Abstract
Identification of shared features between avian and mammalian auditory brainstem circuits has provided much insight into the mechanisms underlying early auditory processing. However, previous studies have highlighted an apparent difference in inhibitory systems; synaptic inhibition is thought to be slow and GABAergic in birds but to have fast kinetics and be predominantly glycinergic in mammals. Using patch-clamp recordings in chick brainstem slices, we found that this distinction is not exclusively true. Consistent with previous work, IPSCs in nucleus magnocellularis (NM) were slow and mediated by GABAA receptors. However, IPSCs in nucleus laminaris (NL) and a subset of neurons in nucleus angularis (NA) had rapid time courses twofold to threefold faster than those in NM. Furthermore, we found that IPSCs in NA were mediated by both glycine and GABAA receptors, demonstrating for the first time a role for fast glycinergic transmission in the avian auditory brainstem. Although NM, NL, and NA have unique roles in auditory processing, the majority of inhibitory input to each nucleus arises from the same source, ipsilateral superior olivary nucleus (SON). Our results demonstrate remarkable diversity of inhibitory transmission among the avian brainstem nuclei and suggest that differential glycine and GABAA receptor activity tailors inhibition to the specific functional roles of NM, NL, and NA despite common SON input. We additionally observed that glycinergic/GABAergic activity in NA was usually depolarizing and could elicit spiking activity in NA neurons. Because NA projects to SON, these excitatory effects may influence the recruitment of inhibitory activity in the brainstem nuclei.
Introduction
The vertebrate auditory system uses several environmental cues to determine the location of sound sources, including the relative timing and intensity of acoustic signals between the two ears (Konishi, 2003). In both birds and mammals, specialized circuits devoted to these distinct sound characteristics are established in the brainstem. In birds, auditory nerve fibers diverge to innervate nucleus magnocellularis (NM) and nucleus angularis (NA) (Parks and Rubel, 1978). Neurons in NM bilaterally relay precise timing information to targets in nucleus laminaris (Boord, 1968; Parks and Rubel, 1975; Fukui et al., 2006). Nucleus laminaris (NL) neurons rely on these signals to compute submillisecond differences in sound arrival time between each ear that arise when a sound originates closer to one side of the head, termed interaural timing differences (ITDs) (Carr and Konishi, 1990; Konishi, 2003). In contrast, NA is thought to represent the first stage in the processing of sound intensity, as well as other nontiming aspects of auditory signals (Sullivan and Konishi, 1984; Takahashi et al., 1984; MacLeod and Carr, 2007).
Examination of the intrinsic properties of auditory brainstem neurons as well as their excitatory inputs have revealed similar anatomical and biophysical specializations in the avian and mammalian brainstem nuclei, indicating shared principles for encoding sound (Oertel, 1999; Trussell, 1999; Carr and Soares, 2002). However, investigations of inhibitory synaptic transmission suggested a striking distinction between birds and mammals. In mammals, inhibition undergoes a developmental switch from GABAergic in immature animals to predominantly glycinergic transmission in the adult (Kotak et al., 1998; Smith et al., 2000; Nabekura et al., 2004; Awatramani et al., 2005). Glycinergic currents in the mammalian brainstem typically have rapid kinetics, with decay time constants of only a few milliseconds or less (Smith et al., 2000; Awatramani et al., 2004; Magnusson et al., 2005), and can retain the phase-locked temporal structure of auditory signals. In contrast, inhibition in the avian auditory brainstem is thought to be GABAergic throughout development (Code and Rubel, 1989; von Bartheld et al., 1989; Lachica et al., 1994). In NM, in which IPSCs have slow kinetics, GABA release becomes asynchronous at high rates of activity and IPSCs summate to mediate tonic inhibition (Lu and Trussell, 2000). The anatomy of inhibition is also unique in birds; inhibition to NM, NL, and NA arises predominantly from the same source, ipsilateral superior olivary nucleus (SON) (Lachica et al., 1994; Monsivais et al., 2000; Burger et al., 2005), which is not thought to provide phase-locked inhibition to its targets (Lachica et al., 1994; Yang et al., 1999). SON receives excitatory projections from ipsilateral NL and NA. These distinctions have led to the suggestion that inhibition serves fundamentally different roles in birds and mammals (Grothe, 2003). We tested this generalization by examining inhibitory transmission in NM, NL, and NA neurons in slices of embryonic chick brainstem.
Materials and Methods
Tissue preparation.
Coronal slices of brainstem (250 μm thick) were prepared from 17- to 20-d-old chick embryos [embryonic day 17 (E17) to E20]. In experiments using hatchling chicks (post-hatch day 0), chicks were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane before they were killed. After decapitation, a portion of brainstem containing the auditory nuclei was blocked in the coronal plane and affixed to the stage of a vibratome (Leica VT1000S or VT1200S) and then sectioned. During dissection and slicing, tissue remained immersed in an oxygenated saline solution containing the following (in mm): 140 NaCl, 5 KCl, 3 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 10 HEPES, and 10 glucose, pH 7.4 using NaOH (warmed to ∼35°C) (300 mOsm). After sectioning, slices were allowed to recover in the same solution at 35°C for 1 h and then transferred to a recording chamber or maintained at room temperature (∼22°C) until use.
Electrophysiology.
During recordings, slices were continuously perfused with oxygenated saline solution identical to that used for slice preparation, but with the addition of 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (DNQX) (20 μm) and dl-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (dl-APV) (100 μm). For all experiments, bath solution was warmed to 35 ± 1°C using an in-line heater (Warner Instruments). Bath temperature was constantly monitored with a thermister positioned at the tip of the microscope objective. Cells were visualized for recordings on the stage of an upright microscope (Olympus BX51W) using infrared Dodt gradient contrast optics and a 60× magnification objective. Neurons in NM, NL, and NA were identified by cellular morphology and location within the slice. Recordings from NM and NL were obtained from the caudal one-third of the nuclei, corresponding to the middle to low characteristic frequency regions (Rubel and Parks, 1975). In this study, we did not explore whether tonotopic variation in IPSC properties exists in NM and NL. In NA, cells were recorded throughout the extent of the nucleus. For whole-cell voltage-clamp experiments, patch pipettes were filled with one of two CsCl-based internal solutions. One contained the following (in mm): 130 CsCl, 2 MgCl2, 10 tetraethylammonium (TEA)-Cl, 10 HEPES, 4 BAPTA-Cs4, and 4 Mg-ATP, pH 7.2 with CsOH (290 mOsm). The other consisted of the following (in mm): 120 CsCl, 1.4 MgCl2, 10 TEA-Cl, 0.4 Na2-GTP, 10 Tris2-phosphocreatine, 4 Mg-ATP, 10 HEPES, and 4 BAPTA-Cs4, pH 7.2 with CsOH (290 mOsm). QX-314 [N-(2,6-dimethylphenylcarbamoylmethyl)triethylammonium chloride] (1 mm) was sometimes included in the internal solution. IPSCs recorded with either internal solution and with or without QX-314 did not exhibit different decay kinetics or pharmacology, so data using the different CsCl-based internal solutions were pooled. To confirm that strychnine blocked a Cl−-mediated current in NA neurons (see Results), some recordings were obtained using a low [Cl−] internal solution containing the following (in mm): 108 CsMeSO3, 5 CsCl, 1.4 MgCl2, 0.4 Na-GTP, 15 Tris2-phosphocreatine, 4 Mg-ATP, 10 HEPES, and 8 BATPA-Cs4, pH 7.2 with CsOH (290 mOsm). These measurements were not included in the reported averages. To assess the action potential firing phenotype of NA neurons, recordings from a subset of NA cells were acquired using a K+-based internal solution containing the following (in mm): 81 K-gluconate, 32 KCl, 4.5 MgCl2, 14 Tris2-phosphocreatine, 4 Na2-ATP, 0.3 Tris-GTP, 9 HEPES, 0.1 EGTA, and 0.2 sucrose, pH 7.2 with KOH (290 mOsm). All recording pipettes were pulled from borosilicate glass (WPI) and had a resistance of 1.5–3 MΩ when filled with CsCl-based internal solution. Reported membrane potential values were corrected for empirically determined junction potentials of −5.9 and −11.4 mV for CsCl-based and K+-based internal solutions, respectively. IPSCs were evoked by locally applying electrical stimuli (<50 μm from soma; 20–60 V, 100–200 μs) through double-barreled glass pipettes (5–10 μm tip diameter) filled with bath solution. In experiments examining evoked IPSCs using the CsCl-based internal solutions, the membrane potential of the recorded cell was held at −35.9 mV. Evoked IPSCs were recorded at −71.4 mV in experiments using the K+-based pipette solution. Spontaneously occurring IPSCs (sIPSCs) were measured while the membrane potential of the recorded cell was maintained at −65.9 mV. For gramicidin perforated-patch recordings, pipettes were filled with a solution containing the following: 140 mm KCl, 10 mm NaCl, 10 mm HEPES, 60–100 μg of gramicidin D, and 50 μm Alexa Fluor 488. After seal formation, series resistance (Rs) was monitored in voltage-clamp mode by applying a 10 mV voltage step across the pipette tip. Experiments were initiated once Rs declined to 20–50 MΩ, which typically took 20–30 min. The integrity of the membrane under the recording electrode was confirmed periodically by checking that Alexa Fluor 488 signal was excluded from the recording pipette. Gramicidin perforated-patch measurements were not corrected for junction potential (Kim and Trussell, 2007). Glycine (1 mm in bath solution) was pressure-applied using a picospritzer (100 ms, ∼1–2 psi application; 1 psi = 6.89 kPa; Parker Instrumentation) through a patch pipette positioned adjacent to the soma of the recorded cell. SR-95531 [6-imino-3-(4-methoxyphenyl)-(6H)-pyridazinebutanoic acid hydrobromide] and strychnine were applied by bath perfusion. Pharmacological agents were obtained from Ascent Scientific except for strychnine, which was from Sigma-Aldrich. Extracellular and intracellular solution components were obtained from Sigma with the following exceptions: BAPTA-Cs4 and Alexa Fluor 488 were from Invitrogen, and QX-314 was from Alomone Labs.
Electrophysiological data acquisition and analysis.
Recordings were acquired using a MultiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices) and pClamp 10.0 software. Signals were digitized at 20 or 50 kHz using a Digidata 1322A (Molecular Devices) and low-pass filtered at 10 kHz. For whole-cell voltage-clamp experiments, series resistance (<15 MΩ) was compensated 60–80% during recordings. Series resistance was left uncompensated during perforated-patch recordings and was instead monitored continuously throughout the experiments by measuring the peak of the instantaneous current response to 5–10 mV voltage step commands. Membrane potential values for perforated-patch recordings were corrected offline by subtracting the voltage error, calculated by multiplying holding current by series resistance, from the command holding potential. Analyses were conducted using Clampfit 10.0 (Molecular Devices) or custom procedures written in IgorPro (WaveMetrics) (procedures written by M. T. Roberts, University of Texas, Austin, TX). The decay phases of evoked IPSCs were fit with exponential functions with one, two, or three components. The best fit was selected by comparing the sum of squared errors between fits with different numbers of exponential components. We selected those fits that resulted in a sum of squared errors of less than half that of a fit with fewer components. Spontaneous IPSCs were detected and aligned using the variable-amplitude template function event detection feature of Axograph X. Because of the small amplitudes of many of the spontaneous IPSCs, we determined their time courses by normalizing all events to their peak amplitudes and fitting a single-exponential function to decay phases of the IPSCs. Fits obtained in this manner were visually inspected, and events in which we were unable to satisfactorily fit the data, which represented a small minority of the total number of events, were removed. All reported values are mean ± SD. Unless noted otherwise, statistical significance (p < 0.05) was tested using unpaired two-tailed Student's t tests.
Immunohistochemistry.
Chicks were transcardially perfused with warm (∼40°C) PBS solution (0.1 m), pH 7.4, followed by ice-cold 1% formaldehyde and 2% glutaraldehyde in PBS. The brains were removed from the skulls, rinsed in PBS, and sectioned at 30 μm on a vibratome. After sectioning, the tissue was washed and then incubated in 1% sodium borohydride in PBS for 30 min at room temperature to reduce background glutaraldehyde fluorescence. After extensive washing, the tissue was incubated in block solution consisting of 2% normal goat serum, 1% bovine serum albumin (BSA), and 0.1% saponin in PBS for 1 h at room temperature, followed by overnight 4°C coincubation with primary antibodies to glycine (rabbit anti-glycine; 1:200 or 1:1000; AB139, lots 0610044052 and LV1508357; Millipore Corporation) and GABA (mouse anti-GABA; 1:10,000; mAB 3A12, lot ps2; Swant) diluted in block solution. Both antibodies were raised to their respective target neurotransmitter conjugated to BSA by glutaraldehyde and have been used previously in chick nervous tissue (Matute and Streit, 1986; Kalloniatis and Fletcher, 1993). After primary antibody incubation, sections were washed and incubated for 2 h at room temperature with fluorescence-conjugated secondary antibodies (Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-mouse and Alexa Fluor 568 or 633 goat anti-rabbit; 1:500; Invitrogen). After washing, sections were mounted on slides, dehydrated in ascending alcohols, and delipidized in xylenes. The tissue was then rehydrated and coverslipped using Fluoromount G medium (Southern Biotechnology Associates).
Antibody-specificity controls.
Preadsorption control experiments were performed to confirm specificity of the anti-Gly and anti-GABA antibodies under the fixation and tissue preparation conditions used in this study (supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Anti-Gly or anti-GABA antibodies were preincubated with glycine–BSA or GABA–BSA conjugates (Abcam) for 24–48 h at 4°C before application to tissue sections. Preadsorption control tissue was compared in parallel with tissue from the same animal in which the antibodies were applied without preadsorption. Preincubation of anti-Gly antibody with a 100-fold molar excess of Gly–BSA protein eliminated glycine-like immunoreactivity (supplemental Fig. 1A,B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Likewise, preincubation of anti-GABA antibody with a 100-fold molar excess of GABA–BSA protein resulted in a greatly reduced GABA-like immunoreaction (supplemental Fig. 1E,F, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). When anti-Gly was preincubated with the same amount of GABA–BSA used to block the anti-GABA reaction, glycine-like labeling was preserved (supplemental Fig. 1C,D, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), and preincubation of anti-GABA with the same amount of Gly–BSA used to block anti-Gly labeling did not inhibit anti-GABA labeling (supplemental Fig. 1G,H, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Background fluorescence was assessed in all immunohistochemistry experiments by examining tissue processed identically to experimental samples but in which primary antibody was omitted.
Image acquisition and analysis.
Fluorescence images were acquired using a confocal microscope (Olympus FV1000) by sequential scanning of Alexa Fluor 488 and Alexa Fluor 568 or Alexa Fluor 633 signals using an oil-immersion objective (60× magnification, numerical aperture 1.42). Lack of crosstalk between fluorescence channels was confirmed by examining tissue labeled with only one of each of the secondary antibodies. Image analysis was conducted using NIH ImageJ software. Overlap between anti-Gly and anti-GABA signals was quantified in single confocal sections acquired at a depth of ∼1.5–2 μm below the surface of the tissue from regions within each brainstem nucleus using methods similar to those previously used by Muller et al. (2006). After applying a median filter to reduce noise, images from the different fluorescence channels were separately thresholded by eye. A binary mask was then applied to each thresholded image, and overlapping regions between the binary images representing the different immunofluorescent signals were identified. The total area of overlap for each analyzed image was determined and expressed as a percentage of the total area of anti-GABA or anti-Gly signals.
Results
Evoked IPSCs exhibit region-specific decay kinetics
To compare inhibitory synaptic transmission between NM, NL, and NA, we obtained whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings from visually identified neurons within each region in tissue slices of auditory brainstem. IPSCs were evoked by extracellular stimulation and measured in the presence of ionotropic glutamate receptor antagonists (20 μm DNQX, 100 μm dl-APV).
The durations of stimulus-evoked IPSCs were distinct between the different nuclei. Although rise times were similar for IPSCs recorded in NM, NL, and NA [Table 1, only significant difference between NM and NL (p = 0.05)], the decay time courses were distinct among the different nuclei. IPSCs recorded in NM decayed approximately threefold slower than those in NL (Fig. 1, Table 1). In NA, IPSCs typically had a fast time course, similar to those recorded in NL. However, slower currents with kinetics resembling NM IPSCs were also observed (Fig. 1, Table 1). The decay phases of NM IPSCs were best fit by the sum of two (n = 4) or three (n = 7) exponentials (see Materials and Methods). Weighted time constants were not significantly different between biexponential and three-component exponential fits (p = 0.12). The mean weighted time constant for IPSCs recorded in NM neurons was 26.0 ± 5.0 ms. NL IPSC decays were best fit by the sum of two exponentials with a weighted time constant of 8.1 ± 2.1 ms. Fast NA IPSCs were similar in time course to NL IPSCs, with a weighted decay time constant of 11.6 ± 9.0 ms. Slow NA IPSCs (3 of 15 cells) were best fit by the sum of three exponentials and had a weighted time constant 23.1 ± 5.6 ms. Identical IPSC time courses were also observed in tissue from hatchling chicks [postnatal day 0 (P0)]. In hatchlings, the mean weighted decay time constants for IPSCs were 25.6 ± 6.2 ms in NM (n = 4), 8.8 ± 2.8 ms in NL (n = 4), and 12.0 ± 5.7 ms in NA (n = 5). Although maximal IPSC peak amplitudes varied widely within each nucleus, IPSCs in NA were on average smaller than those in NM and NL (Table 1, p < 0.01 for NA compared with NM or NL), similar to previous observations of smaller excitatory currents in NA compared with NM and NL (MacLeod and Carr, 2005; MacLeod et al., 2007).
Summary of IPSC properties in chick auditory brainstem
Divergent time courses of inhibition in chick auditory brainstem nuclei. Example stimulus-evoked IPSC recordings from neurons in nucleus magnocellularis (A, B), nucleus laminaris (C, D), and nucleus angularis (E, F). A, C, E, Individual, overlaid current traces from stimuli applied at 0.2 or 0.5 Hz. B, D, F, Averaged current traces (10–20 stimuli). Gray lines are biexponential fits to the current decay phases. Stimulus artifacts in this and subsequent figures have been blanked for clarity.
NA is a heterogeneous nucleus composed of several types of neurons with distinct intrinsic firing characteristics (Soares et al., 2002; Fukui and Ohmori, 2003). We therefore recorded from some NA neurons using a K+-based pipette solution so that NA neurons could be characterized in current-clamp configuration before recording IPSCs. We divided neurons into two categories: those that fired only one or two spikes at the beginning of depolarizing current injection (hereafter termed “onset spiking”) and those that fired multiple action potentials over the duration of positive current injection (“multiple spiking”). Previous work has subdivided repetitively firing NA neurons into several categories (Soares et al., 2002), but we grouped all non-onset firing cells together as multiple spiking neurons because we observed they could exhibit different firing patterns depending on their initial membrane potential. Furthermore, Fukui and Ohmori (2003) found that NA neurons could be classified as either tonically firing or onset firing neurons in tissue from post-hatch chicks. Evoked IPSCs recorded from onset firing or multiple spiking NA neurons had different time courses (Fig. 2). On average, onset spiking neurons had IPSCs that were significantly faster than those in multiple spiking neurons (weighted τdecay, 2.7 ± 0.8 and 16.7 ± 8.9 ms, respectively; p < 0.001). The population of multiple spiking neurons exhibited a range of IPSC decay times (weighted τdecay ranged from 7.6 to 28.6 ms). Although the IPSC decay values cannot be directly compared between cells recorded using the CsCl-based internal solution and the K+- based internal because of the different pipette solutions and holding potentials used (−35.9 mV for CsCl experiments, −71.4 mV for K+-gluconate recordings), it is likely that the slow IPSCs measured using the CsCl-based internal solution were recorded from multiple spiking neurons, whereas the fast IPSCs were recorded from both onset and multiple spiking neurons.
Unique IPSC decay kinetics between onset spiking and multiple spiking neurons in NA. A, Response to −200 and +600 pA current injection in an example onset spiking neuron. B, Average evoked IPSC in same cell as in A. C, Multiple spiking neuron response to −200 and +600 pA injection. D, Evoked IPSC measured from same cell shown in C. Gray lines in B and D show exponential fit to IPSC decay phase.
Distinct pharmacology between the timing and intensity pathways
We tested the neurotransmitter phenotype of evoked IPSCs by bath application of SR-95531 (10–20 μm) and strychnine (0.5 μm), which are antagonists of GABAA and glycine receptors, respectively. For IPSCs recorded in NA, we unexpectedly observed that SR-95531 application only eliminated a fraction of the inhibitory current recorded in every NA neuron tested (n = 12). Figure 3A shows an example cell in which IPSCs were evoked under control conditions (black trace), then in the presence of SR-95531 (red), then subsequently during application of strychnine after washout of SR-95531 (blue), and finally in the presence of both SR-95531 and strychnine (purple). Neither SR-95531 nor strychnine application alone completely blocked evoked currents, but both drugs together eliminated IPSCs, demonstrating the existence of GABAergic and glycinergic components of the inhibitory currents. In addition to its well characterized effect on glycine receptors, strychnine antagonizes some nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subtypes at the concentrations used in this study (Matsubayashi et al., 1998). To eliminate the possibility that the strychnine-sensitive current in NA neurons was mediated by nicotinic receptors, we performed some recordings using a low [Cl−] internal solution [CsMeSO3-based solution (see Materials and Methods); predicted ECl− = −82 mV]. Outward currents were recorded at a holding potential of −30 mV in both the presence and absence of SR-95531 under these conditions (data not shown; n = 4), consistent with both components of the evoked current being mediated by Cl−-conducting channels.
IPSCs recorded from NA neurons have both GABAergic and glycinergic components. A, Example stimulus-evoked IPSCs recorded from an NA neuron under control conditions (black), in the presence of SR-95531 (10 μm; red), after SR-95531 washout (gray), after application of strychnine (0.5 μm; blue), and in the presence of both SR-95531 and strychnine (purple) are shown above. The current traces show data from the time points indicated by the same color data points in the graph below. Each data point in the graph represents the average peak amplitude of six consecutive sweeps (data acquired at 0.1 Hz) from one example cell. B, Summary of weighted decay kinetics for IPSCs in NA neurons for mixed glycinergic/GABAergic IPSCs recorded under control conditions, glycinergic currents isolated by SR-95531 application, and GABAergic currents isolated by application of strychnine, or digital subtraction of currents recorded in SR-95531 from control IPSCs. Inset shows peak scaled IPSCs for same responses shown in A. C, Comparison of peak current of IPSCs in NM (n = 4), NL (n = 4), and NA (n = 5) neurons before and during exposure to SR-95531 (10–20 μm).
As summarized in Figure 3B, glycinergic and GABAergic components of IPSCs recorded in NA neurons had distinct kinetics. The decay phases of the glycinergic component of evoked IPSCs, isolated by recording in the presence of SR-95531, were best fit by the sum of two exponentials (τ1 = 1.8 ± 0.4 ms; τ2 = 5.0 ± 0.7 ms; % τ1 = 41.7 ± 3.4%), except for two cells whose decays were fit adequately with a single-exponential function (τ = 2.1 and 3.8 ms; mean τdecay for all cells = 3.1 ± 0.6 ms). The GABAergic component of evoked IPSCs, isolated pharmacologically using strychnine or by digital subtraction of traces acquired during SR-95531 application from control current traces, had decays best fit with the sum of two exponentials (τ1 = 6.0 ± 1.4 ms; τ2 = 35.1 ± 4.2 ms; % τ1 = 63.5 ± 8.6%), with a weighted time constant of 16.9 ± 3.8 ms.
In agreement with previous findings, evoked IPSCs in NM and NL were blocked ≥95% during bath application of SR-95531 (10–20 μm) (Fig. 3C) (Funabiki et al., 1998; Yang et al., 1999; Lu and Trussell, 2000; Monsivais et al., 2000; Lu et al., 2005; Howard et al., 2007). In the five NA neurons recorded with the CsCl-based pipette solution, SR-95531 reduced the peak amplitude of the control IPSC by 47.6 ± 14.2% (range of 33.6–68.4%) (Fig. 3C).
Evidence for corelease of GABA and glycine in NA
In NA, the glycinergic and GABAergic components of evoked IPSCs that we observed could arise from the simultaneous recruitment of distinct inhibitory fibers or from corelease of both neurotransmitters from the same presynaptic neuron (Jonas et al., 1998; Awatramani et al., 2005). To distinguish between these possibilities, we recorded sIPSCs from NA neurons under control conditions and in the presence of SR-95531 or strychnine. Because these sIPSCs represented a mixture of random vesicle fusion events (miniature IPSCs) and spontaneous action potential evoked release, this approach allowed us to monitor activity of single presynaptic fibers and therefore address whether GABA and glycine can be released from the same presynaptic neuron. Figure 4A–C shows example peak amplitude-scaled control sIPSCs and those recorded during SR-95531 or strychnine application for one NA neuron. A summary of IPSC decay kinetics for each condition from four NA neurons is presented in Figure 4D. In control conditions, the mean decay time constants for sIPSCs ranged from 1.0 to 40.2 ms, with a mean time constant of 7.8 ± 2.9 ms (Fig. 4A,D). Glycinergic sIPSCs recorded in the presence of SR-95531 had fast decay kinetics (mean τdecay = 2.9 ± 1.2 ms; range of 0.9–8.9 ms) (Fig. 4B,D). The duration of GABAergic sIPSCs isolated by strychnine application exhibited a range of values that were on average slower than glycinergic IPSCs (mean τdecay = 11.7 ms ± 4.3 ms; range of 1.6–43.6 ms) (Fig. 4C,D). The broad distribution of sIPSC decay kinetics recorded in the presence of strychnine and overlap with decay kinetics of events recorded in SR-95531 prevented a quantitative assessment of the relative contributions of GABA and glycine-mediated transmission to spontaneous events. However, the significant shift in the overall population of τdecay values recorded under control conditions to faster or slower values in the presence of SR-95531 or strychnine, respectively (Fig. 4D) (p < 0.0001, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test), indicates that a considerable number of control sIPSCs arose from the release of both GABA and glycine from the same axon. If most IPSCs arose from release of GABA only or glycine only, under control conditions, we should have observed two separate populations of decays with means matching those recorded in SR-95531 or strychnine, which would have appeared as a biphasic curve in the cumulative probability plot. The observation that SR-95531 or strychnine application significantly reduced the amplitudes of sIPSCs (Fig. 4E) (p < 0.0001, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test) also supports this interpretation.
Spontaneous IPSCs in NA indicate that GABA and glycine are coreleased. A–C, Peak-normalized average spontaneous IPSC (thick colored line) and 20 individual spontaneous IPSCs (gray lines) recorded in control conditions (A) and in the presence of strychnine (B) or SR-95531 (C) from the same NA neuron (average IPSC from 540 events in A, 150 events in B, and 56 events in C). D, Cumulative histograms of IPSC decay times for sIPSCs recorded in control (black), strychnine (red), and SR-95531 (blue). Mean ± SD of distributions for each experimental condition from four NA cells are shown. Inset shows peak-scaled average sIPSCs under the different conditions for the single NA cell shown in A–C. sIPSC decay time distributions were significantly different between the conditions (p < 0.0001, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). E, Cumulative distributions of sIPSC peak amplitudes under control conditions (black) and in the presence of strychnine (red) or SR-95531 (blue) (4 cells; mean ± SD). Peak amplitudes of events in control conditions were significantly different from those in either drug (p < 0.0001, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test).
As a complementary approach for investigating whether GABA and glycine arise from the same or different sources, we examined the expression patterns of these neurotransmitters in the chick auditory brainstem using immunohistochemistry. Double-immunofluorescent labeling with a GABA-specific antibody in combination with a glycine-specific antibody revealed punctate expression of both neurotransmitters around cell bodies and in the neuropil of NA (Fig. 5A). These puncta likely correspond to inhibitory nerve terminals contacting NA neurons. Bouton-like structures exhibiting only GABA-like immunoreactivity or only glycine-like immunoreactivity were observed (Fig. 5A, double arrows and arrowheads, respectively), but many puncta also exhibited colabeling for both neurotransmitters (Fig. 5A, arrows). In NA, 36.4 ± 5.2% of the glycine-like signal also exhibited GABA-like immunoreactivity (%GABA/Gly), whereas 36.8 ± 10.1% of GABA-like labeling also had glycine-like immunoreactivity (%Gly/GABA). Interestingly, glycine labeling was also observed in NM and NL (Fig. 5B,C), despite the apparent lack of glycine receptor-mediated currents in these nuclei (Fig. 3C). Similar overlap between GABA and glycine signals was observed in NM (42.3 ± 16.6% %GABA/Gly; 47.5 ± 6.9% %Gly/GABA) and NL (36.6 ± 12.9% %GABA/Gly; 33.9 ± 4.1% %Gly/GABA) (p > 0.05 for all comparisons between nuclei; data from three different chicks, one image/brainstem region analyzed per animal). Together with our sIPSC recordings, these findings suggest that GABA and glycine can be coreleased from the same presynaptic axon in NA and possibly also in NM and NL.
Colocalization of GABA and glycine in embryonic chick auditory brainstem. Immunofluorescent labeling of GABA (green) and glycine (red) in NA (A), NM (B), and NL (C) of an E18 chick. A–C, Merged stacks of confocal images acquired in 0.5 μm intervals in the z-axis (A, 18 sections; B, 5 sections; C, 5 sections). Aa–Ac, Single confocal sections corresponding to dashed box in A showing GABA (Aa), glycine (Ab), and both fluorescent signals merged (Ac). Ba–Bc and Ca–Cc show same for NM and NL, respectively. Arrows indicate double-labeled puncta. Arrowheads indicate bouton-like structures showing only glycine labeling. Double arrows show puncta with GABA labeling only. Asterisk in A denotes a cell body exhibiting labeling for GABA and glycine. Cell bodies exhibiting GABA-like and/or glycine-like immunoreactivity were infrequently observed within NA (<3–4 cells when present, typically no cells observed). Scale bar in C applies to A–C. Scale bar in Cc applies to Aa–Cc.
Depolarizing Egly in NA
A unique feature of inhibition in NM is that GABAergic currents have a depolarizing effect on NM neurons throughout development (Lu and Trussell, 2001; Monsivais and Rubel, 2001; Howard et al., 2007). GABAergic transmission remains inhibitory under most circumstances in mature animals because of high expression levels of low-threshold activated K+ channels in NM neurons (Howard et al., 2007). Because the majority of NA neurons have fundamentally different intrinsic membrane properties compared with NM neurons (Soares et al., 2002; Fukui and Ohmori, 2003) and less robust low-threshold K+ conductances (Fukui and Ohmori, 2003), we examined the polarity of inhibition in NA (Fig. 6). To estimate the reversal potential for Cl−-mediated conductances, we measured currents in response to exogenously applied glycine using the gramicidin perforated-patch technique (Ebihara et al., 1995). In the majority of these recordings (9 of 12 cells), the reversal potential for glycine-induced currents (Egly) was depolarized compared with resting membrane potential (mean Egly= −41.5 ± 6.6 mV; mean resting membrane potential for all cells tested, −61.9 ± 3.5 mV). A small subset of cells (3 of 13) was also observed with hyperpolarized Egly values (−70.8 ± 3.5 mV). In the NA neurons we studied, Egly values did not seem to correlate with the age of tissue (E16–E20) or the action potential firing phenotype of the recorded cells. To confirm our results from gramicidin perforated-patch recordings, we also made extracellular recordings from several NA neurons in the cell-attached configuration. In three of four neurons, puff application of 1 mm glycine caused the cell to fire a single action potential (see example in Fig. 6C). We also performed cell-attached recordings from four cells in the presence of the K+-channel blocker 4-aminopyridine (4-AP) (30 μm), which was added to enhance excitability in our slices. Again, action potentials were recorded in response to glycine application in three of the four neurons examined. Two of these cells fired multiple action potentials in response to glycine (for example, see Fig. 6D), one cell fired a single action potential, and the last cell did not exhibit any spiking activity. Together, our results from gramicidin perforated-patch and cell-attached recordings indicate that most NA neurons in late embryonic chick tissue have depolarized Cl− reversal potentials. Cell-attached recordings were additionally used to examine responses to glycine application in NA neurons in tissue from hatchling chicks (P0). In hatchling tissue, four cells responded to glycine puffs with action potential firing (either from rest or increased spiking above spontaneous firing), whereas three cells with spontaneous spiking activity responded with a decrease in spike output. Ten additional cells tested did not exhibit changes in action potential firing with glycine application.
Depolarizing reversal potential for glycine-mediated currents in NA. A, Example responses recorded from an NA neuron using the gramicidin perforated-patch technique. Currents were recorded in response to a 100 ms puff of glycine (1 mm; inverted triangle indicates onset of application) at a series of holding potentials between −85 and −27 mV. B, Summary of reversal potentials for glycinergic currents (Egly) in NA neurons. Circles show Egly values for multiple spiking cells. Triangles show measurements for onset spiking cells. Squares show average values for depolarizing (open) and hyperpolarizing (filled) Egly values. C, Cell-attached recording from an NA neuron. Glycine application elicited an action potential (see inset). D, Cell-attached recording from a different NA neuron obtained in the presence of 30 μm 4-AP. Cell spiked multiple times in response to glycine exposure. In both C and D, 1 mm glycine was applied for 1 s starting at the time denoted by the inverted triangle.
Excitatory effects of GABA/glycine in NA
Given that the depolarizing Egly values measured for the majority of NA neurons studied was above action potential threshold (−47.2 ± 1.4 mV in multiple spiking neurons, −51.1 ± 2.0 mV onset spiking cells), we examined whether GABAergic/glycinergic inputs to NA neurons could exert an excitatory effect on these cells. To approximate the depolarizing Egly values we measured using gramicidin perforated-patch recordings, whole-cell recordings were acquired using a K+-based internal solution with a [Cl−] calculated to yield an ECl− of −35 mV ([Cl−] of 41 mm). Immediately after acquiring whole-cell recordings, cellular firing characteristics of NA neurons were assessed by current injection in current-clamp mode. After measuring IPSCs in response to repetitive stimulation in voltage clamp, recordings were switched back to current-clamp and responses with the same stimulus pattern was investigated. Figure 7 shows example responses from an onset spiking cell (Fig. 7A–C) and a multiple spiking neuron (Fig. 7D–F). In both cell types, postsynaptic potentials recorded in the presence of glutamate receptor blockers could elicit action potentials. Of four onset neurons tested, three fired at least one action potential over the course of 10 stimuli applied at 100 Hz. Similarly, four of five multiple spiking neurons fired action potentials in response to stimulation of inhibitory fibers.
Excitatory effects of GABA/glycinergic input to NA neurons. Whole-cell recordings were made from NA neurons using a pipette solution with a calculated ECl− of −35 mV. A, Response to current injection (−200 pA, +600 pA) in an onset spiking neuron. B, IPSCs evoked at 100 Hz in the same cell as in A. C, Current-clamp measurements from the same cell in response to the same stimulus used in B. D–F, Same as in A–C but for a multiple spiking neuron. Note that the cells presented in this figure are different from those shown in Figure 2.
Discussion
We found that the time course and neurotransmitter phenotype of inhibitory currents were distinct between NM, NL, and NA. These divergent properties may contribute to the unique computational tasks performed in the different brainstem regions. Additionally, we observed that glycinergic/GABAergic inputs could excite NA neurons, which could have important implications for the recruitment of SON activity.
Implications for ITD coding
The approximately threefold difference in IPSC decay kinetics we measured between NM and NL neurons indicates that inhibition may have a more complex role in ITD processing than appreciated previously. Most proposals for how inhibition contributes to ITD coding assume a slow time course for GABAergic currents (Funabiki et al., 1998; Monsivais et al., 2000; Grothe, 2003; Burger et al., 2005; Dasika et al., 2005). In NM, it is clear that inhibitory inputs do not preserve temporal information but instead exert a tonic influence (Lu and Trussell, 2000). Our results raise the possibility that inhibition may have more phasic effects in NL compared with NM. However, timing information is probably not retained in the activity of GABAergic inputs to NL. SON neurons do not phase lock as well as NM cells (Lachica et al., 1994) and do not appear to express intrinsic properties suited to preservation of timing information (Yang et al., 1999). However, heterogeneity in both tone-evoked response properties and anatomical features has been observed in SON neurons (Carr et al., 1989; Lachica et al., 1994). It is possible that a subpopulation of SON neurons may provide phase-locked inhibitory signals to NL, although no evidence exists to suggest that this may be the case. Further work will be needed to resolve whether precisely timed inhibition contributes to NL function.
Previous studies did not observe fast kinetics for inhibition in NL neurons from similarly aged chicks when recordings were performed at room temperature (Funabiki et al., 1998; Yang et al., 1999). Because our recordings were obtained closer to physiological temperature (41°C in chickens), the measurements presented here likely more closely resemble in vivo inhibitory currents than those reported previously.
Glycinergic transmission in the avian auditory brainstem
Perhaps the most surprising finding of this study was that glycinergic transmission contributes significantly to nonglutamatergic currents in all NA neurons examined. Glycinergic transmission had not been believed previously to play an important role in the bird auditory system because of a lack of immunohistochemical evidence for significant glycine expression in the chicken auditory nuclei (Code and Rubel, 1989) as well as observations that inhibition in NM and NL could be completely abolished by antagonists of GABAA receptors (Funabiki et al., 1998; Lu and Trussell, 2000). Our pharmacological and immunohistochemical evidence for glycinergic transmission raise the possibility that, at least within NA, inhibition in the avian and mammalian auditory systems is more similar than originally believed. Because our study focused on late-stage embryos, it is possible that the glycinergic currents we observed are not a permanent feature but instead reflect a transient stage of NA development. However, glycine-like immunoreactivity remains abundant in the NA of hatchling chicks (P0), and hatchling NA neurons still respond to glycine application (data not shown). Thus, glycinergic transmission likely still persists at an age at which NA neurons exhibit well developed intrinsic properties (Fukui and Ohmori, 2003) and hearing is intact.
Interestingly, immunofluorescent labeling also revealed coexpression of GABA and glycine in NM and NL, despite the lack of glycinergic synaptic currents in these regions. Whether glycine release within NM and NL has any function in these nuclei despite the apparent lack of postsynaptic glycine receptors remains to be determined. Glycine could potentially modulate excitatory and/or inhibitory transmission by acting on presynaptic glycine receptors (Turecek and Trussell, 2001) or extrasynaptic receptors on NM or NL neurons. Coreleased GABA was demonstrated recently to speed the decay of glycinergic currents by acting as a coagonist at glycine receptors (Lu et al., 2008). An intriguing possibility is that coreleased glycine could act to modulate the response of postsynaptic GABA receptors on NL and NM neurons.
Depolarizing ECl− in NA
An important caveat to our estimation of Cl− reversal potential is that our experiments were performed in late-stage chick embryos. Although we did not observe any obvious relationship between tissue age and ECl− in the cells we tested, we cannot rule out the possibility that our experiments were conducted before a developmental switch in the polarity of inhibition in NA. Using cell-attached recordings, we found that NA neurons in tissue from hatchling chicks could fire action potentials in response to glycine application, demonstrating that NA neurons in more mature tissue can also have a depolarized ECl− value. However, inhibitory responses were also observed in some cells, and most cells did not respond to glycine puffs with spiking activity, although the lack of responses in these latter cells could be attributable to reduced excitability, perhaps as a result of higher low-threshold K+ channel expression (Fukui and Ohmori, 2003). Importantly, in vivo recordings have demonstrated clear inhibitory influences in the sound-evoked response properties of mature NA units (Warchol and Dallos, 1990; Koppl and Carr, 2003). The obvious inhibition of spiking activity observed in some NA cells in response to presentation of tone or noise stimuli may indicate that GABAergic/glycinergic inputs are hyperpolarizing in at least some cell types in the mature animal.
Our current-clamp experiments indicate that GABAergic/glycinergic postsynaptic potentials may exert an excitatory influence on NA neurons under certain conditions. It should be noted that we observed a range of depolarizing Egly values, as well some instances in which Egly was hyperpolarizing. Differential expression or activity of Na+/K+/Cl− transporters and/or K+/Cl− transporters could underlie differences in ECl− (Payne et al., 2003). If SON neurons project to the same NA neurons from which they receive input, excitatory GABA/glycine influences in NA could establish a positive feedback loop that would increase activity in both nuclei. This could in turn alter the level of inhibition to NM and NL neurons and thereby enhance the functional range of inhibitory signaling in the brainstem. Positive feedback could also enhance the range of sound intensity coding by NA neurons by amplifying changes in the level of auditory nerve input to NA. In this scenario, additional mechanisms, such as synaptic depression, spike threshold accommodation, or metabotropic receptor activity (e.g., GABAB receptors), would be required to prevent overexcitability.
Sources of GABAergic/glycinergic input
Several anatomical studies have identified robust projections from ipsilateral SON to NM, NL, and NA (Lachica et al., 1994; Yang et al., 1999; Burger et al., 2005; Nishino et al., 2008), which has led to the prevailing view that the majority of GABAergic input to the timing and intensity pathways arises from SON. In partial support of this view, Nishino et al. (2008) recently demonstrated that lesioning SON altered ITD coding in the ipsilateral NL of chickens, presumably as a result of a loss of inhibitory input to NM and NL. Because our electrophysiological and immunohistochemical data indicate that a significant amount of GABAergic and glycinergic innervation arises from a common source, we consider it likely that SON can provide mixed GABAergic/glycinergic input to the different brainstem nuclei. We occasionally observed a few GABA and/or glycine-like immunoreactive cell bodies within or around NA (Fig. 5A) and in neurons between NM and NL (data not shown), consistent with previous reports of GABAergic neurons adjacent to or within these nuclei (Müller, 1987; Carr et al., 1989; von Bartheld et al., 1989). Thus, non-SON sources may also contribute some GABAergic and/or glycinergic input to NM, NL, and NA, although this may represent a small fraction of the input to these nuclei.
Although we did not investigate the specific mechanisms underlying the unique features of GABAergic and glycinergic currents in NM, NL, and NA, it is likely that postsynaptic factors had an important role. Differences in postsynaptic GABAA receptor subunit composition or receptor modulation probably accounts for the distinct decay kinetics between NM and NL. Differences in release time course are not likely to contribute to the observed differences in IPSC kinetics because, in NM, IPSCs evoked at the low stimulus frequencies used in this study have identical kinetics to miniature IPSCs, indicating that evoked release is highly synchronous in these cells at low stimulus frequency (Lu and Trussell, 2000). Because glycine appears to be expressed presynaptically not only in NA but also in NM and NL, the presence or absence of postsynaptic glycine receptors likely underlies the pharmacological differences between the sound intensity and timing pathways, similar to previous findings of postsynaptic selection of coreleased GABA and glycine in the mammalian cerebellum (Dugué et al., 2005). Considering that most of the inhibitory input to NM, NL, and NA likely arises from ipsilateral SON, the divergent properties of inhibitory transmission that we observed suggest that postsynaptic specializations permit segregation of inhibitory influences between different functional pathways despite a common source.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R01 DC004450 (L.O.T.) and a Cornelia H. Stevens Achievement Rewards for College Scientists scholarship (S.P.K.). We thank K. Bender, Y. Kim, C. Mello, K. Spinelli, T. Velho, and H. Zhao for advice and technical assistance, M. Roberts for providing Igor analysis procedures and advice, and P. Gillespie and Gillespie laboratory members for sharing equipment.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Sidney P. Kuo, Oregon Health and Science University, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Road, L335A, Portland, OR 97239. kuos{at}ohsu.edu