Abstract
Previous research suggests that visually responsive neurons in the frontal eye field (FEF) respond to visual targets even when they are not the goal of a saccadic eye movement. These results raise the possibility that these neurons respond to visual targets independent of the effector that is to be used to acquire the target locations. In the present study, we examined whether a plan to execute a saccade or a reach to a visual target influenced the response to and the representation of targets in the FEF. We recorded single unit responses to the onset of the target, during the delay period, and around the time of the movement, on interleaved saccade and reach trials of a delayed-response task. We found that the responses of approximately equal percentages of visual, visuomovement, and movement neurons (50%, 58%, and 58%, respectively) were greater on saccade trials than on reach trials in at least one interval of the delayed-response task. Converse biases, in favor of reaches, were much less frequent (13%, 10%, and 19%, in visual, visuomovement, and movement neurons respectively). Thus, although visual neurons may not be directly involved in triggering saccadic eye movements, they are nonetheless highly saccade-biased, with percentages comparable to neurons that are directly involved in triggering saccadic eye movements.
Introduction
The frontal eye field (FEF), located on the anterior bank of the arcuate sulcus in monkey, is involved in transforming sensory signals into motor commands for saccadic eye movements (for review, see Tehovnik et al., 2000). Within FEF there is a continuum of responses, from visual neurons that respond to the onset of a visual target in the receptive field, to movement neurons that respond around the time of a saccade (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985). Visual neurons remain tonically active when a delay is introduced between target presentation and saccade onset, suggesting that they are involved in the maintenance of spatial information (Funahashi et al., 1989; Lawrence et al., 2005). It is unclear, however, whether this maintenance occurs primarily or exclusively for saccades.
It is possible that FEF visual neurons are saccade biased, preferentially responding to and maintaining visual information for saccades relative to other purposes, such as reaches. Consistent with this possibility, Goldberg and Bushnell (1981) found enhanced responses from visual neurons when a target was the goal of a saccade compared to the object of a discrimination, and in at least one neuron, they found enhanced responses for a saccade compared to a reach. Because they did not use a delayed-response task, however, the extent to which this enhancement characterizes the maintenance of visual information is unknown. Mushiake et al. (1996) examined modulation in movement-related neurons and found that the presaccadic responses were uneffected by a simultaneous reach. While suggestive, these results do not rule out the possibility that reaches in the absence of a saccade drive FEF movement neurons. Moreover, because they examined only movement-related neurons, the extent to which these findings characterize visual neurons in FEF is not known.
It is also possible that visual neurons are not saccade biased. Consistent with this possibility, recent research confirms that visually responsive neurons in FEF respond to salient targets even when they are not the goal of a saccade (Thompson et al., 1997; Murthy et al., 2001; Sato et al., 2003). In a visual search task in which saccades are executed to an “oddball” target, visually responsive neurons signal the target before the saccade (Schall et al., 1995). However, this signal is not temporally correlated with the saccade onset (Thompson et al., 1996), occurs even when the saccade is mistakenly executed to a target other than the oddball stimulus (Murthy et al., 2001), and persists when the behavioral response is a lever press rather than a saccade (Thompson et al., 2005). These results suggest that visual neurons in FEF respond to and maintain visual targets in an unbiased manner.
We revisited this issue using a delayed-response paradigm in which the animal was required to look at or reach toward the remembered location only after a short memory period. We found biases for saccades that were comparable across cell types, including visual neurons. Thus, our data support a saccade-specific model of FEF.
Materials and Methods
Recording procedure.
Animals were seated in a custom-designed monkey chair (Crist Instruments) with a fully open front that allowed for unconstrained arm movements to visual stimuli. Stimuli were back-projected by a CRT projector (Electrohome) onto a modified touch panel (Keytec) located 25 cm in front of the animal. Arm position was monitored using the touch panel, and eye position was monitored using a scleral search coil (CNC Engineering). The experiment took place in complete darkness in a sound-attenuated room.
FEF recordings were made from two adult male Rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) monkeys. Recording chambers were placed flush with the skull (25 mm anterior and 20 mm lateral, Horsley–Clarke coordinates), contralateral to the preferred hand. The determination of handedness, and as a result, chamber location, was based upon which hand the animal used more frequently early in training. Animals were viewed remotely to ensure the use of the dominant hand throughout a session. If, on the very rare occasion, the animal used the nondominant hand, lights were flashed to warn the animal, and the experiment resumed only when the animal reached for the fixation point with the dominant hand. Structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was used to confirm the placement of each chamber with respect to the arcuate sulcus and also to localize recording sites [see the study by Lawrence et al. (2005) for recording site reconstruction].
Stimulation procedure.
Neurons recorded within 200 μm of sites at which electrical microstimulation of <50 μA evoked a consistent saccadic eye movement were defined as FEF neurons (Bruce et al., 1985). To make this determination, the animal began by fixating a blue target for 400 ms. The fixation target was extinguished, and 100 ms later there occurred, with equal probability, a 70 ms interval of either stimulation or no stimulation. The fixation point reappeared 230 ms later on stimulation (biphasic, 250 microseconds/phase, 350 Hz, 70 ms duration) and on no-stimulation trials. The animal was rewarded on every stimulation trial, and also on no-stimulation trials in which the eyes remained within 4.5° of the extinguished fixation point. Because it is possible to elicit small eye movements at low thresholds in regions outside of FEF, only neurons recorded near stimulation sites resulting in perturbations greater than 2° were used in the analyses. Significant perturbations (t test, p < 0.05) ranged from 2.2° to 28°, with a mean perturbation of 8.2 ± 0.3°.
Receptive field mapping procedure.
Spatially selective FEF neurons were then identified, and their receptive fields mapped, using a non-delayed center-out task. This task consisted of interleaved saccade trials, and combined saccade and reach trials. Movements were executed to eccentric stimuli presented in one of 8 possible directions, spaced 45° apart, at a range of 3 eccentricities, for a total of 24 possible targets. This mapping determined the provisional “preferred” direction (the direction with the largest response) and the “null” direction (the direction with the smallest response, with the constraint that the null direction was 180° from the preferred direction) based on the response of the neuron in the 100–200 ms interval following the onset of the target on the combined saccade and reach trials. From the responses obtained 100–200 ms after target appearance in the center-out movement task, and from the size of the stimulation-evoked eye movement, two target locations were chosen for further testing in the target-delay-cue paradigm (see below): one target in the preferred direction and one in the opposite (null) direction.
The preferred and null directions of spatially selective neurons were then confirmed using a target-delay-cue task. In this task, a target is presented, and following a delay period, a cue is presented signaling the effector (saccade or reach) in which the animal is to move to the target location (for additional details, see Lawrence and Snyder, 2006). The preferred direction of a neuron was defined as the direction associated with the largest response in any of the following three intervals of the target-delay-cue task: the early visual response (50–150 ms), the late visual response (150–250 ms) (both averaging across saccade and reach trials), and the movement-related response on saccade trials (−100 to 0 ms before the onset of the saccade). Data from 87 spatially selective FEF cells (62 from the left hemisphere of M1, 25 from the right hemisphere of M2) were recorded. Animals then completed interleaved saccade and reach trials of the delayed-response task. Results of delayed-saccade trials have been previously reported (Lawrence et al., 2005).
Delayed-response task.
A trial of the delayed-response task began when the animal fixated and touched a central blue fixation point for 500 ms (see Fig. 1). A red or green target, signaling a saccade or reach trial, was then presented for 300 ms in one of two symmetric locations, chosen to fall either inside the receptive field (RF), or outside of the RF (the color mapping was reversed in the two animals). Eccentricity was adjusted based on the size of the stimulation-evoked a saccadic eye movement and the responses during the saccade-plus-reach and mapping tasks. The animal was required to maintain central fixation throughout the delay period (800 ms). The end of the delay period was signaled by the offset of the fixation point. The animal then had up to 700 ms to move the appropriate effector (the median reaction time was 142 ms on saccade trials and 187 ms on reach trials) to within 7° (saccade trials) or 10° (reach trials) of the location at which the target had previously appeared. Plots of both saccade and reach endpoints can be found in the supplemental material (supplemental Fig. S1, available at www.jneurosci.org).
Correct trials were rewarded and incorrect trials were aborted, incurring a ∼1 s time out. Animals failed to acquire the initial fixation or touch targets, or broke fixation during the delay period, on ∼10% of trials. On saccade trials, animals moved too late, made a reach instead of or in addition to a saccade, or failed to move into the window on another 10.2% of trials. The equivalent error rate on reach trials was higher (23.7%). After the reward was given on correct reach trials, on only a small percentage of the trials (2.0%) did the eyes move 4° or more toward the arm when it was positioned at the remembered target location. Animals performed 10 or more interleaved repetitions of each of the four types (reaches or saccades into or out of the RF of the neuron).
Data analyses.
To examine whether the tuning on trials which required reaches was different from the tuning on trials which only required saccades, we compared the tuning of both visual and movement responses on the saccade trials and combined saccade and reach trials of the center-out task (see Materials and Methods). We used the center out task not only because it provided the basis for the selection of RFs, but also because it contained a higher resolution mapping of preferred direction than the main task: in the center out task we collected data from 8 different target directions and 3 different target eccentricities for a total of 24 target locations. For each neuron, we calculated visual responses (interval 50 to 250 ms following the onset of the target) and movement responses (the 200 ms interval surrounding the onset of the movement) on saccade trials and on combined saccade and reach trials. Separate cosine tuning curves were then fit to the visual responses and the movement responses for each neuron for both saccade and combined saccade and reach trials (see supplemental Material, Fig. S2, available at www.jneurosci.org). We found that there was no significant difference in the tuning of the visual or movement responses (Watson's Two-Sample Test of Homogeneity p > 0.05; median difference = 10.5° and 19.6°, for visual and movement responses, respectively). Thus, the tuning on combined saccade and reach trials was not significantly different from the tuning on saccade trials.
Delayed-saccade and delayed-reach trials were divided into three intervals: target (50 to 350 ms interval following the onset of the target), delay (450 to 950 ms following the onset of the target), and movement (100 ms interval preceding the onset of the movement), unless otherwise noted. Paired t tests were performed on raw spike counts for single unit and population data, comparing the activity on saccade and reach trials (in which the target appeared in the RF), for each of the intervals of the delayed-response task.
Detailed information on the classification of FEF neurons into visual, visuomovement, and movement cell types can be found in Lawrence et al. (2005). Briefly, for each neuron, the classification was based on the contrast ratio between visual and motor responses ([motor − visual]/[motor + visual]) on saccade trials of the delayed-response task for each neuron. Neurons with a contrast ratio of −1.0 had purely visual-related responses, while neurons with a contrast ratio of 1.0 had purely movement-related responses. Correlation was used to examine the relationship between the effector-bias in the response (saccade minus reach) and the visuomotor index. Separate correlations were performed for each interval of the delayed-response task (target, delay, movement).
Individual neurons with visuomotor indices ranging from −1.0 to −0.4 were classified as visual neurons, those with indices between −0.4 and 0.4 were classified as visuomovement neurons, and those with indices from 0.4 to 1.0 were classified as movement neurons. Separate one-way ANOVAs were then performed, one on each interval of the delayed- response task, to determine whether effector biases differed across the three cell types. For ANOVAs with significant F values, one-sample t tests were performed to identify which cell types showed significant effector biases.
To determine whether the activity on saccade and reach trials into the RF diverged during the delayed-response task, the data were digitally low-pass filtered at 24 Hz. Divergence was defined as the start of the first interval at which 25 consecutive pairs of points differed by at least 3 SEs from one another. For all other analyses, no filtering was used. For presentation purposes only, we low-pass filtered the neuronal data in Figures 2 (4 Hz low pass filter) and 4 (9 Hz low pass filter).
Results
Single neurons
The majority of single neurons in FEF demonstrated greater activity on saccade trials compared to reach trials during at least one interval of the delayed-response task. Examples of the activity of single visual, visuomovement, and movement neurons on saccade trials (black) and reach trials (gray) are presented in Figure 2. For each neuron, the activity is aligned both on the presentation of the target (left) and on the onset of the movement (right).
An example visual neuron, presented in Figure 2 (left panel), exhibited a burst of activity in response to the onset of the target with significantly greater activity on saccade trials than on reach trials (53.33 ± 6.15 sp/s and 35.33 ± 5.24 sp/s, respectively; mean ± SE; t test, p < 0.05; interval 100–200 ms following the onset of the target in the RF). This response was maintained throughout the delay period with significantly greater activity on saccade trials than on reach trials (46.13 ± 2.0 sp/s and 32.13 ± 2.02 sp/s, respectively; p < 0.0001; interval 450–950 ms following the onset of the target in the RF). In the two-hundred-millisecond interval before the onset of the movement, the activity on saccade trials, but not on reach trials, decreased precipitously. In fact, there was no significant difference in activity on saccade and reach trials in the 100 ms interval before the onset of the movement into the RF (41.33 ± 5.33 sp/s and 50.00 ± 4.14 sp/s, respectively; p > 0.20). Following the onset of movement into the RF, the response reversed, with significantly greater activity on reach trials than on saccade trials (32.00 ± 3.80 sp/s and 1.0 ± 0.72 sp/s, respectively, p < 0.0001, interval 200–400 ms following the onset of the movement into the RF). Thus, this visual neuron demonstrates an early bias for saccade targets that was maintained throughout the delay period but was reversed following the onset of the movement.
An example visuomovement neuron, presented in Figure 2 (middle panel), exhibited a similar burst of activity in response to target onset on saccade and reach trials (85.0 ± 9.57 sp/s and 71.0 ± 10.80 sp/s, respectively; p > 0.30). Following the transient burst, there was an increase in activity during the delay period on saccade trials but not on reach trials. The delay period activity on saccade trials was significantly greater than the delay period activity on reach trials (36.60 ± 5.55 sp/s and 11.80 ± 1.92 sp/s; p < 0.001). In the final 100 ms of the delay period, activity was 70.0 ± 8.03 sp/s on saccade trials but only 13.0 ± 3.35 sp/s on reach trials (p < 0.0001). Before the onset of the movement, activity remained significantly greater on saccade trials (87.0 ± 9.32 sp/s) than on reach trials (29.0 ± 5.47 sp/s; p < 0.0001). Activity decreased precipitously with the onset of movement on saccade trials. Thus, this visuomovement neuron demonstrates a bias for saccades during the delay period and around the time of the movement.
An example movement neuron, presented in Figure 2 (right panel), was unresponsive on both saccade and reach trials after target onset (5.33 ± 2.15 sp/s and 6.00 ± 2.35 sp/s, saccade and reach trials respectively, p > 0.84) and during the early delay period (3.67 ± 1.24 sp/s and 1.33 ± 0.77 sp/s, saccade and reach trials respectively, p > 0.10; interval 450 – 650 ms following the onset of the target in the RF). However, late in the delay period, a differential response emerged, with significantly greater activity on saccade trials than on reach trials (10.33 ± 2.51 sp/s and 2.00 ± 0.82 sp/s, p < 0.01; interval 750–950 ms following the onset of the target in the RF). In the 100 ms interval before the onset of the movement into the RF, this neuron responded robustly on saccade trials but not at all on reach trials (107.33 ± 7.40 sp/s and 2.00 ± 1.07 sp/s, respectively; p < 0.0001). Thus, this neuron demonstrates an overwhelming bias for saccades very late in the delay period and around the time of the movement.
Delayed-saccade and delayed-reach trials were divided into three intervals: target (50 to 350 ms interval following the onset of the target), delay (450 to 950 ms following the onset of the target), and movement (100 ms interval preceding the onset of the movement), unless otherwise noted. Table 1 presents the percentage (and number) of visual, visuomovement, and movement neurons that were significantly biased (p ≤ 0.05) for saccades (S > R) or reaches (R > S) during any one interval of the delayed- response task. We found that similar percentages of visual, visuomovement and movement neurons showed a significant bias for saccades over reaches (50%, 58%, and 58%, respectively; left hand column) during at least one interval of the delayed-response task. While a few cells showed the reverse pattern, that is, significantly greater responses on reach compared to saccade trials (right-hand column), the number of cells with a significant reach bias was not greater than would be expected by chance for any of the three cell types (proportion test, all p values >0.5). Table 2 further presents separately, for each interval of the delayed-response task, the percentage (and number) of visual, visuomovement, and movement neurons that were significantly biased (see “Significant”, p ≤ 0.05) for saccades or reaches. The percentage (and number) of neurons for which the saccade or reach bias did not reach significance is also presented (see “Nonsignificant”).
As is evident in Table 2, in visual neurons, the response to the target and during the delay period was significantly greater on saccade trials than on reach trials in almost one-third of these neurons. The percentage of neurons with a significant bias for saccades decreased to less than one- fourth of all visual neurons around the time of the movement. In visuomovement neurons, the response to the onset of the target was significantly greater on saccade trials than on reach trials in just over one-tenth of all neurons. The percentage of neurons with a significant bias increased to slightly more than one-fourth of all visuomovement neurons during the delay period, and to almost one-half of all visuomovement neurons around the time of the movement. Movement neurons, by definition, were relatively nonresponsive to the onset of the target. Not surprisingly, then, there no neurons with significantly greater activity on saccade trials than on reach trials around the time of target onset. During the delay period, however, a significant bias for saccades developed in one-sixth of all movement neurons, and increased sharply around the time of the movement to almost two thirds of all movement neurons. It should be noted that, because we were interested the activity that could be directly involved in triggering a movement, we quantified the movement-related response in the 100 ms interval before the onset of the movement (see Materials and Methods). Four movement neurons demonstrated a bias for saccades only after the onset of the saccade (300 to 500 ms following the onset of the movement) (see supplemental Material, Fig. S3, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material, for an example neuron). As a result, these neurons were not included in the total percentage of movement neurons with saccade-biased responses during the movement interval.
To examine the relationship between cell type and saccade bias, we plotted the difference in activity between saccade and reach trials for individual neurons as a function of the visuomotor index, for target, delay, and movement intervals of the delayed-saccade task (Fig. 3). The visuomotor index was constructed by calculating the contrast ratio between visual and motor responses for each neuron (see Materials and Methods for additional details). Neurons with indices from −1.0 to −0.4 were classified as visual neurons (on the left of each panel), those with indices between −0.4 and 0.4 as visuomovement neurons (center of each panel), and those with indices from 0.4 to 1.0 as movement neurons (on the right of each panel). Data points above the horizontal line represent cells with a saccade bias, while points below the line represent cells with a reach bias. Filled and unfilled data points represent cells from M1 and M2, respectively.
Significant negative correlations were found between the saccade bias and the visuomotor index for the target and delay intervals (r = −0.25, p < 0.05, both intervals) as the saccade bias decreased from visual to movement neurons during these intervals, and a significant positive correlation was found for the movement interval as the saccade bias increased from visual to movement neurons (r = 0.36, p < 0.001). For each of the three intervals, data were then binned by cell type (see superimposed bar graphs in Fig. 3) and subjected to an ANOVA. All three ANOVAs were significant (all F(1,85) > 4.0, all p < 0.05). For the target and delay intervals, there was a significant saccade bias in visual neurons (t(29) > 2.5, p < 0.05), but not in visuomovement (t(30) < 1.35, p > 0.30) or movement neurons (t(25) < 1, p > 0.30). In contrast, for the movement interval, there was a significant saccade bias in visuomovement and movement neurons (t(30) = 4.76, p < 0.001, t(25) = 4.24, p < 0.001, respectively), but not in visual neurons (t(29) = 1.43, p > 0.15). Thus, the saccade bias to the onset of the target and during the delay period decreases from visual to movement neurons, while the saccade bias around the time of the movement increases from visual to movement neurons.
Populations
Population averaged responses on saccade trials (red) and reach trials (green) are presented in Figure 4 (see supplemental Material, Fig. S4, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material, for the population averaged responses shown separately for each animal). The top panel shows that visual neurons are biased to respond to, and maintain, visual information for saccades. More specifically, there was significantly greater activity on saccade trials than on reach trials in response to the onset of the target (saccade minus reach: 5.28 ± 1.91 sp/s; t(29) = 2.76; p < 0.01) and during the delay period (saccade minus reach: 6.40 ± 2.37 sp/s; t(29) = 2.70; p < 0.05), but not around the time of the movement (4.88 ± 3.41 sp/s; t(29) = 1.43; p > 0.15). Following the movement into the RF (300 to 500 ms following the onset of the movement), the pattern of activity was reversed, with greater activity on reach trials than on saccade trials (saccade minus reach: −5.88 ± 2.71 sp/s; t(29)=-2.17; p < 0.05).
In contrast to the population of visual neurons, the population of visuomovement neurons (Fig. 4, middle panel) did not show a bias for saccades until late in the delay period. While the population of visuomovement neurons was responsive to target onset in the RF, there was no significant difference in the response on saccade and reach trials (saccade minus reach: 1.75 ± 1.77 sp/s; t(30) = 0.99; p > 0.30). In fact, visuomovement neurons did not become saccade biased until late in the delay period (saccade minus reach: 10.44 ± 2.91 sp/s; t(30) = 3.58; p < 0.01; 950–1150 ms interval following the onset of the target in the RF). These neurons were highly saccade biased around the time of the movement (saccade minus reach: 15.02 ± 3.16 sp/s; t(30) = 4.76; p < 0.0001). Following the movement into the RF, there was no difference in the response on saccade and reach trials (saccade minus reach: −1.83 ± 2.11 sp/s; t(30) = 0.86; p < 0.35).
The population of movement neurons (Fig. 4, bottom panel) showed little task-related modulation until just before the onset of movement. Thus, there was no difference between saccade and reach trials in response to the presentation of the target (saccade minus reach; −0.32 ± 0.61 sp/s; t(25)=-0.52; p > 0.60), or during the delay period (saccade minus reach; 0.92 ± 0.93 sp/s; t(25) = 0.99; p > 0.33). A saccade bias emerged late in the delay period (interval 950 – 1150 ms following the onset of the target; saccade minus reach; 10.02 ± 2.40 sp/s; t(25) = 4.17; p < 0.001), and increased dramatically around the time of the movement (saccade–reach; 28.11 ± 6.62 sp/s; t(25) = 4.24; p < 0.0001). Following the movement into the RF, the response on saccade and reach trials returned to baseline levels (saccade minus reach: 1.21± 2.13 sp/s; t(25) = 0.57; p > 0.50).
A direct comparison of the time course of activity on saccade and reach trials confirmed these findings. On average, activity on saccade and reach trials first became biased for saccades in visual neurons 116 ms after the onset of the visual stimulus (see Materials and Methods for details of the analysis). Activity in visuomovement neurons, in contrast, did not show a sustained bias until 953 ms after target onset, or 147 ms before the end of the memory period. Similarly, a sustained bias did not emerge in movement neurons until 1016 ms after target onset. Thus, the responses of visual neurons became biased for saccades shortly after the onset of the visual target (Fig. 4, top panel). In contrast, the responses of visuomovement and movement neurons did not become biased for saccades until late in the delay period (Fig. 4, middle and bottom panels, respectively).
While the response around the time of the movement was biased for saccades, there was nonetheless an increase in activity just before reaches, especially in visuomovement neurons. This reach-related activity (in the 100 ms interval preceding the onset of the movement) was significantly greater on trials in which the reach was executed to a remembered target in the RF than out of the RF in visuomovement neurons (difference of 6.04 ± 1.95 sp/s; p < 0.01), but not in visual (1.77 ± 1.59 sp/s; p > 0.25) or movement neurons (1.52 ± 2.03 sp/s; p > 0.45). While the functional significance of response around the time of the reach is difficult to interpret, these results suggest that the response in visuomovement neurons was spatially specific and was not due to nonspatial factors such as arousal.
Discussion
The present results demonstrate that visual neurons in FEF are biased to respond to and maintain visual targets for saccadic eye movements. Fifty percent of visual neurons were significantly more active on saccade trials than on reach trials during at least one interval of the delayed-response task, and only 13% of visual neurons showed the reverse effect (Table 1). These percentages are similar to the corresponding values for visuomovement and movement cells (58% and 10%, and 58% and 19%, respectively). A strong bias for saccade compared to reach trials is also seen in the population-averaged responses of all three cell types, including visual neurons (Fig. 4). While the saccade bias in the population of visuomovement and movement neurons occurred primarily during the movement period, the saccade bias in the population of visual neurons emerged shortly after target onset and was maintained throughout the delay period. These results suggest that while visual neurons are not directly involved in triggering saccadic eye movements, they are nonetheless highly biased to respond to, and maintain, target locations for saccades compared to reaches.
Relation to previous research
The present results are consistent with the results of the classic study of Bruce and Goldberg (1985) which compared FEF responses during saccade and reach trials and concluded that “frontal eye field visual activity is best understood as providing targets for visually guided saccades, and not as participating in a more general analysis of the visual world.” However, several recent lines of inquiry have suggested that visual neurons in FEF do play a more general role in visual processing. In particular, a number of studies have argued that visual neurons are involved in allocating visual attention to salient target locations independent of whether such locations are the goal of a saccade (Thompson et al., 1997, 2005; Murthy et al., 2001; Sato et al., 2003).
One such study, the results of which are reprinted in Figure 5 (top), found that visual neurons in FEF discriminate between a target (thick trace) and a distractor (thin trace), even when the required response to the target is a lever turn and not a saccade (Thompson et al., 2005). These results suggest that visual neurons do not preferentially respond to targets for upcoming saccades, but instead respond to any task-relevant target regardless of how that target will be used. In contrast, the results of the present study suggest that visual neurons preferentially respond to targets for upcoming saccades (Fig. 5, bottom). Indeed, the target-evoked activity on reach trials declined rapidly to baseline or near-baseline levels, despite the fact that the target was clearly important to the animal's performance of the task. Thus, the Thompson et al. (2005) study suggests that the response of a visual neuron in FEF is determined by whether or not the stimulus is task-relevant (i.e., salient), whereas the current study suggests that the response is determined by whether or not the stimulus is the target of an upcoming saccade. How can this discrepancy be resolved?
One possibility is that, despite being task relevant, attention is not allocated to reach targets (Bisley and Goldberg, 2003). The results of Deubel and Schneider (2003), however, suggest that attention is allocated to reach targets, at least initially (see also Deubel et al., 1998). They probed the locus of attention shortly after the cue to move in delayed-saccade and -reach trials. The discrimination of a probe away from the movement endpoint was impaired on saccade trials, consistent with the well known finding that attention is coupled to the saccade endpoint around the time of the movement (Shepherd et al., 1986; Hoffman and Subramaniam, 1995; Deubel and Schneider, 1996). This was not the case for long delayed reach trials (>300 ms). For these trials, a probe appearing 80 ms after a cue to initiate a reach was discriminated equally well at all locations, both at and away from the reach endpoint. These results suggest that attention is not sustained at reach targets. They are not conclusive, however, since they quantify attention as the movement is initiated and not during the delay period. Thus, while it is possible that monkeys do not attend to reach targets during a delay period and that visual neurons in FEF reflect the attended locations in space in both the current study and in Thompson et al. (2005), the evidence is inconclusive.
A related possibility is that attention is allocated to reach targets, but that the neuronal correlates of this attentional allocation are not found within the saccade system (FEF), but are found instead within the reach system (e.g., dorsal premotor cortex). Such a possibility would necessitate different attentional systems for different effectors. While this may seem improbable, Rizzolatti et al. (1994), in their formulation of the “premotor theory of attention,” which hypothesizes that the circuitry for shifts of attention and goal-directed movements is one and the same, raise this very possibility. (A strict interpretation of the premotor theory of attention would dictate that the neuronal correlates of attentional allocation would be found in neurons that are thought to be directly involved in triggering a movement, for example, FEF movement neurons. The present results suggest instead that while the neuronal correlates of attentional allocation may be found within the circuitry of a movement system, they are not found in those neurons directly involved in triggering the movement.)
The premotor theory of Rizzolatti et al. (1994) hypothesizes that shifts of spatial attention before saccades result from the activation of the saccade circuitry, and similarly, that shifts of spatial attention before reaches result from the activation of the reach circuitry. This would necessitate, however, that the activity in visual neurons in Thompson et al. (2005) is saccade related. How could this be the case when a saccade was never executed to a target in their study? It is possible that the target in the visual search task used in Thompson et al. (2005) not only attracts attention but also evokes a plan for a saccade. In both the search and the reach task, the subject is explicitly and carefully trained to suppress the natural impulse to look at the target. A residual saccade plan may nonetheless remain. This plan may be stronger in the search task, where receiving a reward requires making a difficult discrimination. In contrast, target identification and localization in the reach task is quite easy, and therefore a plan to saccade to the target may be present only transiently. Thus, the results of both Thompson et al. (2005) and the current study could be explained if FEF visual neurons specifically encode the location of targets for saccades.
Relation to other areas
Previously, Snyder and colleagues have found that the responses of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) and in the parietal reach region (PRR) of the parietal cortex are biased for planning saccades and reaches, respectively (Snyder et al., 1997; Quian Quiroga et al., 2006), and that there is a nonspatial component to these planning signals, since the bias appears even when the spatial target of the movement is not yet known (Calton et al., 2002; Dickinson et al., 2003). They hypothesized that LIP and PRR are nodes in a saccade and reach network, respectively, that also includes frontal and subcortical saccade centers (e.g., frontal eye fields, superior colliculus, cerebellum) and reach centers (e.g., dorsal premotor cortex) (Snyder et al., 2000). They further hypothesized that the relationship between areas within each network may be either hierarchical or distributed. Frontal cortex may elaborate nascent saccade and reach plans from the parietal cortex (a hierarchical or serial architecture), or frontal and parietal cortex may work in parallel to produce saccade and reach plans (a distributed architecture) (Snyder et al., 2000). Recently, we have shown that nonspatial saccade planning signals are present in FEF, but that neither the onset nor the magnitude of these signals are earlier, or larger, respectively, in frontal cortex than in parietal cortex, consistent with a distributed model (Lawrence and Snyder, 2006). The fact that saccades and reaches are coordinated suggests that saccade and reach networks interact. This interaction could begin as early as parietal cortex (Cohen and Andersen, 2002). Neurons in the frontal cortex, the supplementary eye field, the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA), and the SMA all respond differently to isolated eye or arm movements compared to coordinated movements, but this is not true of FEF (Mushiake et al., 1996; Fujii et al., 2002). Although the present study was not designed to examine the interaction between saccades and reaches, the lack of FEF modulation during reach trials shown in the current study, combined with the results of Mushiake et al. (1996), suggests that saccade-reach interactions do not occur in the FEF.
The saccade bias in visual neurons in FEF could provide a “top-down” attention signal to earlier visual areas. Indeed, presaccadic enhancement of visual responses have been found in V4 (Fischer and Boch, 1981a,b; Tolias et al., 2001; Mazer and Gallant, 2003; Moore and Chang, 2009). Also consistent with top-down attentional modulation, Moore and Armstrong (2003) have found that stimulation of FEF neurons enhances visual responses in retinotopically corresponding locations of V4. It seems likely that V4 neurons would be similarly biased during a delay period before a saccade. Indeed, the results of recent functional MRI studies are consistent with a saccade bias in the extrastriate cortex of humans (Levy et al., 2007; Beurze et al., 2009). An interesting question for future research is whether V4 neurons are also enhanced during the delay period before a reach in the absence of a saccade. Such an effect would presumably be mediated by projections from areas other than FEF.
Footnotes
-
The project described was supported by awards to L.H.S. (R01 EY012135) and to B.M.L. (F32 EY013898) from the National Eye Institute (NEI). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NEI or the National Institutes of Health.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Bonnie M. Lawrence, Department of Psychology, Mather Memorial Building, #109, Case Western Reserve University, 10900 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH 44106. bonnie.lawrence{at}case.edu