Abstract
Nociception begins when Aδ- and C-nociceptors are activated. However, the processing of nociceptive input by the cortex is required before pain can be consciously experienced from nociception. To characterize the cortical activity related to the emergence of this experience, we recorded, in humans, laser-evoked potentials elicited by physically identical nociceptive stimuli that were either perceived or unperceived. Infrared laser pulses, which selectively activate skin nociceptors, were delivered to the hand dorsum either as a pair of rapidly succeeding and spatially displaced stimuli (two-thirds of trials) or as a single stimulus (one-third of trials). After each trial, subjects reported whether one or two distinct painful pinprick sensations, associated with Aδ-nociceptor activation, had been perceived. The psychophysical feedback after each pair of stimuli was used to adjust the interstimulus interval (ISI) of the subsequent pair: when a single percept was reported, ISI was increased by 40 ms; when two distinct percepts were reported, ISI was decreased by 40 ms. This adaptive algorithm ensured that the probability of perceiving the second stimulus of the pair tended toward 0.5. We found that the magnitude of the early-latency N1 wave was similar between perceived and unperceived stimuli, whereas the magnitudes of the later N2 and P2 waves were reduced when stimuli were unperceived. These findings suggest that the N1 wave represents an early stage of sensory processing related to the ascending nociceptive input, whereas the N2 and P2 waves represent a later stage of processing related, directly or indirectly, to the perceptual outcome of this nociceptive input.
Introduction
Nociception is defined as the neural process of encoding and processing noxious stimuli (Loeser and Treede, 2008). Although nociception is most often the cause of pain, it is not synonymous with pain, which is a conscious experience that can occur in the absence of nociception. It is now universally accepted that cortical activity is necessary for the generation of a painful experience (Bushnell and Apkarian, 2005). The cortical regions activated by nociceptive input are well described, particularly those that are consistently activated during pain (for example, the anterior insula) (Craig, 2009). However, the cortical activity specifically related to the conscious detection of nociceptive stimuli is widely undefined. Isolating such activity would thus represent a fundamental advance toward understanding the specific cortical processes through which pain emerges from nociception.
Brief radiant heat pulses, generated by infrared laser stimulators, may be used to excite selectively Aδ- and C-fiber free nerve endings located in the superficial layers of the skin (Bromm and Treede, 1984). Such stimuli elicit a number of electrical brain responses, some of which can be detected in the human EEG (Carmon et al., 1976; Mouraux et al., 2003). Although the laser stimulus coactivates several distinct ascending somatosensory pathways (Iannetti et al., 2003), the detected responses have been shown to be exclusively related to the activation of type-II Aδ mechano-heat nociceptors (Treede et al., 1995) and spinothalamic neurons located in the anterolateral quadrant of the spinal cord (Treede, 2003). Laser-evoked potentials (LEPs) comprise a number of waves that are time locked to the onset of the stimulus. The largest wave is a negative–positive complex maximal at the scalp vertex (N2–P2; peaking at 200–350 ms when stimulating the hand dorsum) (Bromm and Treede, 1984). This complex is preceded by a smaller negative wave (N1; peaking at ∼160 ms) maximal over the temporal region contralateral to the stimulated side (García-Larrea et al., 1997). Several studies have shown that these waves reflect a combination of cortical activities originating from primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, the insula, and the anterior cingulate cortex (García-Larrea et al., 2003).
Here, we devised a novel experimental paradigm to determine how the N1, N2, and P2 waves of the LEP relate to the perception of pain (see Fig. 1). The experiment relied on the premise that the interval between two identical sensory stimuli is an important determinant of whether or not the second stimulus is perceived: if two stimuli are delivered close in space using a short interstimulus interval (ISI), subjects are likely to perceive them as a single percept, whereas if two stimuli are delivered using a long ISI, subjects are likely to perceive them as two distinct percepts (Mouraux et al., 2004). Indeed, it is generally accepted that spatial proximity and temporal simultaneity are two main factors that determine whether physically separated stimuli are integrated as a unified percept (Meredith et al., 1987; King and Calvert, 2001). Here, we characterized the relationship between conscious perception and the magnitude of the N1, N2, and P2 waves by comparing the LEPs elicited by perceived and unperceived stimuli.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
Ten healthy, right-handed subjects participated in the study (four males and six females; aged between 24 and 38 years). Subjects were recruited from research staff and students at the University of Oxford (Oxford, UK). All participants gave written informed consent. The Oxfordshire Local Research Ethics Committee approved the study (REC: 07/Q1603/9).
Nociceptive stimulation.
Pulses of radiant heat were generated by an infrared neodymium:yttrium-aluminum-perovskite (Nd:YAP) laser with a wavelength of 1.34 μm (Electronical Engineering). The laser pulse was transmitted via an optic fiber and focused by lenses to a spot diameter of ∼8 mm at the target site. After each stimulus, the spot location was shifted by 20 mm in a random direction, to avoid nociceptor fatigue and sensitization. The spot location was controlled by a computer that used two servo-motors (HS-422; Hitec RCD; angular speed, 60°/160 ms) to orient the laser beam along two perpendicular axes (see supplemental video, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Each spot location was assigned a coordinate on a two-dimensional spatial grid (proximal–distal axis, ∼4 cm; medial–lateral axis, ∼4 cm) whose limits were adjusted to fit within the left-hand dorsum (see Fig. 2).
To familiarize subjects with the nociceptive stimulus, a small number of low-energy laser pulses were delivered to the left-hand dorsum. The energy of the stimulus was then adjusted to elicit a clear pricking pain sensation (2.4 ± 0.2 J), related to the activation of Aδ-nociceptors (Treede et al., 1995).
Experimental design.
In a previous study we showed that if two identical laser stimuli are applied close in space (i.e., ∼2 cm) and time (i.e., ISI < 600 ms), the likelihood of perceiving the second stimulus as a distinct percept is strongly reduced (Mouraux et al., 2004). In the present experiment, we took advantage of this psychophysical phenomenon to compare the brain responses elicited by a pair of physically identical laser stimuli, the second of which was either perceived or not perceived. For each participant, the ISI was adjusted continuously throughout the experiment, such that the likelihood of perceiving or not perceiving the second stimulus as a distinct percept tended to be equal. The experimental paradigm we used bears some resemblance to the attentional blink paradigm, in which two visual targets are presented within a continuous flow of nontarget stimuli (Raymond et al., 1992). Studies using such paradigms have shown that, when the time interval between the two target stimuli is short (typically <500 ms) and when the second target stimulus is quickly masked by a subsequent stimulus, the ability to detect the second target is significantly impaired (Giesbrecht and Di Lollo, 1998). This attentional blink phenomenon is thought to result from the early cortical representation of the second target being overwritten by the subsequent nontarget stimulus, while the central processes required for its consolidation are still engaged by the first target. In the present experiment, because the second stimulus of the pair was not followed by a subsequent masking stimulus, we did not expect to observe such a phenomenon, although both the paradigm we used and the attentional blink paradigm aim to identify neural activity related to the conscious detection of a sensory stimulus.
The experiment consisted of five blocks (Fig. 1). Each block was separated by a 5 min interval and consisted of 20 trials in which two stimuli were applied in rapid succession and 10 trials in which a single stimulus was applied. The intertrial interval was 10–15 s. Double-stimulus and single-stimulus trials were intermingled randomly. After each trial, the subject was asked to report whether one or two temporally distinct pricking sensations had been perceived. This specific instruction was required because the laser stimulus concomitantly activates Aδ- and C-nociceptors, thus eliciting a dual sensation of “first pain” and “second pain” (Lewis and Ponchin, 1937). First pain, related to the activation of Aδ-nociceptors, is characterized by a distinct pricking quality. Second pain, related to the activation of C-nociceptors, is characterized by a diffuse, long-lasting warm or burning sensation (Nahra and Plaghki, 2003). By asking subjects to detect a stimulus-triggered “pricking” sensation, we thus made sure that the sensation of second pain elicited by the first stimulus was not interfering with the detection of the first pain sensation elicited by the second stimulus. The ISI for the first double-stimulus trial was 500 ms. The ISI of the following double-stimulus trial was determined by the number of percepts elicited by the preceding double-stimulus trial: if a single pricking sensation was perceived (P1), the ISI was increased by 40 ms. If two pricking sensations were perceived (P2), the ISI was reduced by 40 ms.
This adaptive staircase algorithm ensured that the ISI of double-stimulus trials was maintained, throughout the experiment, at a value at which the likelihood of perceiving and not perceiving the second stimulus was equal. Importantly, the physical characteristics of both the first and the second laser stimulus were identical throughout the experiment. Hence, the likelihood of perceiving or not perceiving the second laser stimulus was unlikely to depend on differences in the incoming nociceptive input. The inclusion of single-stimulus trials reduced the effect of expectation on the EEG responses elicited by the second stimulus. Because single-stimulus trials were included, the overall probability of perceiving a second stimulus tended toward p = 0.33. Also, the number of percepts reported after a single-stimulus trial was used to assess whether the subjective reports of perception during double-stimulus trials were reliable (Table 1).
Control experiment.
To explore the intensity of the perceived sensation, we performed an additional experiment on six healthy, right-handed subjects (four males and two females; aged between 24 and 35 years). The same adaptive stimulation paradigm used during the LEP experiment was used for the control experiment (Fig. 1). Sixty trials (two blocks of 30 trials: 10 single-stimulus trials and 20 double-stimulus trials) were delivered to the dorsum of the right hand. After each trial, subjects were asked to report the number of perceived pricking sensations, as well as the intensity of each elicited pricking pain percept, using a numerical rating scale ranging from 0 to 10, where 0 was defined as “no pain” and 10 was defined as “pain as bad as it could be” (Jensen et al., 1989). The intensity ratings given by each subject was rescaled so that 0 and 100 represented the lowest and highest pain rating of every subject.
EEG recording.
All subjects were seated in a comfortable chair and wore protective goggles. They were asked to focus on the sensations that arose from hand stimulation, relax, and keep their eyes open and gaze slightly downward. Acoustic isolation was ensured using headphones through which white noise was played at comfortable listening level.
The EEG was recorded using 30 Ag–AgCl electrodes placed on the scalp according to the International 10–20 system, using the nose as common extracephalic reference. The electrooculogram was recorded using two electrodes; one placed at the upper left and the other at the lower right side of the right eye. The electrocardiogram was recorded using two electrodes placed at the left and right wrist. Signals were amplified and digitized using a sampling rate of 1024 Hz and a precision of 12 bits, giving a resolution of 0.195 μV (SD32; Micromed).
EEG analysis.
Analyses were performed using Letswave (http://amouraux.webnode.com) (Mouraux and Iannetti, 2008a), EEGLAB (http://sccn.ucsd.edu), and Matlab (Mathworks).
Continuous EEG recordings were segmented into epochs, −1 to 2 s relative to the onset of all stimuli. After off-line bandpass filtering (0.5–40 Hz fast Fourier transform filter), electrooculographic and electrocardiographic artifacts were removed using a validated method based on an independent component (IC) analysis (Jung et al., 2000). In all datasets, ICs related to eye movements had a large electrooculogram channel contribution and a frontal scalp distribution. The number of ICs removed from each dataset ranged from 2 to 4. Epochs were then baseline-corrected (reference interval, −1 to 0 s). Finally, epochs containing artifacts exceeding ±100 μV were rejected from additional analysis.
The spatial distributions of the stimulus location of unperceived (P1) and perceived (P2) double-stimulus trials were defined by the frequencies of trials occurring within bins of equal areas arranged in a 4 × 4 grid along the proximal–distal and medial–lateral axes of the hand dorsum (Fig. 2, left). An iterative procedure was then used to successively remove trials from the pools of P1 and P2 trials, until the relative frequency of trials did not differ by >5% in each bin (Fig. 2, right). A similar procedure was then performed to match the distribution of ISI across P1 and P2 double-stimulus trials. On average, this trial selection procedure led to the removal of 13 ± 4% of trials.
Finally, average waveforms were computed, time-locked to the onset of the first (S1) and the second (S2) stimulus of P1 and P2 double-stimulus trials, thus yielding four average waveforms for each subject: S1/P1, S1/P2, S2/P1, and S2/P2. For each waveform, the latency and the baseline-to-peak amplitude of N1, N2, and P2 waves were measured as follows. The N1 wave was measured at the temporal electrode contralateral to the stimulated side (T4), referenced to Fz. It was defined as the negative deflection preceding the N2 wave, which appears as a positive deflection in this montage. The N2 and P2 waves were measured at the vertex (Cz) referenced to the nose. The N2 wave was defined as the most negative deflection after stimulus onset. The P2 wave was defined as the most positive deflection after stimulus onset.
Statistical analyses.
Statistical comparisons were performed using Matlab (Mathworks).
A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was used to examine the main factors of “perception” (P1, second stimulus not perceived, vs P2, second stimulus perceived) and stimulus “repetition” (S1, first stimulus, vs S2, second stimulus), as well as the possible interaction between the two factors, on the amplitudes and latencies of the N1, N2, and P2 waves. When significant, post hoc paired t tests were used to perform pairwise comparisons.
Results
All subjects reported a clear painful pricking sensation to laser stimulation. The mean intensities of the first percept elicited by single- and double-stimulus trials and of the second percept elicited by double-stimulus trials were not significantly different (F = 0.88; p = 0.527) (supplemental Fig. S1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Table 1 shows the mean (± SD) ISI for all double-stimulus trials, as well as the percentage of single-stimulus trials that led to the perception of two stimuli. All stimuli elicited a negative wave (N2) followed by a positive wave (P2), both maximal at the vertex (Fig. 3). This complex was preceded by a smaller negative wave (N1) maximal over the temporal region contralateral to the stimulated side (Figs. 4, 5).
N1 wave
The N1 wave was clearly identified in the average waveforms of 9 of 10 subjects.
The mean (± SD) baseline-to-peak amplitude of the N1 wave elicited by the first stimulus of the pair (S1) was −2.7 ± 1.3 μV when the second stimulus was not perceived (S1/P1) and −3.5 ± 2.7 μV when it was perceived (S1/P2). The mean baseline-to-peak amplitude of the N1 wave elicited by the second stimulus of the pair (S2) was −1.5 ± 1.0 μV when the second stimulus was not perceived (S2/P1) and −1.9 ± 2.1 μV when it was perceived (S2/P2). There was no significant effect of the factor perception on the magnitude of the N1 waves elicited by the first and the second stimulus (p = 0.368), and there was no significant interaction between the effects of the factors perception and repetition (p = 0.350) (Table 2; Fig. 4, bottom left). Thus, the N1 wave elicited by the first and the second stimulus was not affected by whether or not the second stimulus was perceived. In contrast, the magnitude of the N1 wave was significantly affected by the factor repetition (p = 0.004) (Table 2; Fig. 5, bottom left): the N1 wave elicited by the second stimulus was significantly smaller than the N1 wave elicited by the first stimulus, independently of whether the second stimulus was perceived or not perceived (Table 3).
The mean peak latency of the N1 wave was 173 ± 21 ms for S1/P1, 168 ± 24 ms for S1/P2, 170 ± 23 ms for S2/P1, and 170 ± 21 ms for S2/P2. There were no significant main effects of the factors perception and repetition on the latency of the N1 waves, and there was no significant interaction between the factors perception and repetition.
N2 wave
The N2 wave was clearly identified in the average waveform of all subjects (Fig. 3).
The mean baseline-to-peak amplitude of the N2 wave elicited by the first stimulus of the pair (S1) was −9.9 ± 4.3 μV when the second stimulus was not perceived (S1/P1) and −10.8 ± 5.1 μV when the second stimulus was perceived (S1/P2). The mean baseline-to-peak amplitude of the N2 wave elicited by the second stimulus of the pair (S2) was −4.8 ± 2.1 μV when the second stimulus was not perceived (S2/P1) and −9.5 ± 4.7 μV when the second stimulus was perceived (S2/P2). There was a significant main effect of the factor perception on the magnitude of the N2 waves elicited by the first or the second stimulus (p = 0.015), and there was a significant interaction between the factors perception and repetition (p = 0.030) (Table 2; Fig. 4, top left). Post hoc comparisons revealed that, when the second stimulus was perceived, the magnitude of the N2 wave elicited by the second stimulus was enhanced, whereas the magnitude of the N2 wave elicited by the first stimulus was unchanged (Table 3). Furthermore, there was a significant main effect of the factor repetition on the magnitude of the N2 wave (p = 0.001) (Table 2; Fig. 5, top left): post hoc comparisons revealed that the N2 wave elicited by the second stimulus was significantly smaller than the N2 wave elicited by the first stimulus when the second stimulus was not perceived, but not when the second stimulus was perceived (Table 3).
The mean peak latency of the N2 wave was 236 ± 28 ms for S1/P1, 234 ± 18 ms for S1/P2, 219 ± 27 ms for S2/P1, and 215 ± 20 ms for S2/P2. There was no significant main effect of the factors perception and repetition on the latency of the N2 waves, and there was no significant interaction between the factors perception and repetition.
P2 wave
The P2 wave was identified in the average waveform of all subjects (Fig. 3).
The mean baseline-to-peak amplitude of the P2 wave elicited by the first stimulus of the pair (S1) was 8.6 ± 2.7 μV when the second stimulus was not perceived (S1/P1) and 11.8 ± 3.7 μV when the second stimulus was perceived (S1/P2). The mean baseline-to-peak amplitude of the P2 wave elicited by the second stimulus of the pair (S2) was 3.9 ± 2.0 μV when the second stimulus was not perceived (S2/P1) and 8.3 ± 3.1 μV when the second stimulus was perceived (S2/P2). There was a significant main effect of the factor perception on the magnitude of the P2 waves elicited by the first or the second stimulus (p = 0.002), but there was no significant interaction between the factors perception and repetition (p = 0.362) (Table 2; Fig. 4, top left). Thus, the P2 waves elicited by S1 and S2 were similarly affected by perception: when the second stimulus was perceived, both the magnitude of the P2 wave elicited by the first stimulus and the magnitude of the P2 wave elicited by the second stimulus were enhanced (Table 3). Furthermore, there was a significant main effect of the factor repetition on the magnitude of the P2 wave (p = 0.005) (Table 2; Fig. 5, top left): post hoc comparisons revealed that the P2 wave elicited by the second stimulus was significantly smaller than the P2 wave elicited by the first stimulus, independently of whether the second stimulus was perceived or not perceived (Table 3).
The mean peak latency of the P2 wave was 361 ± 35 ms for S1/P1, 360 ± 36 ms for S1/P2, 355 ± 60 ms for S2/P1, and 371 ± 46 ms for S2/P2. There was no significant main effect of the factors perception and repetition on the latency of the P2 waves, and there was no significant interaction between the factors perception and repetition.
Discussion
Nociceptive stimulation triggers a number of temporally distinct cortical processes, reflected by the N1, N2, and P2 waves of laser-evoked potentials. Here, by examining how these waves relate to the conscious detection of a nociceptive stimulus, we investigated the specific cortical processes through which pain emerges from nociception.
The study yielded four main findings. (1) The magnitude and latency of the N1 waves elicited by perceived and unperceived stimuli were similar. This indicates that the N1 represents an early stage of sensory processing during which the perceptual outcome of nociceptive input is not yet determined. (2) The magnitudes of both the N2 and the P2 waves elicited by perceived stimuli were significantly larger than those elicited by unperceived stimuli. This indicates that the N2 and P2 represent a later stage of sensory processing that is related to the perceptual outcome of nociceptive input. (3) The magnitude of the P2 wave elicited by the first stimulus predicted the conscious detection of the second stimulus (i.e., its magnitude was significantly greater in trials in which the subsequent second stimulus was perceived). This suggests that the state of responsiveness of the cortical generators of the P2 wave influences the conscious detection of nociceptive input. (4) The magnitudes of the N1, N2, and P2 waves were all significantly reduced by stimulus repetition, regardless of whether the stimulus was perceived. This suggests that the effect of stimulus repetition on nociceptive sensory processing does not require the conscious detection of the incoming sensory input.
The N1 wave reflects early stages of sensory processing, related to nociceptive input
The amplitude and latency of the N1 waves elicited by perceived and unperceived laser stimuli were similar (Fig. 4, bottom). This indicates that the N1 wave, which is thought to originate mainly from the operculo-insular region contralateral to the stimulated side and, possibly, from the contralateral primary somatosensory cortex (García-Larrea et al., 2003), reflects early stages of sensory processing that occur regardless of whether the nociceptive stimulus reaches conscious awareness. Since the physical characteristics of perceived and unperceived nociceptive stimuli were matched, our finding suggests that the magnitude of the N1 wave is mainly driven by the magnitude of the eliciting nociceptive input, rather than by the magnitude of pain perception. Consistent with this view, Baumgärtner et al. (2006) showed that the N1 wave can be recorded in anesthetized monkeys. Therefore, our results suggest that the correlation between the magnitude of the N1 wave and pain perception that was reported in two of our recent studies did not result from a relationship between the N1 wave and pain perception per se, but instead, was mostly determined by explicit (Iannetti et al., 2008) or unavoidable (Iannetti et al., 2005) variations in the strength of the peripheral nociceptive input.
Our finding is consistent with experimental observations reported in other sensory modalities. Indeed, studies examining the cortical processing of innocuous somatosensory stimuli (Libet et al., 1967; Meador et al., 2002; Schubert et al., 2006) have also shown that early cortical responses (e.g., the P60 and the N80 waves) are consistently elicited by near-threshold somatosensory stimuli regardless of whether they are perceived, whereas later responses (e.g., the N140 and the P250 wave) become prominent only when the stimulus is perceived.
The N2 and P2 waves reflect later stages of sensory processing, related to the perceptual outcome of the nociceptive input
In contrast to the behavior of the N1 wave, the magnitudes of the N2 and P2 waves elicited by perceived stimuli were significantly greater than the magnitudes of the N2 and P2 waves elicited by unperceived stimuli (Fig. 4). This indicates that the N2 and P2 waves reflect cortical processes related, directly or indirectly, to the conscious detection of nociceptive input, and agrees with the early findings by Carmon et al. (1978), showing that the magnitude of the laser-evoked N2 and P2 waves correlates better with the intensity of perception than with the intensity of the nociceptive stimulus per se.
It should be noted that unperceived stimuli were still able to elicit N2 and P2 waves, albeit of much reduced magnitude (Fig. 3) (see also the single-trial analysis reported in supplemental Fig. S2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). This observation suggests that not all cortical activities underlying the N2 and P2 waves reflect neural processes related to the conscious detection of the nociceptive stimulus. In agreement with this notion, it is well established that N2 and P2 waves do not originate from a single discrete cortical area, but from an extensive array of cortical areas including bilateral operculo-insular regions and the anterior cingulate cortex (García-Larrea et al., 2003), and there is no reason to suppose that all cortical areas contributing to the N2 and P2 waves are involved in the conscious detection of the nociceptive stimulus. Some regions (for example, the anterior insula) may play a greater role in stimulus awareness (Craig, 2009).
Interestingly, the magnitude of the P2 wave elicited by the first stimulus was significantly greater in trials in which the second stimulus was perceived compared with trials in which the second stimulus was not perceived. In other words, the magnitude of the P2 wave elicited by the first stimulus predicted the conscious detection of the second stimulus. This indicates that, during the time interval shortly preceding the second stimulus, at least some of the cortical generators of the P2 wave were more “responsive” to nociceptive input in those trials in which the second stimulus was perceived. Thus, it appears that the likelihood of perceiving the second stimulus was at least partly determined by spontaneous trial-to-trial fluctuations of the responsiveness of P2 cortical generators. What might be the functional significance of these trial-to-trial fluctuations? It is well known that a great part of the variability of any given cortical response can be attributed to the spontaneous dynamics of ongoing activity (Arieli et al., 1996; Fiser et al., 2004), and several studies have shown that the level of arousal is an important determinant of the magnitude of the P2 wave of LEPs (Beydoun et al., 1993; Legrain et al., 2002), as well as of stimulus detection and discrimination performance (Posner, 1980; Parasuraman, 1984). It is important to note that, although trial-to-trial fluctuations caused by mind wandering could account fully for the relationship between perception and the magnitude of the P2 wave elicited by the second stimulus, the same does not apply to the N2 wave. Indeed, perceiving or not perceiving the second stimulus conditioned the magnitude of the N2 wave elicited by the second stimulus, but not the magnitude of the N2 wave elicited by the first stimulus. This indicates that, distinct from the P2 wave, the N2 wave reflects processes that are related to the perceptual outcome of the stimulus, independently of spontaneous trial-to-trial fluctuations of the participant's attentional state.
Other studies have shown that the electrocortical activity preceding the onset of a stimulus (e.g., the magnitude of prestimulus alpha-band EEG oscillations) (Babiloni et al., 2006, 2008; Zhang et al., 2008) can predict sensory-discriminative performance as well as the magnitude of subsequent stimulus-evoked potentials. This relationship has also been hypothesized to reflect differences in the responsiveness of the cortical networks relevant to processing the stimulus.
Finally, it is important to note that the observed relationship between the magnitude of the N2 and P2 waves and the perceptual outcome of the nociceptive stimulus does not imply that these waves necessarily reflect cortical activity specific for the processing of nociceptive input (Mouraux and Iannetti, 2009).
The effect of stimulus repetition
The magnitudes of the N1, N2, and P2 waves were all significantly reduced by stimulus repetition (Fig. 5). Compared with the first stimulus, the second stimulus of the pair elicited N1, N2, and P2 waves of significantly smaller magnitude. The reduction in the magnitude of the EEG responses elicited by a repeated stimulus, often referred to as “repetition suppression” (Grill-Spector et al., 2006), occurs when identical stimuli are presented at a short (e.g., <2 s) and constant ISI (Hari et al., 1982; Angel et al., 1985; Raij et al., 2003; Iannetti et al., 2008). It has been conclusively demonstrated that this phenomenon is not attributable to neural refractoriness, but instead may result from the increased predictability of the repeated stimulus (Mouraux et al., 2004; Sable et al., 2004; Mouraux and Iannetti, 2008b). Indeed, when pairs of stimuli are presented at unpredictable ISIs, the magnitude of the response elicited by the second stimulus is entirely unaltered, even at intervals as short as 100 ms (Budd and Michie, 1994; Mouraux et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2008). In the present experiment, the effect of stimulus repetition affected equally the N1, N2, and P2 waves, and was present regardless of whether the second stimulus was perceived. This suggests that the neural mechanisms underlying repetition suppression do not require the conscious detection of the incoming sensory input, and would thus agree with the notion that repetition suppression reflects a preconscious mechanism preventing nonsalient sensory inputs from reaching consciousness (Sable et al., 2004).
Conclusion
Our results indicate that early-latency electrocortical responses to noxious stimulation (i.e., the N1 wave) reflect neural activity more related to the ascending nociceptive input, whereas later responses (i.e., the N2 and P2 waves) reflect functionally distinct neural activity more related to the emergence of pain from nociception.
This finding has a number of important implications, both in clinical practice and drug discovery. It indicates that the N1, N2, and P2 waves of LEPs can be used as functionally distinct biomarkers to explore neuropathic pain mechanisms (for example, the peripheral and central mechanisms thought to contribute to pain in fibromyalgia) (Lorenz et al., 1996). Furthermore, it indicates that LEPs can be used to characterize novel analgesics based on their modulation of nociceptive transmission and processing at peripheral and central levels.
Footnotes
-
M.C.L. was supported by the United Kingdom Medical Research Council. A.M. is a Marie Curie Postdoctoral Research Fellow and a “Chargé de Recherche” of the Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research. G.D.I. is University Research Fellow of The Royal Society.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Gian Domenico Iannetti, Department of Physiology, Anatomy and Genetics, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3QX, UK. giandomenico.iannetti{at}dpag.ox.ac.uk