Abstract
The wake-promoting basal forebrain (BF) is critically involved in sustaining cortical arousal. In the present study, we investigated how aging affects the capacity of the BF to cope with continuous activation during prolonged waking. Increased neuronal activity induces lactate release in the activated brain area, and BF stimulation increases cortical arousal. We used in vivo microdialysis to measure lactate levels in the BF, and electroencephalography (EEG) to measure cortical arousal, during 3 h sleep deprivation (SD) in three age groups of rats. Lactate increased during SD in young but not in aged (middle-aged and old) rats. The increase in high-frequency (HF) EEG theta power (7–9 Hz), a marker of cortical arousal and active waking, was attenuated in the aged. Furthermore, a positive correlation between BF lactate release and HF EEG theta increase was found in young but not in aged rats. We hypothesized that these age-related attenuations result from reduced capacity of the BF to respond to increased neuronal activation. This was tested by stimulating the BF with glutamate receptor agonist NMDA. Whereas BF stimulation increased waking in young and old rats, lactate increase and the HF EEG theta increase were attenuated in the old. Also, the homeostatic increase in sleep intensity after SD was attenuated in aged rats. Our results suggest that an age-related attenuation in BF function reduces cortical arousal during prolonged waking. As the quality of waking is important in regulating the subsequent sleep, reduced cortical arousal during SD may contribute to the age-related reduction in sleep intensity.
Introduction
Wakefulness is a metabolic challenge for brain regions responsible for arousal (Porkka-Heiskanen et al., 2002; Cirelli, 2006; Vyazovskiy et al., 2008), and it has been suggested that during prolonged waking supplementary energy stores are used to sustain neuronal activity (Benington and Heller, 1995). Aging is associated with changes in sleep–wake architecture, including vigilance state fragmentation (i.e., the inability to maintain prolonged periods of wake and sleep), and attenuated homeostatic sleep response to sleep deprivation (Carskadon et al., 1982; Buysse et al., 1992; Ohayon et al., 2004; Cajochen et al., 2006). In addition, reduced capacity of the brain cells to cope with challenges, such as increased activity or energy demand, is reported during aging (Toescu and Xiong, 2004). Yet, it is not well known how these age-related cellular changes affect sleep–wake regulation.
The cholinergic basal forebrain (BF), an important area in regulation of wakefulness and cortical arousal (Jones, 2008; Parikh and Sarter, 2008), is selectively sensitive to both prolonged waking (Basheer et al., 2004) and to aging (Schliebs and Arendt, 2006). Healthy aging is associated with morphological and functional changes in the BF (Armstrong et al., 1993; Sarter and Bruno, 2004; Murchison and Griffith, 2007; Baskerville et al., 2008). During prolonged waking, the wake-promoting BF cells are continuously active, which increases energy demand (Wigren and Porkka-Heiskanen, 2009), extracellular levels of nitric oxide (Kalinchuk et al., 2006b), and energy metabolites adenosine and lactate (Porkka-Heiskanen et al., 1997; Kalinchuk et al., 2003).
Several lines of evidence now indicate that lactate, released from astrocytes in response to enhanced or sustained neuronal activity, is used as an energy fuel in active neurons (Pellerin et al., 2007). This activity-dependent lactate release has been detected in the activated brain areas during sensory and cognitive stimulations in humans (Prichard et al., 1991; Sappey-Marinier et al., 1992; Urrila et al., 2003) or during behavioral or experimental neuronal activation in animals (Kuhr and Korf, 1988; Hu and Wilson, 1997; Ros et al., 2006; Caesar et al., 2008). In the BF, extracellular lactate level increases during sleep deprivation (SD) when rats are kept awake by presentation of novel objects (Kalinchuk et al., 2003), which stimulates active waking (Huber et al., 2007) and electroencephalographic (EEG) theta (5–9 Hz) activity (Vyazovskiy and Tobler, 2005; Wigren et al., 2007; Kaur et al., 2008).
Theta activity in rodents is generally regarded as a marker of cortical arousal (Vinogradova, 1995; Kahana et al., 2001) and particularly the higher theta frequencies (∼7–9 Hz) correlate with active wake (Maloney et al., 1997; Young and McNaughton, 2008). Glutamatergic stimulation of the BF increases active waking, high-frequency (HF) EEG theta power and BF lactate level (Cape and Jones, 2000; Wigren et al., 2007), indicating that the lactate increase in the BF follows from increased neuronal activity occurring during EEG theta enriched active waking.
We tested the hypothesis that BF neuronal activity and the following lactate increase are important in sustaining waking and cortical arousal during SD, and that in aging this mechanism is attenuated. Using in vivo microdialysis, we measured BF lactate levels during 3 h SD and during sustained BF stimulation (NMDA infusion) in three age groups of rats (young, middle-aged, old). EEG was recorded to monitor vigilance states and cortical arousal during and after the experiments.
Materials and Methods
Animals and surgery
All animal procedures were approved by the University of Helsinki Ethical Committee for Animal Experiments and by the Regional Committee of the State Provincial Office and performed according to applicable national and European Union legislation. Three age groups of male Hannover Wistar rats: young (Y) (3–4 months; 362 ± 7 g; n = 17), middle-aged (M) (12–14 months; 561 ± 24 g; n = 11), and old (O) (23–25 months; 676 ± 35 g; n = 13) were housed on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 8:30 A.M.) at constant temperature (+22 ± 1°C). Food and water were provided ad libitum. Animals were gradually habituated to handling at least 1 week before surgery.
Under general anesthesia (2.5 mg/kg, i.p., diazepam; 0.4 mg/kg, i.p., medetomidine; 60 mg/kg, i.p., ketamine; dosages were adjusted according to age), rats were placed in a stereotaxic instrument (David Kopf Instruments) and implanted with the following: (1) chronic epidural EEG electrodes and dorsal nuchal electromyogram (EMG) electrodes for studying vigilance states and EEG activity, and (2) with microdialysis guide cannula (CMA 11 guide; CMA/Microdialysis) for subsequent insertion of microdialysis probe. The bipolar screw EEG electrodes were placed frontoparietally to the skull: 2 mm rostral, 2 mm lateral from bregma, and 4 mm rostral, 1 mm lateral from lambda. The microdialysis guide cannula was inserted unilaterally 2 mm above the basal forebrain area: 0.3 mm posterior, 2 mm lateral, and 5 mm ventral to bregma. The guide cannula and the electrode connector were fixed to the skull with acrylic dental cement.
After surgery, rats were single-housed in Plexiglas boxes and allowed to recover from 1 up to 2.5 weeks. They were then connected to flexible counterbalanced cables for EEG/EMG recording and habituated to recording cables for 2–3 d. The experiments were not started until their behavior and sleep–wake cycle had normalized.
In vivo microdialysis and EEG/EMG recording
We used in vivo microdialysis for collection of lactate samples and for glutamatergic stimulation of the BF [reverse microdialysis of 0.3 mm NMDA (Sigma-Aldrich) in perfusion fluid] as in the studies by Kalinchuk et al. (2003) and Wigren et al. (2007).
Twenty hours before the first experiment, microdialysis probes (CMA 11 probe; membrane length, 2 mm; diameter, 0.24 mm; mean recovery rate, 10%; CMA/Microdialysis) were lowered through the guide cannula into the BF. On the day of the experiments between 9:00 and 10:00 A.M., rats were connected to the EEG/EMG recording cables and to the in vivo microdialysis tubing. Tubing were connected to the microdialysis syringe pump (CMA/Microdialysis) and continuous perfusion (1 μl/min) of artificial CSF (aCSF) (147 mm NaCl, 3 mm KCl, 1.2 CaCl2, 1 mm MgCl) took place throughout the 6 h in vivo microdialysis period between 10:00 A.M. and 4:00 P.M.
The EEG/EMG signal was fed to an eight-channel digital amplifier (Cyber Amp 380; Molecular Devices), amplified (gain, 10,000), analogically filtered (high pass, 0.4 Hz; low pass, 40 Hz), digitized at 104 Hz with 1401 unit (CED), monitored on-line during the experiments, and stored on a computer using Spike2 software (CED) for additional off-line analysis.
Experiments
For each experiment, collection of 30 min (30 μl) samples was started at 10:00 A.M. and continued for 6 h until 4:00 P.M. (together, 12 samples were collected). Samples were stored at −80°C. At the end of the microdialysis period, animals were disconnected from the microdialysis tubing while the EEG/EMG recording was continued until the next morning (9:00 A.M.). During the first experimental day [baseline day (BL)], animals were left undisturbed (except for connecting/disconnecting of microdialysis tubing in the beginning and end of the microdialysis period) to allow spontaneous sleep–wake behavior to occur. On the following days, two types of experiments were performed: either sleep deprivation or glutamatergic stimulation of the BF. Before treatments, aCSF perfusion alone was performed for 2–3 h to collect samples for the determination of the pretreatment lactate level. Between 9:00 and 10:00 A.M. of the experimental days, animals were connected to microdialysis tubing and therefore EEG data from this time point were not recorded.
Prolonged waking/SD.
To keep animals awake and to evoke active waking and exploratory behavior; novel objects (including wooden blocks, new bedding material, and stretchable strings) were introduced to rat cages. To increase sleep pressure and to induce subsequent recovery sleep, SD was continued for 3 h. Whenever animals appeared sleepy or slow waves were visible in the EEG, new objects were introduced and/or the cage was gently tapped. This method is referred to as SD by gentle handling in the literature (Franken et al., 1991). After the end of the 3 h treatment period, animals were left undisturbed for recovery sleep recording until the next morning. If an animal was used for both experiments, SD was always performed first and minimum 48 h was allowed between experiments.
Glutamatergic stimulation (NMDA) of the BF.
To study the mechanisms of lactate release in the BF and to activate the animals without the presence of the experimenter, the BF was stimulated with NMDA [in vivo microdialysis fluid (aCSF) was switched to 0.3 mm NMDA in aCSF for 3 h]. At the end of NMDA treatment, microdialysis fluid was switched back to aCSF for 1 h to allow wash-off time before disconnection of the microdialysis tubing and start of undisturbed recovery sleep recording. Stimulation of the BF with this concentration has previously been shown to increase neuronal activation, to increase cortical arousal, to induce waking, to promote exploratory behavior, and to increase waking EEG theta power (Page et al., 1993; Manfridi et al., 1999; Cape and Jones, 2000; Wigren et al., 2007). NMDA-induced waking also increases homeostatic sleep pressure as it was previously shown to induce subsequent recovery sleep (Wigren et al., 2007).
HPLC
Microdialysis samples were assayed for lactate as in the study by Hallström et al. (1989) using HPLC system (Shimadzu) for separation, and UV spectrophotometer (Waters Corporation) for detection. Detection limit/signal-to-noise ratio was 10 μm/3:1, respectively (Grob, 1985). Lactate concentration was determined by comparing sample peak areas with those of standards using CLASS VP software (Shimadzu). To minimize day-to-day variation, baseline lactate concentrations for each animal were determined as a mean value of samples collected during the baseline day and during the pretreatment periods of the treatment days. To detect lactate responses to SD or to glutamatergic stimulation, samples collected during the treatments were normalized to the samples collected from the pretreatment period of the same day for each individual animal. To correlate lactate release with EEG theta activity during SD, individual 30 min samples (every other sample; together, six samples per rat) were normalized to the pretreatment level and matched with EEG data of the corresponding 30 min collection period. The dead volume of the microdialysis tubing was taken into consideration when matching samples with EEG data.
Histological verification of probe location
After the end of the experiments, animals received a lethal dose of pentobarbital (100 mg/kg). Color ink was injected through a modified microdialysis probe to mark the location of the probe tip. The brains were removed, frozen on dry ice, and stored at −80°C. Twenty micrometer coronal frozen sections were cut using a freezing microtome, stained with toluidine blue, coverslipped, and visually inspected under a light microscope to verify the location of the probe tip using rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 1998). Only those rats with probe locations in a close vicinity of the target area (BF) including horizontal diagonal band of Broca (HDB), substantia innominata (SI), magnocellular preoptic area (MCPO), lateral preoptic area (LPO) and the basal nucleus of Meynert (B) were included in the analysis. Probe tip locations are shown in Figure 1. Off-target experiments (probes outside the BF) were not included in the present study as the site-specific (outside BF compared with BF probes) extracellular lactate increase during 3 h SD in young rats has repeatedly been shown previously (Kalinchuk et al., 2003, 2006b).
Scoring and analysis of vigilance states
Before analysis, all EEG traces were digitally high-pass filtered (0.8 Hz cutoff). We used semiautomatic scoring of slow-wave sleep (SWS) in 30 s epochs as in the studies by Kalinchuk et al. (2003) and Stenberg et al. (2003) to measure the overall distribution and amount of SWS (the 23 h sleep pattern) during the BL and experimental days. Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep was scored manually. Wake epochs and the number of state transitions (number of shifts among behavioral states: wake⇄SWS⇄REM→wake) were calculated from these two scores.
All the recordings that were used for power-spectral analysis (the microdialysis period between 10:00 A.M. and 4:00 P.M. and the subsequent 3 h postmicrodialysis recovery period) were scored manually in 5 s epochs with Spike2 sleep scoring script Sleepscore, version 1.01 (CED), according to standard criteria: SWS was recognized as high-amplitude EEG associated with low-voltage EMG and presence of slow delta (0.8–4 Hz) oscillations in the EEG, REM sleep as low-amplitude, high-frequency EEG with absence of EMG and presence of prominent EEG theta (5–9 Hz), and waking as low-amplitude, high-frequency EEG with high-voltage EMG. Artifacts or epochs with mixed states were marked and excluded from power spectral analysis.
Two additional measures were applied to investigate the behavior of the rats during SD as follows.
Number of sleep bouts.
Sleep bouts were defined as more than one 5 s epoch scored as SWS. As rats were always activated when they appeared sleepy or slow waves occurred in the EEG, this measure also reflects the number of interventions needed to keep the rats awake during SD.
Number of high EMG activity bouts.
SD increases exploratory behavior and it is associated with increased movement (i.e., increased muscle activity). To control for the possible age differences in moving activity, we counted the number of high EMG activity bouts with a custom-made Spike2 script. The total EMG power (fast Fourier transformation routine in Spike2) of the 23 h EMG recording was calculated first and based on this mean value of the total EMG power, the script automatically scored the EMG data either as high activity or as low activity in 5 s epochs. Because a mean value of the total EMG power over the whole recording was used to define the threshold, only the high EMG activity bouts (e.g., when the rat was actively moving around) were determined as high activity. From most of the 23 h EMG recordings of the BL and SD, the script accurately recognized the activity period of the rats during the dark period and the SD period. Rats, in which these periods were not recognized by the script, were discarded from the analysis of activity bouts during SD.
Power spectral analysis of the EEG
EEG power (in square microvolts) spectra of the manually scored EEG data were generated in Spike2 (CED) separately for consecutive 5 s wake and SWS epochs [fast Fourier transform routine (FFT), 256; Hanning window, 0.4 Hz resolution] within a frequency range of 0.8–35 Hz and averaged over the time period that was investigated (6 h between 10:00 A.M. to 4:00 P.M. for the baseline, 3 h for the treatment periods, and 3 h for the posttreatment recovery periods of SD and NMDA). These spectra were always normalized to total power of the same recording period before additional analysis. To study the effects of treatments (SD, NMDA) compared with BL, the spectra of the 3 h treatment periods and the 3 h posttreatment periods were always compared and/or normalized to the corresponding spectra of the BL day. Waking theta power was defined as 5–9 Hz [low frequency (LF) theta as 5–7 Hz; HF theta as 7–9 Hz] and gamma power as frequencies >30 Hz. We used waking EEG theta as a measure of cortical arousal and wake quality and calculated the ratio of HF theta versus LF theta because LF theta correlates with quiet waking and HF theta with active waking (Maloney et al., 1997; Young and McNaughton, 2008).
SWS delta and recovery sleep
Prolonged waking invariably leads to homeostatic recovery sleep response: increases in SWS duration and SWS delta power (Tobler and Borbély, 1990). To test whether treatments (SD, NMDA) induced recovery sleep, we calculated SWS delta power (defined as 0.8–4 Hz) and SWS amount from the posttreatment EEG recordings and compared them with the corresponding time points of the BL day.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using SigmaStat 3.1 (SPSS Science Software). Statistics were applied (1) to study the effect of treatments (SD, NMDA) within an age group (“within an age group comparisons”) and (2) to compare age groups (“between age groups comparisons”). Before comparing the effect of treatments between age groups, the data were normalized: the power spectra and the vigilance state counts were normalized to the corresponding time points of the BL day within an individual animal, whereas lactate levels of the treatment period were normalized to the pretreatment level within an individual animal.
For simple comparisons of independent variables, we used two-tailed Student's t tests to compare two variables, or one-way ANOVA if more than two variables were compared. To isolate which variable(s) differed from the others, one-way ANOVA was followed by Holm–Sidak multiple-comparison procedure as a post hoc test. For simple comparisons of dependent variables, we used paired t tests (two variables such as pretreatment vs treatment) or one-way repeated measures of ANOVA (more than two variables such as vigilance states in time series or EEG power in frequency bins). For combined analysis of independent and dependent variables, we used two-way repeated measures of ANOVA. In these analyses, the treatment or age was set as an independent variable and EEG measures (such as vigilance states in time series or power in frequency bins) were set as dependent variables. Holm–Sidak was used as a post hoc test. In case the data were not normally distributed, equivalent nonparametric tests were applied: one-way ANOVA was replaced with Kruskal–Wallis one-way ANOVA on ranks followed by Dunn's comparison. For correlation analysis, Pearson's product moment correlation was used. A value of p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Data are shown as mean ± SEM; statistical significances are denoted with hash symbols (#) for the within an age group comparisons and with asterisks (*) for the between age group comparisons. Age groups are indicated as follows: young rats with Y (black circles; •); middle-aged rats with M (crosses; ×), and old with O (open circles; ○).
Results
Baseline
The 23 h sleep–wake pattern
The 23 h (from 10:00 A.M. to 9:00 A.M.) baseline sleep–wake profile (percentage of time spent in SWS, in REM sleep, and the mean number of state transitions) in 1 h time bins can be found in supplemental Table 1 (available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). A statistically significant reduction of SWS amount in the old (n = 13) compared with young (n = 11) rats was found during the second, third, and fourth hour of the BL recording (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Holm–Sidak; F(44) = 1.450; p = 0.032). However, most differences between age groups were found in the number of state transitions: the middle-aged and the old had more state transitions than the young (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Holm–Sidak; F(44) = 1.421; p = 0.041).
Vigilance states during the 6 h in vivo microdialysis period
All rats slept on average 60% of the time (SWS, 50 ± 1%; REM, 10 ± 1; n = 35). Statistically significant differences between the age groups in the time spent in SWS, REM sleep, or wake (calculated as mean 6 h values) were not found (data not shown).
BF lactate
There was an age-dependent reduction in baseline lactate level (one-way ANOVA; F(2) = 10.118; p < 0.001): old rats (n = 13) had lower lactate concentration than the middle-aged (n = 9) or young (n = 17) (Holm–Sidak; p = 0.004 and p < 0.001, respectively) (Fig. 2A). To control for day-to-day variation in lactate levels, mean BL lactate concentrations were calculated from all samples collected during the BL microdialysis period as well as from samples collected during the pretreatment periods of the treatment days.
EEG power spectra of the in vivo microdialysis period
To assess cortical arousal during waking and sleep intensity during SWS, vigilance state-specific mean EEG power spectra (frequencies between 0.8 and 35 Hz, in 0.4 Hz bins) were calculated by averaging over all the 5 s wake or SWS epochs within the 6 h in vivo microdialysis period, and the age groups were compared (nY = 17; nM = 5; nO = 13). Statistically significant interactions between age and frequency in the wake EEG power (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Holm–Sidak; F(168) = 8.242; p < 0.001) were found in the delta (0.8–4.0 Hz) and in theta (5–9 Hz) frequency ranges (Fig. 2B). The peak frequency of waking theta was 6.5 Hz in the young but had shifted to 6.1 Hz in the middle-aged and the old (Fig. 2B, right panel). Old rats also had more power in the LF theta range.
In the SWS EEG power, statistically significant interaction between age and frequency (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Holm–Sidak, F(168) = 5.133; p < 0.001) was found in the delta range between the old and the young, and between the middle-aged and the young (Fig. 2C, top right panel). In the theta range, differences were found between the young and the old (Fig. 2C, left panel).
Sleep deprivation
Before sleep deprivation, in vivo microdialysis samples were collected for the determination of the pretreatment lactate level. During this period, statistically significant differences between the age groups in the time spent in sleep or wake were not found (data not shown). During SD, rats were awake 95.7 ± 1% of the time (Y, 98.6 ± 0.6%, n = 10; M, 97.9 ± 1.2, n = 5; O, 92.7 ± 1.1, n = 13).
BF lactate
In young rats, lactate concentration increased significantly during SD compared with the pretreatment level (paired t test; n = 10; t(9) = 2.922; p = 0.017). This is in agreement with previous studies from our laboratory (Kalinchuk et al., 2003, 2006b; Wigren et al., 2007). In contrast, significant lactate responses were not detected in middle-aged or old rats, and there was a statistically significant difference in the lactate response between the young and the old (Kruskal–Wallis one-way ANOVA on ranks with Dunn's; H(2) = 9.662; p = 0.008) (Fig. 3A).
Waking EEG power spectra
Mean EEG power spectra of the 3 h SD period compared with BL spectra of the corresponding time period is shown in figure (Fig. 3B). During SD, EEG power decreased (compared with BL) in all age groups in the delta (0.8–4 Hz) and in the LF theta (5–7 Hz) range, whereas a prominent increase was found in the HF theta (7–9 Hz) range (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Holm–Sidak; treatment by frequency; Y, F(84) = 12.396, p < 0.001; M, F(84) = 2.781, p < 0.001; O, F(84) = 17.929, p < 0.001, respectively). Between age group analysis demonstrated statistically significant attenuations in the middle-aged and old compared with the young in the HF theta and in the gamma (>30 Hz) range (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Holm–Sidak; age by frequency; F(168) = 2.431; p < 0.001).
Waking theta and the “quality” of waking during SD
To characterize the behavior (i.e., the quality of waking) of the rats during SD the high-to low-frequency theta ratio (HF theta/LF theta) (Fig. 4A,B, left panels) was calculated together with the number of sleep bouts (Fig. 4A, right panel) and with the number of high EMG activity bouts (Fig. 4B, right panel).
The mean high- to low-frequency theta ratio (Fig. 4A, left panel) increased in all age groups (paired t tests; Y, t(9) = 4.567, p = 0.001; M, t(4) = 3.309, p = 0.030; O, t(12) = 4.414, p = 0.001), but this increase was significantly attenuated in old rats compared with the young (one-way ANOVA with Holm–Sidak; F(2) = 3.519; p = 0.045). Old rats had more sleep bouts during SD than the young (Kruskal–Wallis one-way ANOVA on ranks with Dunn's; H(2) = 8.668; p = 0.013) (Fig. 4A, right panel). As rats were always activated when sleep occurred, the maximum sleep bout lengths were only 0.6 ± 0.2 min for the young (n = 10), 0.5 ± 0.2 min for the middle-aged (n = 5), and 1.3 ± 0.2 min for the old (n = 13).
When the theta ratio (Fig. 4B, left panel) and the high EMG activity bouts (Fig. 4B, right panel) were calculated in 30 min time bins over the SD period, statistically significant (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA; F(2) = 7.941; p = 0.002; followed by Holm–Sidak) age differences were found in all time points in the theta ratio, whereas no differences between age groups were found in the number of high EMG activity bouts.
Correlation of BF lactate release with waking theta
To test the hypothesis that lactate release in the BF is linked to cortical arousal/active waking (measured with the HF theta increase), we investigated whether there is a statistically significant correlation between lactate increase (percentage of pretreatment level) and the waking theta response (high-to-low waking theta ratio, percentage difference). For this purpose individual lactate samples (collected during the microdialysis period of the SD day and normalized to the pretreatment level; on average, six samples per animal) were matched with EEG data (high-to-low waking EEG theta ratio) of the corresponding 30 min collection period. Statistically significant positive correlation was found in the young group (n = 10) between lactate release and waking theta response [Pearson's product moment correlation; nsamples = 42; correlation coefficient (r), 0.779; p < 0.01] but not in the middle-aged (n = 5) (nsamples = 29; r = 0.295; p = 0.121) or the old (n = 13) (nsamples = 68; r = 0.037; p = 0.765) (Fig. 5).
Glutamatergic stimulation of the BF with NMDA
As the greatest (and statistically significant) differences in both BF lactate release and waking theta responses during SD were detected between the young and old, NMDA activation was performed to these age groups only. In line with previous reports (Cape and Jones, 2000; Wigren et al., 2007), waking compared with BL was increased in the young group (n = 5) (68 ± 4% increase; paired t test; t(4) = 16.653; p < 0.001). In the old group (n = 4), waking was increased by 31 ± 10%, but this increase did not reach statistical significance. There was a statistically significant difference in the amount of waking induced by NMDA between the Y and the O (t test; t(7) = 3.669; p = 0.008).
BF lactate
In young rats (n = 5), glutamatergic stimulation significantly increased BF lactate concentration when compared with the pretreatment level (paired t test; t(4) = 2.975; p = 0.041), a finding that has been described previously (Wigren et al., 2007). In contrast, glutamatergic stimulation did not evoke significant lactate responses in old rats (n = 4).
Waking EEG power spectra
When the mean EEG power spectra of the 3 h NMDA infusion period were compared with the corresponding BL power spectra (Fig. 6), we found that in both age groups BF activation significantly attenuated waking EEG power in the delta frequency range and in the LF theta range, whereas an increase was found it in the HF theta range (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, treatment by frequency, with Holm–Sidak; Y, F(84) = 5.499, p < 0.001; O, F(84) = 1.845, p < 0.001). Between age group analysis demonstrated a statistically significant attenuation in the HF theta increase in the old group compared with the young (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA; age by hertz; Holm–Sidak; F(84) = 1.779; p < 0.001).
Recovery sleep
To test whether treatments (NMDA, SD) induced recovery sleep, SWS amount and SWS EEG power spectra calculated from the posttreatment EEG recordings were compared with the corresponding time points of the BL day.
SWS amount
Within an age group statistical analysis (paired t tests) demonstrated significant increases in the time spent in SWS during the first manually scored 3 h post-SD time period compared with BL in all age groups (Y, t(9) = 5.289, p < 0.01; M, t(4) = 11.433, p < 0.01; O, t(12) = 6.381, p < 0.001). Statistically significant differences in the SWS response between the age groups were not found. [The amount of SWS compared with BL during the recovery night after SD calculated in 1 h bins can be found in supplemental Table 2 (available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).] After NMDA treatment, a significant increase in SWS compared with BL was found only in the young group (t(4) = 4.355; p = 0.012).
SWS EEG power spectra
To assess sleep intensity during recovery sleep, EEG power spectra of the first (manually scored) 3 h post-SD period were compared with the corresponding BL spectra (Fig. 7). In all age groups, SWS EEG power compared with BL was significantly (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Holm–Sidak; treatment by frequency; Y, F(84) = 21.977; M, F(84) = 18.880; O, F(84) = 32.539; p < 0.001) increased in the delta frequency range, whereas a significant reduction was found in the frequency ranges >6.1 Hz. Between age group comparisons demonstrated a statistical significant attenuation in the delta power in the middle-aged and old compared with the young, whereas the SWS EEG of the old and middle-aged rats contained more HF EEG (>7.3 Hz) power than that of the young (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Holm–Sidak; age by frequency; F(168) = 2.662; p < 0.001).
Discussion
We found that the activity-dependent increases in BF lactate and waking EEG theta power were attenuated in aged animals (middle-aged and old) during prolonged waking/sleep deprivation and that neither of these responses could be experimentally induced by glutamatergic stimulation of the BF.
Reduced BF lactate release in aging
In contrast to young, lactate release did not increase in the BF during SD in the aged animals (middle-aged and old). Our results are in line with previous human studies showing that cognitive stimulation, which increases lactate in the prefrontal cortex of young subjects, failed to do so in the aged (Urrila et al., 2003, 2004). It would be interesting to know whether the attenuation of activity-dependent lactate release is a general phenomenon in the aging brain or whether it is specific for some brain regions only (such as the BF and the prefrontal cortex). The attenuated BF lactate response in our study could have resulted either from reduced sensitivity of the aged BF to excitatory input, reduced release of excitatory transmitters, or from age-related impairments in lactate production or release.
The assumption that age-related attenuation of lactate response is attributable to reduced sensitivity of BF to respond to excitatory input, is supported by previous studies showing age-related reductions in BF stimulation-induced cortical acetylcholine release (Mitsushima et al., 1996; Giovannini et al., 1998; Fadel et al., 1999; Herzog et al., 2003; Sarter and Bruno, 2004). In line with these studies, we found that experimental stimulation of the BF with NMDA did not increase local lactate release or induce enough neuronal activation to significantly increase wakefulness in old rats. Also, several studies have demonstrated age-related decreases in NMDA receptor density and alternations in its function (Gonzales et al., 1991; Segovia et al., 2001).
Another explanation for the lack of activity-dependent lactate release could be the reduced capacity of the aged brain cells to cope with increased energy demand (Toescu and Xiong, 2004). Expression of genes related to energy metabolism are chronically altered in the aged brain, including increased expression of lactate dehydrogenase, an enzyme involved in lactate metabolism (Poon et al., 2006; Pawlyk et al., 2007). Compared with other brain areas, cells in the BF, particularly the cholinergic cells, appear more sensitive to age-related impairments (Sarter and Bruno, 2004; McKinney and Jacksonville, 2005). A recent study found that the age-related increase in expression of genes related to energy production was more pronounced in the cholinergic cells of the BF than in the cholinergic cells of the brainstem (Baskerville et al., 2008). Thus, if energy production in the BF is challenged already at baseline, it is reasonable to assume that, on activation, the capacity of the BF to further increase energy production and lactate release is reduced.
Waking EEG theta and BF activity
In rodents, EEG theta activity can be divided into two components: the LF theta (5–7 Hz) associated with quiet wake and sleepiness and the HF theta (7–9 Hz) associated with active wake (Maloney et al., 1997; Vyazovskiy and Tobler, 2005; Young and McNaughton, 2008). We regarded HF EEG theta as a measure of both active waking/cortical arousal and BF activity because in vivo recordings have shown that the cholinergic cells of the BF are more active during active wake than quiet wake and that they burst in association with EEG theta (Lee et al., 2004, 2005). Furthermore, HF EEG theta power can be increased by experimental activation of the BF (Cape and Jones, 2000; Wigren et al., 2007).
With stimulation of the BF by NMDA and during 3 h SD, particularly the HF theta is enhanced, whereas application of NMDA antagonist during SD selectively attenuates HF theta (Wigren et al., 2007). In the present study, the increase in HF theta during SD was reduced in old and middle-aged rats compared with the young. This could be attributable to reduced sensitivity of aged BF to excitatory stimulus, as evidenced by reduced wake promotion and HF theta power increase to NMDA stimulation in the aged. During baseline waking, our old animals had significantly more power in the LF theta range than the young. This finding is consistent with other studies in both humans (van der Hiele et al., 2008) and animals (Colas et al., 2005). Increased waking LF EEG theta is suggested to implicate a reduced arousal level (Schacter, 1977; Cajochen et al., 1995, 2001, 2002; Lorenzo et al., 1995; Aeschbach et al., 1997, 1999; Finelli et al., 2000; Lafrance and Dumont, 2000; Strijkstra et al., 2003). Reduced baseline arousal could reduce the capacity of the old to increase active wake (a shift from low- to high-frequency theta enriched waking), particularly during SD.
Correlation between BF lactate release and HF EEG theta increase
To further substantiate the assumption that lactate release in the BF is linked to active waking and HF theta, we found a strong positive correlation between BF lactate release and HF EEG theta increase in young animals, which was lost in the aged. Together, our results show that both measures of BF activation, local lactate release and HF EEG theta, are reduced in aging.
Implications for sleep homeostasis
Sleep is governed by a homeostatic process in which duration and quality of waking regulate subsequent sleep (Borbély, 1982; Huber et al., 2007). In our aged animals, SWS delta (0.5–4 Hz) power, a marker of sleep intensity and homeostatic sleep drive (Achermann and Borbély, 2003), was reduced in baseline and its increase in response to SD was attenuated. This finding is in line with other studies with aged rats (Mendelson and Bergmann, 2000; Shiromani et al., 2000). In addition, we found age differences also in the higher frequency ranges: in all age groups, frequencies >7.3 Hz were attenuated in the recovery SWS EEG compared with BL, but this attenuation was reduced in the aged. In other words, during recovery sleep, aged animals had less delta activity and more high frequency activity than the young. As delta activity is an indication of sleep intensity and the higher frequencies are an indication of cortical arousal, these results further emphasize the fact that, in aging, recovery sleep intensity is attenuated.
The mechanisms contributing to the age-related attenuation in sleep homeostasis are not well understood. There are two probable causes for reduced recovery sleep in aging: (1) the capacity to produce sleep is reduced (Klerman and Dijk, 2008) and/or (2) accumulation of homeostatic sleep pressure is reduced.
In young animals, increases in the extracellular levels of the endogenous sleep factors adenosine and nitric oxide (NO) in the BF during SD are repeatedly reported to induce sleep (Porkka-Heiskanen et al., 1997, 2000; Basheer et al., 2004; Murillo-Rodriguez et al., 2004; Kalinchuk et al., 2006a,b). In our previous study, we found aging related attenuations in the levels of adenosine and NO in the BF during SD, suggesting a reduction in the buildup of homeostatic sleep pressure (Rytkönen et al., 2008). Furthermore, local infusion of NO donor into the BF failed to increase sleep intensity in old rats, as it did in the young, suggesting that also the sensitivity of the aged BF to sleep-inducing factors is reduced.
The present study demonstrated that cortical arousal (as measured by EEG theta and gamma power) was reduced in aged rats during SD. Previous studies have shown that wakefulness enriched with high levels of active wake and HF EEG theta is more potent in increasing sleep pressure and recovery sleep delta power than the same amount of quiet waking (Huber et al., 2007; Wigren et al., 2007). Thus, the reduced adenosine and NO responses during SD and the attenuation of homeostatic sleep response in recovery sleep could be attributable to the fact that aged animals spent less time in active wake.
Sleep problems are becoming increasingly prevalent in the modern society, particularly when the population ages. Understanding of the cellular mechanisms contributing to these problems is crucial for appropriate treatment. Our results suggest that an age-related attenuation in BF function leads to reduced cortical arousal, particularly during prolonged waking. As quality of waking is important in regulating subsequent sleep, reduction in cortical arousal during waking may contribute to the age-related reduction in sleep intensity.
Footnotes
-
This study was supported by the Academy of Finland, the Finnish Graduate School of Neuroscience, and European Union Grants LSHM-CT-2005-518189 and MCRT-CT-2004-512362. We thank Dr. Ernst Mecke, Pirjo Saarelainen, Sari Levo-Siitari, and Natalia Gass for their excellent assistance and technical help.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Henna-Kaisa Wigren, Institute of Biomedicine/Physiology, Biomedicum Helsinki, University of Helsinki, P.O. Box 63, FIN-00014 Helsinki, Finland. henna-kaisa.wigren{at}helsinki.fi