Abstract
Genetic screens for Drosophila mutants defective in pavlovian olfactory memory have provided unique insight into the molecular basis of memory storage. Occasionally, these singular genetic lesions have been assembled into meaningful molecular pathways and neural circuitries. For the most part, however, these genes and their expression patterns in the CNS remain fragmented, demanding new clues from continued mutant screens. From a behavioral screen for long-term memory (LTM) mutants, we have identified ben (CG32594), which encodes a novel protein. Mutations of ben specifically disrupt LTM, leaving earlier memory phases intact. The role of ben appears physiological rather than developmental, because acutely induced expression of a ben+ transgene in adults rescues the mutant's LTM defect. More interestingly, induced expression of ben+ specifically in mushroom bodies (MBs), but not in the ellipsoid body of the central complex, is sufficient to rescue the mutant LTM defect. This suggests a role for ben in the MB during olfactory memory formation. We also provide evidence that BEN interacts genetically in both synaptic transmission and LTM formation with SCAMP, a synaptic protein known to be involved in vesicle recycling.
- olfactory learning
- synaptic transmission mutants
- larval neuromuscular junction
- mutant
- learning and memory
- neurotransmission
Introduction
Like other biological processes, complex behaviors (i.e., learning and memory) are shaped by selective pressure and are remarkably conserved across animal phyla. Such a genetic perspective provides the conceptual motivation to use “simple” model systems initially to discover specific genes involved in a given behavioral phenomenon and then to manipulate each gene's expression or function, thereby unraveling the relevant molecular and neuronal mechanisms. Olfactory memory in Drosophila melanogaster is a well established model system to study the genetic and neuronal architecture of associative learning. Of particular relevance, many genes identified from this model system have mammalian homologs with similar functions (Margulies et al., 2005), or, conversely, genes first discovered to be involved in mammalian plasticity also appear to be involved in associative learning in the fly (Xia et al., 2005; Wu et al., 2007, 2008; Bolduc et al., 2008). Therefore, lessons we learn from the neurogenetics of olfactory memory in flies should be relevant to our understanding of human cognition.
Past work on pavlovian olfactory memory in Drosophila has begun to elucidate the underlying molecules and circuits (Margulies et al., 2005; Keene and Waddell, 2007). To date, two observations are apparent. First, behavioral plasticity depends on a large fraction of the neuron's proteome (Ashraf et al., 2006). A biological pathway from synaptic dopamine and/or glutamatergic transmission through gene transcription to local protein translation is emerging as a central mechanism of long-term memory (LTM) formation (Petersen et al., 1997; Dubnau et al., 2003; Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Ge et al., 2004; Presente et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2007; Wu et al., 2007). More complete connections among genes in this network remain to be discovered, however. Second, distinct neuroanatomical structures underlie olfactory memory formation, including the antennal lobe (AL), the mushroom body (MB), and the ellipsoid body (EB) (Heisenberg et al., 1985; Connolly et al., 1996; Zars et al., 2000; Pascual and Preat, 2001; Akalal et al., 2006; Krashes et al., 2007; Lu et al., 2007; Wu et al., 2007). Moreover, imaging studies have revealed neural activities that potentially underlie this behavioral plasticity. Olfactory learning generates (1) one short-lived “memory trace” in the AL (Yu et al., 2004); (2) two additional, more persistent traces in dopaminergic or in dorsal paired medial neurons, both of which innervate MB neurons (Waddell et al., 2000; Riemensperger et al., 2005; Yu et al., 2005); (3) a fourth trace in the MB α′ lobe, which is associated with short- to middle-term memory (Wang et al., 2008); and (4) a fifth trace, specific to axons of the MB α lobe, only after LTM formation (Yu et al., 2006). Additional anatomies and neural activities involved in memory storage or retrieval have not yet been discerned.
To expand our knowledge of genetic pathways and neural circuits, we conducted a behavioral screen for mutants defective in LTM after pavlovian olfactory learning (Lu et al., 2007; Qian et al., 2007) and identified the ben gene (CG32594), which encodes a novel protein. Although BEN is expressed throughout the brain, induced expression of a ben+ transgene in MB is sufficient to rescue the mutant's LTM defect. The ben mutant also is defective in synaptic transmission at the larval neuromuscular junction (NMJ). Perusal of a protein interaction database suggested that BEN interacts with SCAMP, a protein involved in vesicle recycling. Scamp mutants also show defects in synaptic transmission (Fernandez-Chacon and Sudhof, 2000; Lloyd et al., 2000). We confirmed a functional interaction between BEN and SCAMP by showing defects for both LTM formation in adults and synaptic transmission at the larval NMJ in flies heterozygous for mutations in both genes.
Materials and Methods
Fly stocks.
Transposon mutagenesis was performed as described in previous work (Lu et al., 2007; Qian et al., 2007). A PlacW autosomal mutator in a CyO balancer chromosome was used to generate X-linked transposant strains. Two thousand twenty-one adult-viable homozygous strains were screened for defective 24 h memory after spaced training using a pavlovian olfactory task (Tully et al., 1994). After outcrossing to w1118 (isoCJ1) for at least five generations to equilibrate genetic backgrounds and to remove most second-site mutations and after blind and balanced retesting, 11 candidate mutants were discovered with reliable 24 h memory defects. Based on molecular, genetic and behavioral characterizations of mutant strain 20816039 presented herein, we renamed the novel gene, CG32594, ben (“ben” means “stupid” in Mandarin) and the original mutant strain benP1. The strain w[*]P{GawB}NP4013/FM7c was obtained from Kyoto Stock Center (Kyoto Institute of Technology, Kyoto, Japan). The strains y1w67c23P{EPgy2}hiwEY08069 (Bellen et al., 2004) and EP1593 (Lloyd et al., 2000) were obtained from Bloomington Stock Center (Indiana University, Bloomington, IN). The w*;nocSco/CyO;P{tubP-GAL80ts}7 (7018-GAL80ts), w*;P{tubP-GAL80ts}20;TM2/TM6B, Tb1 (7019-GAL80ts) (McGuire et al., 2003), P26 (hs-GAL4), 247-Gal4 (MB247), c739-Gal4 (c739), and Feb170-Gal4 (Feb170) drivers were extant stocks in our laboratory. All these strains were outcrossed with w1118 (isoCJ1) flies for at least five generations to equilibrate genetic backgrounds. hiwND8 and hiwND9 were gifts from the laboratory of Dr. A. DiAntonio (Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO) and were crossed for five generations with FM7a balancer flies, which themselves had been outcrossed with w1118 (isoCJ1). All the flies, if not mentioned specifically, were reared at 25°C and 60% relative humidity with a 14/10 h light/dark cycle. The w1118 (isoCJ1), a Canton-S strain isogenic for the X, second and third chromosomes (Yin et al., 1994), served as a “wild-type” control in all our experiments. Various genetic crosses generated the genotypes tested. Virgin benP1 homozygotes were crossed with benEY08069 males or hiwND8 and hiwND9 males for the genetic complementation experiments. Virgin benP1;hs-Gal4 homozygotes were crossed with UAS-ben+4–8 males for transgenic rescue. Virgin benP1;UAS-ben+4–8 homozygotes were crossed with Gal80ts; 247, c739 or Feb170 males for spatiotemporal rescue experiments. For experiments using the X-linked Feb170 driver, benP1,Feb170 flies were bred first by crossing benP1 virgins with Feb170 males. Virgin F1 females (benP1,+/+,Feb170) then were crossed with FM7a males, and the miniwhite-carrying progeny females with darker eye color (presumably benP1,Feb170 recombinants) were crossed singly to FM7a males to generate a pure-breeding stock. The putative benP1,Feb170 stock was confirmed by (1) detecting the benP1 genomic insertion with PCR and (2) demonstrating the presence of Feb170 by crossing the flies to UAS–green fluorescent protein. Virgin benP1 or benEY08069 homozygotes were crossed with ScampEP1593 males for the epistasis experiments. Virgin ScampEP1593;hs-Gal4 homozygotes were crossed with benP1;UAS-ben+4–8 for the epistatic rescue experiment.
Plasmid rescue of the benP1 PlacW transposon insertion.
Genomic DNA was isolated from homozygous benP1 mutants, digested to completion with EcoRl (NEB), and “plasmid rescued” as described previously (Dubnau et al., 2003). DNA sequence was obtained by automated sequencing (Applied Biosystems) with a primer directed against the 3′ long terminal repeat (LTR) of PlacW (5′-CGCCGACAAGCTTTGCGTACTCGC-3′). A blast of genomic DNA flanking the 3′ LTR identified the genomic insertion site of the PlacW transposon.
Generation of UAS-ben+ transgenic flies.
The expected full-length ben+ cDNA was amplified by Pfu-polymerase (Stratagene) from cDNA clone SD04373 (DGRC) and sequenced to confirm its correctness. NotI restriction sites were added to both ends of the PCR product, which then was subcloned into a pUAST germ-line transformation vector. Transformation was performed by BestGene. Ten transformants were obtained and outcrossed with w1118 (isoCJ1) for at least five generations to equilibrate genetic backgrounds to each other and to the w1118 (isoCJ1) controls. Quantitative PCR determined that the UAS-ben+4–8 line, with the transgene inserted on the second chromosome, showed the highest level of expression for the ben+ transgene. This line thus was chosen for the rescue experiments.
Northern blot analyses.
RNA was isolated from adult fly heads of each genotype with Trizol according to the manufacturer's protocols (Invitrogen). PolyA+ RNA was selected with oligo dT-magnetic beads (Dynal Biotech) according to the manufacturer's protocol. Northern blots were prepared using standard techniques, as in the study by DeZazzo et al. (2000). In brief, 5 μg of polyA+ RNA per sample was electrophoresed through a 1% agarose-formaldehyde gel via 4-morpholinepropanesulfonic acid buffer. Gels were blotted onto nylon membrane (Schleicher & Schuell). P32-labeled probes were generated by random priming with (1) reverse transcription-PCR-generated fragments of ben, hiw, or CG5541 predicted exons or (2) an R1/HIII restriction fragment from the Rp49 gene (O'Connell and Rosbash, 1984). The resulting PCR fragments then were gel purified (Qiagen) before making radiolabeled probes.
Quantitative real-time PCR.
Total RNA was isolated from ∼2000 adult fly heads using Trizol total RNA isolation reagent according to the manufacturer's instructions (Invitrogen). cDNA was synthesized with Expand Reverse transcriptase (Roche Diagnostics) and oligo-dT primers. Quantitative real-time PCR was performed with the PRISM 7500 sequence detection system (Applied Biosystems). TaqMan probes for TATA-binding protein (TBP) (control) and appropriate probes for ben, hiw, and CG5541 were synthesized by Applied Biosystems. Data analysis was performed from quadplex independent samples.
Immunohistochemistry.
Whole-mount immunolabeling of 2- to 3-d-old adult brains was performed as described previously (Chiang et al., 2001, 2002). Briefly, dissected brains were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS at room temperature overnight, transferred to 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS with 2% Triton X-100 at a room temperature, and vacuumed for 1 h. Fixed tissue then was blocked for 2 d at 4°C in PBS containing 2% Triton X-100 and 10% normal goat serum (NGS) and successively incubated for 2 d each (with washing between) at 4°C in PBS containing 1% Triton X-100, 0.25% NGS, and (1) the mouse anti-LacZ monoclonal antibody (1:200 dilution) or the rabbit anti-SCAMP polyclonal antibody (1:200 dilution) or both, or (2) a fluorescence-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1:200 dilution) and/or goat anti-mouse IgG (1:200 dilution). Next, the brain was cleared and mounted in FocusClear (Pacgen) and imaged with a Zeiss LSM 510 confocal microscope. For larvae brain and NMJ staining, the same procedure was followed except for changing the Triton X-100 concentration from 2% to 0.2%.
Induction of the UAS-ben+ transgene.
Acute heat-shock induction was performed according to previously published protocols (Xia et al., 2005). Briefly, flies were collected and incubated at 25°C overnight before heat-shock treatment. The next day, flies were transferred to empty glass vials and submerged for 45 min in a 37°C water bath. After heat shock, flies were transferred to fresh food vials at 25°C and allowed a 3 h recovery period before training/testing. For experiments using Gal80ts (and Gal4), flies were raised at 18°C and kept either at 30°C or at 18°C as adults for 3 d before training, during training, during the retention interval and during testing.
Pavlovian olfactory learning.
We quantified olfactory associative learning by subjecting 1- to 4-d-old adult flies to a pavlovian conditioning procedure (Tully and Quinn, 1985; Tully et al., 1994; Yin et al., 1994). Typically, after one training session, memory retention in normal flies drops to near zero within 24 h. To produce longer-lasting memory, flies were subjected to repetitive training sessions, either massed (10 sessions with no rest interval) or spaced (10 training sessions with a 15 min rest between each). After training, flies were transferred to food vials (at 18°C) for 24 h before testing (25°C) in the T maze. Performance indices (PIs) are normally distributed. Behavioral data were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA with subsequent pairwise planned comparisons between group means. For each experiment, the critical p value (α = 0.05) was adjusted for the number of pairwise comparisons made (Bonferroni's correction).
Sensorimotor responses.
Olfactory acuity was quantified by exposing naive flies to each odor (octanol or methylcyclohexanol) versus air in the T maze. After 120 s, the number of flies in each arm of the T maze was counted, and the PI was calculated for each odor as reported (Tully et al., 1994). Shock reactivity was quantified in naive flies by inserting electric grids into both arms of the T maze and delivering shock pulses to one of the arms. Flies were transported to the choice point of the T maze, allowing them to choose between the two arms. After 120 s, the center compartment was closed, trapping flies in their respective arms. Individual PIs were calculated as for olfactory acuity. Data were analyzed as above.
Electrophysiological recordings at the larval NMJ.
Synaptic currents were recorded from muscle 12 in segment 3 or 4 of third instar larvae (Jan and Jan, 1976; Singh and Wu, 1989) using two-electrode voltage clamp as described previously (Wu and Haugland, 1985; Singh and Wu, 1989). Both the voltage electrode and the current electrode were filled with 3 m KCl with a resistance in the range of 5–10 MΩ. All electrophysiological recordings were performed at room temperature (19–23°C). Stimulation of the precut motor nerve in excitatory junction current (EJC) experiments was achieved by 1 ms positive current stimulation of a loop of motor nerve in a suction electrode. The stimulation threshold was determined by varying stimulation intensity until the amplitude of junction currents appeared. Stimulation intensity was set 1.5 times the threshold. Both spontaneous miniature EJCs (mEJCs) and evoked EJCs were recorded in 0.4 mm Ca2+ Stewart saline (HL-3) (Stewart et al., 1994). All data were acquired at a clamping voltage of −80 mV, using an Axoclamp 2B amplifier (Molecular Devices) in conjunction with pClamp6 software. Consecutive events recorded every 2 min were measured per cell using MiniAnal (Synaptosoft) and averaged to determine the mean mEJC amplitude and frequency. Evoked EJCs were measured from 20 stimulations and analyzed by MiniAnal software. Statistical significance was evaluated as above.
Results
The ben mutant disrupts LTM specifically
We screened 2021 homozygous adult-viable PlacW transposants for defects in 24 h memory after spaced training and identified strain 20816039 as one of 11 candidate mutants (Lu et al., 2007; Qian et al., 2007). We named this mutant strain ben, which means “stupid” in Mandarin, and specified the mutation as benP1. Whereas 1 d memory after spaced training was impaired significantly in the ben mutant, 1 d memory after massed training (ARM) was not (Fig. 1A), suggesting that LTM but not ARM was disrupted (Tully et al., 1994). Memory retention at 5, 30, or 180 min after a single training session also was normal in ben mutants (Fig. 1D), further indicating that early memory (short-term memory and middle-term memory) also was not affected (Tully et al., 1996).
As a result of the molecular-genetic characterization of the ben mutant (see below), we identified a second putative allele in strain EY08069 (benEY). benEY and benP1 each were recessive to ben+, and they failed to complement each other for 1 d memory after spaced training (Fig. 1B). In contrast, benP1 complemented two different alleles of hiw (Fig. S1A,B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). We also generated a precise excision of benP1 and showed a reversion of the mutant LTM defect in this benex8 homozygote (Fig. 1C). Finally, normal performance of the benP1 mutant 5 min after training suggested that it also had normal sensorimotor responses to odors (positive and negative conditioned stimuli) and footshock (unconditioned stimulus). Nonetheless, we assessed olfactory acuity and shock reactivity directly, confirming that these task-relevant behavioral responses were normal in the ben mutant (Table 1). Together, these behavior-genetic data establish that disruption of ben impairs LTM formation specifically.
benP1 disrupts the CG32594 transcript
Plasmid rescue of the PlacW transposon in the benP1 mutant, followed by sequencing of genomic DNA flanking the PlacW insertion site, identified the benP1 PlacW to be inserted 21 bp 5′ of the CG32594 transcript. Functional analysis of ben was complicated by the fact that ben and CG5541 both reside in the largest intron (∼33 kb) of highwire, which is involved in synaptic development and function (Fig. 2A) (Wan et al., 2000; Wu et al., 2005). We analyzed these three genes' expression levels by Northern blot analysis and real-time PCR (Fig. 2B,C; Fig. S2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). The mRNA levels of ben were decreased significantly in the benP1 homozygote and were nearly abolished in the benEY08069 homozygote, which has an EPgy2 element inserted in the first exon (Bellen et al., 2004). In contrast, mRNA levels of hiw and CG5541 were normal in benP1 or benEY homozygotes (Fig. S2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), suggesting the benP1 PlacW insertional mutation specifically disrupts the ben gene and not its neighboring genes.
We have not yet been able to identify antibodies specific to BEN even after several attempts; consequently, we have not determined the subcellular distribution of BEN. Nevertheless, we were able to assess in which neurons of the CNS ben likely is expressed by using the lacZ reporter protein within the PlacW transposon of the benP1 mutant, along with the Gal4 reporter protein in strain 4013 (benGal4), which contains a PGawB transposon inserted in the 5′ untranslated region (UTR) of ben (Fig. 3A,B). In contrast to the negative signal observed in wild-type flies, immunostaining for both anti-LacZ and anti-GAL4 in the corresponding flies revealed widespread expression in the adult brain, with higher levels in MB Kenyon cells (Fig. S3B,C, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Merged confocal images of the anti-LacZ (perinuclear) and anti-GAL4 (cytosolic) signals showed coexpression of these reporter proteins in some Kenyon cells (Fig. 3A). Thus, the overlapping expression of the benP1 PlacZ and benGal4 PGawB reporter genes suggest that ben is expressed in Kenyon cells of the MB.
Induced expression in the adult brain, or restricted expression in the MB, of a ben+ transgene is sufficient to rescue the ben LTM defect
To establish an acute role for ben in LTM, we first attempted rescue of the ben mutant LTM defect by driving the expression of a UAS-ben+ transgene (UAS-ben+4–8) with a hs-Gal4 driver, which induces rapid, ubiquitous expression of ben+ in the adult brain after heat shock. Flies were either heat shocked (25–37°C for 45 min with 3 h recovery) or kept at 25°C during the experimental procedure, and 1 d memory after spaced training was quantified for all groups. In the absence of heat shock, 1 d memory in the ben;UAS-ben+;hs-Gal4 transgenic mutant males was significantly lower than that in the UAS-ben+4 control males (Fig. 3C). When trained 3 h after heat shock, in contrast, transgenic mutants and controls showed similar 1 d memory (Fig. 3C). Heat shock produced no significant effects in wild-type flies (Fig S4, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), thereby allowing the conclusion that heat shock-induced expression of UAS-ben+ produced a specific rescue of the mutant LTM defect.
Consistent with these behavioral rescue data, ben mRNA expression in the absence of heat shock was significantly lower in ben;UAS-ben+;hs-Gal4 transgenic mutant males than in the UAS-ben+ control males and was elevated dramatically in transgenic mutants 3 h after heat shock (Fig. 3D). Given ben's location within an intron of hiw, we also evaluated whether heat shock affected hiw mRNA expression levels in transgenic mutant or control males; it did not (Fig. 3E).
Having established a physiological, rather than a developmental, role for ben during adult LTM formation, we returned to the question of where in the adult brain ben's function might be critical for LTM formation. The widespread expression patterns of the benP1 and benGal4 reporter genes in the adult brain did not provide any clues as to where ben might be required for LTM formation. Based on the literature, however, two anatomical regions seemed possible: MBs (Connolly et al., 1996; Zars et al., 2000; Dubnau et al., 2001; Pascual and Preat, 2001; McGuire et al., 2003; Krashes et al., 2007; Lu et al., 2007; Qian et al., 2007) or the EB of the central complex (Wu et al., 2007). We first used the Gal4 driver 247, which expresses strongly in α/β and γ lobes and weakly in α′/β′ lobes, of MB (Schwaerzel et al., 2002). We combined 247 with tub-Gal80ts to control UAS-ben+ transgene expression temporally as well as spatially (McGuire et al., 2003). One-day memory after spaced training was quantified for all groups. When trained and tested at 18°C (GAL80ts inhibits GAL4), 1 d memory in ben;UAS-ben+/Gal80ts;247 transgenic mutant males and in the ben;UAS-ben+ control mutant males was significantly lower and than in Gal80ts;247 control males (Fig. 3F). When trained and tested at 30°C (GAL80ts fails to inhibit GAL4), 1 d memory in the transgenic mutant males was significantly higher than in mutant control males and was not different from the wild-type control males (Fig. 3F). A temperature shift from 18 to 30°C in control flies produced no effects on 1 d memory (data not shown), therefore suggesting a specific effect of the UAS-ben+ transgene.
This initial result on rescue of the mutant LTM defect with transgenic ben+ expression restricted to MBs was confirmed using two additional Gal4 drivers with preferential expression in MBs. With both c739 and c305a (Krashes et al., 2007; Wang et al., 2008), we again saw induced rescue of the ben LTM defect (Fig. 3G,H). We also induced expression of UAS-ben+ in the EB of the central complex by using the Gal4 driver Feb170 but did not observe any rescue of the mutant LTM defect (Fig. 3I) (Wu et al., 2007, but see Krashes and Waddell, 2008). Finally, we investigated ben's role during memory retrieval rather than memory storage. Transgenic ben;hs-Gal4;UAS-ben+ mutant males were heat shocked (from 25 to 37°C for 45 min) and then were allowed to recover at 25°C for 1 h right before evaluating 1 d memory after spaced training. Transgenic ben;UAS-ben+;hs-Gal4 mutant males showed similar, defective 1 d memory after heat shock (or when not heat shocked) compared with UAS-ben+4–8control males (Fig. 3J). Together, these observations suggest that ben functions specifically in the adult MB (most likely in α/β an α′/β′ lobes) during LTM consolidation.
The ben mutant disrupts synaptic transmission at the larval NMJ
Other memory mutants have demonstrated defects in synaptic plasticity at the larval NMJ (Saitoe, 2001). The benP1 transposon is inserted in a large intron of hiw, however, mutations of which are known to disrupt synaptic structure at the larval NMJ (Wan et al., 2000; Wu et al., 2005). Consequently, we first had to rule out any disruptive effects that the benP1 mutation may have exerted on hiw function. We clearly detected abnormally high numbers of branches and boutons in homozygous hiwND9 mutants. Such synaptic structure was normal in the benP1 mutant, however, which also complemented the defects of hiwND9 (Fig. S1C,D, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Along with our results from genetic complementation for LTM, these data also confirm that these ben mutations do not affect hiw function.
We next evaluated synaptic function in the ben mutants by recording from muscle 12 (segment 3 or 4) of the third instar larva (Jan and Jan, 1976; Singh and Wu, 1989). Postsynaptic responses to both spontaneous and evoked transmitter release were quantified. The mean amplitude of spontaneous mEJCs, also called quantal size, typically is considered to be a measure of postsynaptic sensitivity to transmitter. The mean amplitude of evoked EJCs, in contrast, depends on both postsynaptic receptor function and the amount of transmitter released from the presynaptic terminal. We found the mean mEJC amplitude for benP1 homozygotes to be normal (Fig. 4A), and the mean mEJC frequency for this genotype was slightly lower than normal, but not significantly so (Fig. 4A). In contrast, the mean evoked EJC for benP1 homozygotes was significantly lower than normal, by nearly 35% (Fig. 4B). Thus, the quantal content (number of vesicles released, which is estimated by dividing the mean EJC amplitude by the mean mEJC amplitude) at the synapse of the benP1 mutant was only ∼65% of wild type. This defect in evoked EJCs also was apparent in the ben;UAS-ben+;hs-Gal4 transgenic mutant and the ben;UAS-ben+ mutant control in the absence of heat shock (Fig. 4C). When measured 3 h after a 45 min heat shock, however, the mean amplitude of evoked EJCs was rescued fully (i.e., normal) in the transgenic mutant (Fig. 4C). Considering quantal content is an indication of presynaptic function (Landmesser, 1998), our data suggest that ben is required presynaptically during synaptic transmission. Given our parallel behavioral rescue for LTM, our data suggest that a similar defect in synaptic transmission in the CNS may underlie the LTM impairment of the ben mutant.
BEN and SCAMP interact functionally to disrupt LTM formation and synaptic transmission
Guided by the NMJ defect of the ben mutant, we perused BioGrid, a public database of gene interactions (Stark et al., 2006). We discovered from yeast two-hybrid experiments that BEN interacts with CdsA, which itself interacts with SCAMP (Giot et al., 2003). The Scamp gene encodes a secretory carrier membrane protein family member that has been shown in Drosophila to be involved in vesicle trafficking and neurotransmitter release (Hubbard et al., 2000; Littleton, 2000).
Based on this bioinformatics, we evaluated a functional connection between ben and Scamp using a classic test for genetic epistasis, quantifying 1 d memory after spaced training in benP1/ScampEP or benEY/ScampEP double heterozygotes. Although the Scamp mutation alone appeared to be recessive (LTM was normal), 1 d memory after spaced training was significantly lower than normal in both double heterozygotes (Fig. 5A). This defect also appeared specific for LTM, because 1 d memory after massed training and 5 min memory after one training session were normal in both double heterozygotes (Fig. 5A,B). Importantly, this epistatic LTM defect could be rescued in benP1/ScampEP;UAS-ben+;hs-Gal4 transgenic heterozygotes by heat shock-induced overexpression of ben+ (Fig. 5C), indicating that the interaction between ben and Scamp is physiological rather than developmental.
We also asked whether ben and Scamp functioned together during synaptic transmission at the larval NMJ. Double labeling of larval CNS with anti-lacZ and anti-Scamp in benP1/benP1 flies implied that both are expressed in a similar group of motor neurons (Fig. 6A, arrowheads), and SCAMP immunostaining was apparent in axons of the motor neurons (Fig. 6B). The mean amplitudes of evoked EJCs at the larval NMJ in benP1/+ or ScampEP/+ heterozygotes were similar to that in the wild-type (+/+) control (Fig. 6C). In contrast, the Scamp/Scamp homozygote or the benP1/Scamp double heterozygote showed significantly lower EJC amplitudes, similar to that produced by the benP1 homozygote (Fig. 6C). Thus, SCAMP appears to function together with BEN during synaptic transmission at the NMJ.
Discussion
Disruptions of ben impair LTM
The ben gene resides in a genomic region, near CG5541 within the large intron of hiw (Fig. 2A). To rule out potential second-site effects, we have shown that (1) two independently derived mutations of ben yield LTM defects when homozygous (Fig. 1A); (2) a heteroallelic combination of these two mutations also disrupts LTM (Fig. 1B); (3) two independently derived mutations of hiw do not yield LTM defects when homozygous (Fig. S1A, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material); (4) heteroallelic combinations of either of these hiw mutations with benP1 do not yield LTM defects (Fig. S1B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material); (5) a heteroallelic combination of benP1 and hiwND9 does not disrupt synaptic structure at the NMJ, as is the case for hiw mutants (Fig. S1C,D, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material); (6) Northern blot analyses show reduced levels of mRNA for ben, but not for hiw or CG5541, in benP1/benP1 or benEY/benEY mutants (Fig. 2B,C; Fig. S2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material); (7) induced expression of a ben+ transgene is sufficient to rescue the LTM defect of the benP1 mutant (Fig. 3C,F,G); and (8) induced expression of a ben+ transgene yielded an increase in ben mRNA but had no effect on hiw mRNA (Fig. 3D,E). Together, these data establish that the LTM defect of the ben mutant derives from disruption of the ben gene.
Transgenic expression of ben+ in adult MBs is sufficient to rescue the mutant LTM defect
LTM is induced after spaced, but not massed, training (Tully et al., 1994). Massed training induces a cycloheximide-insensitive form of long-lasting memory (ARM), whereas spaced training induces ARM along with a cycloheximide-sensitive LTM (Tully et al., 1994). Thus, normal 1 d memory after massed training suggests that learning and earlier memory is normal in ben mutants (Fig. 1A,B), a notion also supported by normal memory 5, 30, and 180 min after one training session (Fig. 1D) and normal sensorimotor responses (Table 1) in the mutant. This conglomerate effect on LTM formation is not unique for ben, because disruptions of several other genes also appear specifically to impair LTM (Yin et al., 1994; Pascual and Preat, 2001; Comas et al., 2004; Ge et al., 2004; Presente et al., 2004; Didelot et al., 2006; Lu et al., 2007; Qian et al., 2007).
The LTM defect of ben can be rescued by induced expression of a ben+ transgene in adults (Fig. 3C). Together with the observations that gross structural defects were not detected in the ben mutant, either in the adult brain (Figs. 3A, 5A, and data not shown) or at the NMJ (Fig. S1C, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), inducible rescue indicates a physiological, rather than a developmental, role for ben during LTM formation. So where in the adult brain is ben required to function during LTM formation? Although an antibody against Ben is not yet available, we nonetheless have been able to identify neurons likely to express BEN by assessing the common expression patterns of two reporter genes in the benP1 (LacZ) and ben4013 (Gal4). Immunostaining for LacZ and Gal4 revealed widespread coexpression in the CNS (Fig. 3A,B) and PNS (Fig. 6A). Given that the benGal4 transposon is inserted in the 5′ UTR of ben, this coexpression pattern likely identifies the cells in which BEN is expressed. Although this widespread expression pattern did not provide any anatomical clues for BEN's role in LTM formation, it did suggest that BEN may be expressed in the Kenyon cells of MB.
Extensive evidence has established the MB to be involved in olfactory memory formation (Heisenberg et al., 1985; Nighorn et al., 1991; Han et al., 1992; Skoulakis et al., 1993; de Belle and Heisenberg, 1994; Connolly et al., 1996; Han et al., 1996; Grotewiel et al., 1998; Cheng et al., 2001; Philip et al., 2001; Comas et al., 2004; Didelot et al., 2006; Folkers et al., 2006; Kim et al., 2007; Krashes et al., 2007; Lu et al., 2007; Qian et al., 2007; Xia and Tully, 2007), whereas some emerging evidence also has implicated the EB (Wu et al., 2007) (but see Krashes and Waddell, 2008). Based on these anatomical clues, we used the GAL4/GAL80ts system to restrict induced expression of ben+ to either MB or EB. The LTM defect of ben mutants was rescued with expression of a ben+ transgene in MBs (Fig. 3F–H) but not in EBs (Fig. 3I). Thus, ben may join tequila, yu, and chi as an LTM-specific gene with its relevant function localized to the MB (Didelot et al., 2006; Lu et al., 2007; Qian et al., 2007; cf. Keleman et al., 2007).
BEN and SCAMP function together during synaptic transmission at the larval NMJ
A bioinformatic search suggested that BEN and SCAMP may function together. SCAMPs are ubiquitous components of secretory vesicles in exocrine glands (Cameron et al., 1986; Brand et al., 1991; Laurie et al., 1993) and are present in recycling synaptic vesicles, where they function in clathrin-mediated vesicle budding via the asparagine-proline-phenylalamine tripeptide repeat domain (Brand et al., 1991; Brand and Castle, 1993; Hussain et al., 1999; Okamoto et al., 1999; Sengar et al., 1999; Fernandez-Chacon and Sudhof, 2000).
With this clue, we turned to the well characterized larval NMJ to assess a potential role for ben in synaptic function. We showed that BEN and SCAMP are coexpressed in some motor neurons (Fig. 6A). Immunostaining of SCAMP was weak, however, detecting Scamp in some motor neuron terminals but not in cell bodies in the larval ganglion (Fig. 6B). Development of synaptic structure appeared normal in the benP1 mutant (Fig. S1C, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), but a defect in evoked EJC was apparent in both ben (Fig. 4B) and Scamp homozygous mutants (Fig. 6C). Importantly, evoked EJCs also were defective in ben/Scamp double heterozygotes (Fig. 6C), a classic assessment for genetic epistasis (functional interaction).
Genetic dissection of developmental plasticity at the NMJ, in fact, continues to yield results generally parallel to both behavioral plasticity in Drosophila and in mammals (Saitoe, 2001). For example, increased neural activity or cAMP signaling can produce changes in both synaptic structure and function at the NMJ (Zhong and Wu, 1991; Zhong et al., 1992). Neural excitability activates JNK (c-Jun NH2-terminal kinase) and AP-1, which then induces cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB)-dependent changes in synaptic function (Davis et al., 1996; Sanyal et al., 2002, 2003; Etter et al., 2005) and CREB-independent, but ERK(extracellular signal-regulated kinase)- and ADF1-dependent, changes in synaptic structure (Schuster et al., 1996; DeZazzo et al., 2000; Hoeffer et al., 2003). Because disruption of ben leads to defects in synaptic function rather than structure, ben appears to fall into the CREB-dependent genetic pathway for synaptic plasticity.
BEN and SCAMP function together during LTM formation
We evaluated a potential epistatic interaction between BEN and SCAMP during LTM formation. Scamp or ben heterozygotes showed normal 1 d memory after spaced and massed training. Ben/Scamp double heterozygotes, however, displayed a defect in 1 d memory after spaced training but not after massed training (Fig. 5A) or when tested immediately after a single training session (Fig. 5B). Significantly, the LTM defect of the ben/Scamp double heterozygote was rescued by induced expression of the ben+ transgene (Fig. 5C), establishing that the functional interaction between ben and Scamp is physiological rather than developmental. To our knowledge, this is the first in vivo demonstration that Scamp is involved in behavioral plasticity.
Normal synaptic transmission in MB may be required for LTM formation
Our findings have established that (1) ben's function in MB is sufficient for LTM formation, (2) ben and Scamp work together during synaptic transmission at the larval NMJ, and (3) ben and Scamp cooperate during adult LTM formation. These observations suggest the hypothesis that proper synaptic transmission in MBs may be crucial to LTM formation. Results from several other studies are consistent with this notion. MBs display persistent oscillations in neural activity in naive flies, which are disrupted in amnesiac (memory) mutants (Rosay et al., 2001). Within MBs, persistent neural activity is branch specific (Yu et al., 2005, 2006). Output from α lobes appears required for LTM formation (Pascual and Preat, 2001; Isabel et al., 2004). Activity within the α′/β′ lobes is required for memory formation, whereas that in the α/β lobes is involved with retrieval (Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Yu et al., 2006; Krashes et al., 2007; Wu et al., 2007). Our results with ben conform to this emergent view. The adult LTM defect of the ben mutant is rescued via inducible expression of a ben+ transgene in either α/β lobes or α′/β′ lobes. This implies that ben is involved in the requisite synaptic transmission underlying both memory formation and retrieval. Thus, our failure to see transgenic rescue of memory retrieval in ben mutants resides in the fact that memory formation nonetheless remained disrupted in this experiment. Our results predict that other mutations of genes, which are involved in synaptic transmission and which are expressed in MBs, also may yield (1) LTM-specific defects and (2) MB-sufficient rescue thereof. Perhaps this cellular dysfunction (abnormal neural activity) is the unifying phenotype underlying all olfactory memory genes with critical function localized to the MB.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by the National Basic Research Program of China (973 #2005CB522503) to Z.X., by the National Basic Research Project (973 #2006CB500800), the Tsinghua-Yue-Yuen Medical Sciences Fund to Y.Z., and by Dart Neuroscience LLC to T.T. We are grateful to the Bloomington Stock Center (Indiana University, Bloomington, IN) for providing stocks EY08069 and EP1593, to the Kyoto Stock Center (Kyoto Institute of Technology, Kyoto, Japan) for providing 4013, and to Berkeley Drosophila Genome Project for providing plasmid SD04373.
- Correspondence should be addressed to either of the following: Dr. Tim Tully, Dart Neuroscience LLC, 7473 Lusk Boulevard, San Diego, CA 92075, ttully{at}dartneuroscience.com; or Dr. Zuoping Xie, Department of Biological Sciences and Technology, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China 100084, zuoping{at}tsinghua.edu.cn