al., 1999.↵
- Vaughn, 2000.↵
- Wahl et al., 2007.↵
- Wechsler, 1991.↵
- Witelson, 1989.↵
- Worsley et al., 2004.↵
- Zatorre, 1998.↵
- Zatorre et al., 2002.↵
- Zatorre et al., 2007.↵