Abstract
Motor sequence learning on the serial reaction time task involves the integration of response-, stimulus-, and effector-based information. Human primary motor cortex (M1) and the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) have been identified with supporting the learning of effector-dependent and -independent information, respectively. Current neurocognitive data are, however, exclusively based on learning complex sequence information via perceptual-motor responses. Here, we investigated the effects of continuous theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation (cTBS)-induced disruption of M1 and the angular gyrus (AG) of the IPL on learning a probabilistic sequence via sequential perceptual-motor responses (experiment 1) or covert orienting of visuospatial attention (experiment 2). Functional effects on manual sequence learning were evident during 75% of training trials in the cTBS M1 condition, whereas cTBS over the AG resulted in interference confined to a midpoint during the training phase. Posttraining direct (declarative) tests of sequence knowledge revealed that cTBS over M1 modulated the availability of newly acquired sequence knowledge, whereby sequence knowledge was implicit in the cTBS M1 condition but was available to conscious awareness in the cTBS AG and control conditions. In contrast, perceptual sequence learning was abolished in the perceptual cTBS AG condition, whereas learning was intact and available to conscious awareness in the cTBS M1 and control conditions. These results show that the right AG had a critical role in perceptual sequence learning, whereas M1 had a causal role in developing experience-dependent functional attributes relevant to conscious knowledge on manual but not perceptual sequence learning.
Introduction
Numerous cognitive faculties and behaviors, such as language acquisition, cognitive skill learning, and motor skill learning, can be conceptualized as a sequence learning process. Perceptual-motor aspects of sequence learning have been examined extensively using the serial reaction time task (SRT task), wherein sequential manual key presses confined to four fixed locations are performed in response to visual targets that appear at one of four corresponding spatial locations (Nissen and Bullemer, 1987; Shanks, 2005). Human functional neuroimaging studies indicate that a cortico-cerebellar network underlies sequence learning on the SRT task, with activation in the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) identified with coding an effector-independent (visual/spatial coordinates) description of successive locations (Grafton et al., 1998b; Seidler et al., 2005), whereas the coding of effector-specific (motor/response coordinates) information in egocentric space is identified with early activation within the primary motor cortex (M1) (Grafton et al., 1998b; Willingham, 1998). Current neurocognitive data are, however, only informative about the neural areas related to learning complex sequence information involving perceptual-motor responses.
Recent behavioral evidence has shown that learning on the SRT task can proceed when responses are confined to sequential covert reorienting of visuospatial attention (Song et al., 2008). Hence, motor responses are not a necessary condition for complex sequence learning. This is an important consideration because task demands lead to different instantiations of the response-, stimulus-, and effector-based information that underlie sequence learning (Clegg et al., 1998). By extension, unless the contribution of the IPL to sequence learning can be shown to be independent of response mode, it is conceivable that activation within the IPL during manual sequence learning reflects the operation of more general mechanisms identified with the parietal cortex, such as perceptual-motor integration, task difficulty, or spatial attention (Willingham et al., 2002; Majerus et al., 2006). Here, we examine the role of the right angular gyrus (AG)—a subregion within the IPL—in sequence learning because it has been implicated in the maintenance and control of covert reorienting of attention over time within bilateral hemispaces, as part of a network supporting the detection of salient new items embedded in a sequence (Sack, 2009).
Given these limits on understanding the cortical substrate underlying sequence learning, we investigated the role of M1 and the right AG by administering continuous theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation (cTBS) (three pulses at 50 Hz, repeated every 200 ms) before training on the SRT task. We used the cTBS protocol because it induces disruption that outlasts the time of stimulation (up to 60 min) and the effects are mediated by an analog of the type of long-term potentiation- and/or long-term depression-like changes in synaptic transmission that have been implicated in learning and memory (Ziemann et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2005, 2008; Cheeran et al., 2008). Responses on the SRT task involved either sequential unimanual key presses and free eye movements (experiment 1; manual condition) or covert reorienting of visuospatial attention (experiment 2; perceptual condition). Visual targets followed a probabilistic second-order conditional rule on which transition probabilities between spatial locations were unpredictable (improbable) on 15% of trials (Shanks et al., 2006). We exploited this stochastic property to measure the time course of the cTBS-induced effect on learning [reaction time (RT) to probable vs improbable trials] independently of general response execution and to encourage learning without awareness (implicit vs explicit learning). Learning and awareness were assessed using posttraining subjective, indirect (procedural) and direct (declarative) tests (Albouy et al., 2008). If the IPL is involved with coding effector-independent information related to learning per se, transient disruption should interfere with learning on the SRT task independently of the response mode. Conversely, transient disruption of the hypothesized effector-dependent contribution of M1 should affect learning on the manual, but not perceptual, SRT task.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Forty-eight participants (46 right-handed, 2 left-handed) (Oldfield, 1971) were recruited from the undergraduate student population at Imperial College London. Local research ethics committee approval was granted for the experimental procedure. All participants provided written informed consent and were screened to exclude those with a history of neurological disorder or head injury. Twenty-four participants took part in experiment 1 and 24 participants took part in experiment 2. Within each experiment, the participants were subdivided into three groups according to whether cTBS was administered over the dominant hand area of the primary motor cortex (cTBS M1HAND; n = 8), the angular gyrus of the inferior parietal lobule (cTBS AG; n = 8), or administered as sham cTBS over the vertex (electrode site, Cz; control; n = 8). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. None of the participants had previous experience of the SRT task or sequence learning tasks. All participants were naive to TMS.
Design and materials
Experiment 1 and experiment 2 each comprised three phases (Fig. 1): (1) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-guided neuronavigation and cTBS; (2) a training phase consisting of either a manual SRT task (experiment 1) or perceptual SRT task (experiment 2) performed alongside a vigilance task; and (3) a posttraining direct test phase comprising a sequence awareness questionnaire (Curran, 1997), a manual (experiment 1) or perceptual (experiment 2) cued-generation task, and a manual (experiment 1) or perceptual (experiment 2) recognition test. Conjoint performance across the subjective, indirect (procedural) and direct (declarative) tests administered during phase 3 was used to detect learning and, importantly, to be highly sensitive to the extent to which knowledge was primarily implicit or explicit (Destrebecqz and Cleeremans, 2001). All of the behavioral tasks were implemented using a proprietary program written in C++.
Phase 2.
Training on experiments 1 and 2 involved a 3 (site of stimulation: cTBS M1HAND, cTBS AG, control) × 12 (training blocks: 12) mixed-factorial design, with site as a between-subjects factor and block as a within-subjects factor. Each block of the manual SRT task and perceptual SRT task comprised 100 trials. Twelve training blocks were administered after an initial pseudorandom practice sequence. In each block, ninety-six trials were critical items and the first four trials were buffers. Two basic sequences were generated in accordance with a second-order conditional (SOC) deterministic rule, such that each target location was determined on the basis of the two preceding targets (Reed and Johnson, 1994) (Fig. 1, phase 2). These sequences—SOC1 and SOC2—were modified so that the assigned training sequence appeared according to the target second-order transitions with a probability 0.85 and were replaced by second-order transitions from an alternate sequence with a probability 0.15. Hence, there were short- and longer-range predictive target locations—a probable/improbable ratio of 0.85:0.15 can be learned to asymptote after 9–12 blocks of trials (Shanks et al., 2006). Each block of the 96 critical trials began at a random point in the primary sequence. Target sequences were counterbalanced across participants.
Visual stimuli comprised two sizes of target—a standard target (5 mm diameter) and a large-diameter target (LDT) (8 mm diameter)—that appeared in one of four squares (35 × 35 mm) arranged along the horizontal meridian against a white background. Visual stimuli were presented on a 21 inch Sony Trinitron cathode ray tube computer monitor configured to a screen resolution of 1024 × 769 pixels at a 100 Hz refresh rate, positioned at a 75 cm viewing distance. Screen locations of the squares corresponded with positions 1–4 as read from left to right, with the spatial locations corresponding to numeric values of the probabilistic SOC sequence. In each block of the manual and perceptual SRT task, LDTs were presented between 18 and 36% of trials; the order of the LDTs was random within a block and set at a proportion to ensure that participants performed at ceiling.
Phase 3.
Direct tests of sequence knowledge immediately followed the SRT task and were administered in the same response mode as the training phase (Fig. 1). The sequence awareness questionnaire comprised five propositions (Curran, 1997). A novel cued-generation task followed the sequence awareness questionnaire and comprised an inclusion test (I) and an exclusion test (E). Test order was counterbalanced across participants. Stimulus materials on each trial were generated by starting at each ordinal position of SOC1 and SOC2 for two consecutive positions. Sequence knowledge on the cued-generation task is inferred if follow-on self-generated responses on the inclusion test and/or exclusion test exceed baseline (B) (for a full specification of the baseline, see Results). Intentional, conscious control of acquired sequence knowledge is inferred if participants are able to respond in accordance with test instructions (i.e., I trained > E trained; E trained = B). In contrast, a null effect (i.e., I trained = E trained, or E trained > B) is construed as evidence of implicit knowledge (Destrebecqz and Cleeremans, 2001).
The recognition test followed the cued-generation task and comprised 12 trained (old) and 12 untrained (new) six-element sequences. Twelve sequences (starting from each ordinal position of the 12-element SOC sequence for six consecutive locations) were generated from SOC1 and 12 were generated from SOC2. To ensure sensitivity to even partial declarative knowledge, a six-point scale was used to obtain a confidence rating for each six-element sequence (Shanks and Johnstone, 1999). Manual recognition test performance was also examined for response priming to trained (vs untrained) sequences because recognition is dependent on explicit knowledge and on the processing fluency associated with performing each six-element sequence (Shanks and Perruchet, 2002).
MRI-guided neuronavigation and cTBS
All participants in the M1 and AG cTBS groups underwent an MRI anatomical scan. Scans were obtained in anterior commissural–posterior commissural orientation, using high-resolution (256 slices, 1 mm apart), axial T1-weighted (magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo) images. Scans were acquired using a 1.5 T clinical scanner fitted with an echo-planar imaging upgrade (Siemens).
TMS coil positioning was guided using optical tracking via the frameless stereotactic system (BrainSight; Rogue Research). MRI neuronavigation was used to reduce the potential impact that low statistical power can have on being able to reject the null (Sack et al., 2009). In the cTBS AG condition, the right AG was identified within the posterior region of the IPL as the area directly adjacent to the dorsolateral projection of the superior temporal sulcus; the superior temporal sulcus bifurcates the AG. The AG was localized for each participant in slice and three-dimensional-rendered T1-weighted MRI brain scans using a frameless stereotaxy system (BrainSight; Rogue Research). These data were used to coregister the neuroanatomical site with the scalp surface. Stimulation was administered using a figure-of-eight coil (70 mm diameter) connected to a Magstim Super Rapid stimulator.
The M1HAND site of stimulation was selected as the site that evoked maximum movement in the hand contralateral to the site of stimulation at the lowest level of stimulator output. Individual resting motor threshold (RMT) for M1HAND, as a measure of M1 stimulation (Robertson et al., 2003), was used to determine stimulator output for cTBS in the AG and M1HAND conditions. RMT was established as the lowest output intensity of single-pulse TMS to elicit at least three consecutive movements in the contralateral relaxed hand after five pulses of TMS. cTBS [trains of three pulses at 50 Hz repeated at 200 ms intervals (i.e., at 5 Hz) (Huang et al., 2005)] was applied at 70% of RMT immediately before training on the SRT task. In experiment 1, there was no significant difference in the mean stimulator output between cTBS M1HAND (mean of 44) and AG (mean of 44) conditions (t(14) = 0.14, p = 0.89); correspondingly, in experiment 2, there was no significant difference in the mean stimulator output between the cTBS M1HAND (mean of 44) and AG (mean of 48) conditions (t(14) = 0.99, p = 0.33).
In experiment 1, cTBS was applied for a single continuous 20 s train, whereas in experiment 2, cTBS was applied for 30 s; the longer duration was intended to address an anticipated increase in the training time when recording eye movements on the perceptual SRT task. In the control condition, a commercially available double 70 mm placebo coil system positioned over the Cz electrode vertex position was used to administer stimulation at the same frequency as used with the cTBS conditions (Magstim). The coil replicates the appearance and operation of the standard double 70 mm coil used in the AG and M1 conditions and provides mild cutaneous stimulation and discharge noise without stimulating cortical tissue. An IBM personal computer-compatible computer running E-Prime software (version 1.1; Psychology Software Tools) controlled the delivery of cTBS. Participants wore a close-fitting nylon scalp cap to enable marking of the regions on the scalp designated to overlie the M1HAND area, AG, and the vertex control site (Cz electrode position) (Jasper, 1958).
Eye movement recording
Eye movements were recorded using an Eyelink II infrared eye tracker to ensure continuous central fixation during each block of the perceptual SRT task and perceptual direct tests (spatial resolution, <0.5°; SR Research). Eye movements were monitored in real time by the experimenter to ensure that participants maintained central fixation. Head movements were minimized using a chin rest. Verbal feedback was provided if a participant deviated from central fixation. Viewing was binocular. Monocular, pupil-only eye tracking of the dominant eye was conducted at a sampling rate of 250 Hz. Calibration and validation of eye position was performed using a five-element array. Eye movement fixation data were analyzed offline using a proprietary application developed within MATLAB version 6.5.1 (MathWorks). Saccades with latency of 12 ms or less were excluded from a trial sample, as were saccades that began and ended within a central region (1°). Less than 7% of trials in the perceptual cTBS M1HAND, cTBS AG, and control conditions were associated with saccades, which indicates that participants were able to maintain central fixation.
Behavioral tasks
Participants were tested individually, and both experiments were performed in a dark visual Ganzfeld. An experimental session lasted ∼1 h 30 min in experiment 1 and 1 h 45 min in experiment 2.
Manual and perceptual SRT tasks (phase 2).
Each trial of the SRT task comprised a single target stimulus presented at one of four possible locations. On the manual SRT task, participants were instructed to press one of four keys in response to target onset (RB Series Response Pad, model RB-730; Cedrus Corporation) and were allowed to make free eye movements. Keys were marked 1, 2, 3, and 4 and corresponded to screen locations 1 (left) to 4 (right). Responses in the manual SRT task were made to locations 1–4 with the digitus secundus manus, digitus medius, digitus annularis, and digitus minimus manus of the dominant hand, respectively. On detection of a manual response, the target stimulus was extinguished and the next trial started, or, if no response was detected, the target was extinguished after 3000 ms. On the perceptual SRT task, participants were instructed to maintain central fixation on a circle (0.3 cm diameter) positioned midway between locations 2 and 3. Each trial began with the presentation of a target stimulus at the center of one of the four squares (1000 ms). Extinction of the target was followed by a 200 ms interval. Hence, peripheral targets captured attention in an exogenous and automatic manner, as a component of the endogenous allocation of attention (Juola et al., 1995).
Vigilance task.
Alongside the manual and perceptual SRT tasks, participants were instructed to maintain a cumulative count of the number of LDTs presented during each block and report the value at the end of each block. The vigilance task was important for ensuring that participants attended to the sequence of visual stimuli, particularly on the perceptual SRT task. On-screen feedback on vigilance task performance was based on accuracy relative to the actual number of LDTs: participants entering a count within 5% accuracy were informed that their count was accurate and were asked to continue with their good performance; participants responding with count of 5% error or greater were shown their percentage underestimation or overestimation and were instructed to try harder in the forthcoming block of trials.
Posttraining direct tests of learning and awareness (phase 3).
After selecting a response on the sequence awareness questionnaire to indicate the extent to which regularities had been detected during the training session, participants were informed that the target stimuli had followed a regular repeating sequence during the training session. Each trial of the cued-generation task (manual and perceptual) involved the presentation of two consecutive cue stimuli on the computer screen (Fig. 1, phase 3b). Participants were instructed to either respond with a manual key press (experiment 1; manual cued-generation task) or attend to each cue (experiment 2; perceptual cued-generation task) and then use knowledge acquired on the SRT task to either enter four key presses (experiment 1) or vocalize (experiment 2) four locations that followed on from the two-cue sequence in accordance the test instructions. Instructions on the inclusion test directed participants to respond with the next four positions of the training sequence, even if they felt they could not remember particular parts of the sequence that followed the cues. On the exclusion test, participants were asked to respond with a sequence that was novel and avoided the training sequence. Participants were reminded to avoid responding with contiguous repetitions of a single location or natural sequences because these were not presented during training (e.g., 4-4-2-1 or 1-2-3-4). On the perceptual cued-generation task, the experimenter entered a key press contingent with each vocal response. All self-generated responses led to a black circle appearing in a corresponding target location. After completion of each cued-generation test, participants were asked to indicate their confidence, on a scale of 0–100, with responding in accordance with the test instructions. Responses to each six-element sequence on the recognition test were performed in the same way as on the SRT task (Fig. 1, phase 3c). After responding to each six-element sequence, participants were asked to determine whether the sequence was “old/seen” or “new/unseen”; that is, participants were asked to decide whether or not the sequence had appeared during the training session. After each old/new discrimination, participants were asked to rate how confident they were in their judgment on the six-point scale.
Results
Experiment 1
Participants in all three between-subject conditions achieved >95% accuracy on the vigilance task, which indicates that participants were able to attend consistently to the target sequence and could reliably discriminate LDTs from standard targets. Mean end times in the cTBS AG, cTBS M1HAND, and control conditions were 32 min 57 s, 31 min 52 s, and 33 min 12 s, respectively.
Response execution and learning on the manual SRT task
Mean error rates collapsed across block were <1% for each of the between-subject conditions. A 3 (site: M1HAND, AG, control) × 12 (block: 12) mixed-factorial ANOVA performed on the mean SRT task error rates revealed no significant main effects or interactions (F values <1.2). Hence, there were no systematic differences in error as a function of site of stimulation or block. This level of error on the SRT task is well within level of response accuracy routinely reported under single-task conditions. Manual RTs for the first four buffer trials of each block and error trials were excluded from the analyses performed on the latency data. To assess the effect of training, RTs were first summarized by computing the mean RTs in each block to probable and improbable trials for each participant. Figure 2a shows the mean blockwise learning scores (mean RT to probable trials − mean RT improbable trials) obtained over the manual SRT task, plotted separately for the cTBS M1HAND, cTBS AG, and control conditions. A negative score that is significantly different from 0 is consistent with priming. A 3 (site: M1HAND, AG, control) × 2 (sequence: probable, improbable) × 12 (block: 12) mixed-factorial ANOVA performed on the mean RTs revealed a significant main effect of sequence (F(1,21) = 23.32, p < 0.0001) and block (F(11,231) = 4.89, p < 0.00001). Importantly, although there was no significant main effect of site (F(2,21) = 2.13, p = 0.14), there was a significant interaction between site and sequence (F(2,21) = 5.37, p < 0.01), which indicates that emergence of sequence knowledge differed between groups. All other interaction terms were not significant (F values <1.4).
Figure 2b shows the emergence of sequence knowledge as a function of the mean end times for each block of the manual SRT task. A 3 (site: M1HAND, AG, control) × 12 (block: 12) mixed-factorial ANOVA performed on the mean block end times revealed a significant main effect of block (F(11,231) = 6121.7, p < 0.00001) and no main effect of site (F(2,21) = 1.01, p = 0.38), and, importantly, there was no interaction between site and block (F(22,231) = 1.36, p = 0.13). Hence, the differences in the emergence of sequence knowledge between each site cannot be readily attributed to systematic differences in the time course underlying training in each between-subjects condition.
To summarize the effects of cTBS on learning at each site as a function of exposure to the sequence, a measure of learning across blocks was calculated by obtaining a composite learning score (mean RT to probable trials − mean RT improbable trials) for epochs at the beginning (blocks 1–4), middle (blocks 5–8), and end (blocks 9–12) of the manual SRT task (Fig. 2b). Mean learning scores for the three epochs are shown in Figure 2c. A 3 (site: M1HAND, AG, control) × 3 (epoch: early, middle, late) mixed-factorial ANOVA performed on the mean composite learning scores revealed significant main effects of site and epoch (F(2,21) = 5.37, p < 0.05; F(2,42) = 3.33, p < 0.05, respectively) and a significant interaction between site and epoch (F(4,42) = 3.71, p < 0.05). The latter interaction indicates a difference in the magnitude of the learning effect between the three groups as a function of exposure to the sequence. Hence, despite the similar blockwise time points, learning proceeded at a different rate in the three groups.
Repeated measures t tests performed against chance (0) revealed significant learning at early, middle, and late epochs in the control condition (t(7) = 2.85, 3.41, and 2.28, p values <0.05), whereas in the cTBS M1HAND condition, learning was only significant in the late epoch (t(7) = 3.57, p < 0.01). RTs associated with probable and improbable transition locations in the cTBS M1HAND condition were equivalent until block 10: block 9 mean end time, 23 min 52 s; block 10 mean start and end times, 24 min 18 s and 26 min 40 s, respectively. In cTBS AG condition, there was significant learning in the early (t(7) = 2.41, p < 0.05) and late (t(7) = 3.39, p < 0.01) epochs, whereas the learning score was not significantly different from 0 in the middle epoch (mean start and end times, 11 min 31 s and 22 min 15 s, respectively) (t(7) = 0.24, p = 0.82). The loss of sensitivity to improbable trials was particularly pronounced at block 6: block 6 mean start and end times, 14 min 17 s and 16 min 50 s, respectively. Overall, evidence of a functional-temporal dissociation between the cTBS M1HAND and cTBS AG conditions combined with the results from control condition suggests that the functional effects were not attributable to nonspecific effects associated with TMS or attributable to systematic between-group differences in the time points related to each block end time.
Effects of cTBS over M1 and AG on posttraining direct tests of learning and awareness
Subjective measures of sequence learning.
Participants in the cTBS M1HAND (mean ± SE, 1.9 ± 0.45) and control (mean ± SE, 1.5 ± 0.45) conditions perceived the movement of target stimuli to be essentially random, whereas participants in the cTBS AG condition (mean ± SE, 2.8 ± 0.45) were aware that some positions occurred more often than others, but there was no significant difference between the three groups (Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA, χ2(2) = 2.41, p = 0.30). We also assessed participants' subjective rating of their performance on the cued-generation task. Participants in all between-subjects conditions were more confident in their ability to generate a novel sequence on the exclusion test (mean ± SE, 51.4 ± 4.75) than generate the trained sequence on the inclusion test (mean ± SE, 24.6 ± 3.85) (F(1,21) = 19.10, p < 0.001). No interactions were significant (F values <1.8).
Cued-generation task.
Mean start times for the cued-generation task in the cTBS M1HAND, cTBS AG, and control conditions were 37 min 4 s, 37 min 3 s, and 37 min 46 s, respectively. Performance was assessed by first calculating the proportion of SOC sequence triplets generated out of the maximum number of correct SOC triplets that could be generated. Four possible triplets could be generated on each of the 12 trials per test, thereby yielding a possible 48 correct SOC triplets on each test. Baseline rates of responding were calculated separately for each test as the proportion of triplets that were inconsistent with the training sequence, i.e., the value was the proportion of triplets that corresponded to the untrained sequence on each free-generation test (Wilkinson and Jahanshahi, 2007).
Figure 3a shows the mean proportions of trained and baseline SOCs generated under each site condition for the inclusion and exclusion tests. A 3 (site: M1HAND, AG, control) × 2 (test: inclusion, exclusion) × 2 (sequence: trained, baseline) mixed-factorial ANOVA revealed significant main effects of test (F(1,21) = 9.99, p < 0.01) and sequence (F(1,21) = 20.74, p < 0.001). The interaction between site, test, and sequence was also significant (F(2,21) = 5.17, p < 0.05). No other effects were significant. Planned comparisons revealed that participants in the cTBS AG and control conditions were able to exert intentional, conscious control when responding on the cued-generation task (i.e., I trained > E trained) (F(1,21) = 14.02, p < 0.01 and F(1,21) = 8.00, p < 0.01, respectively), whereas the generation of trained sequences was equivalent across inclusion and exclusion tests in the cTBS M1 HAND condition (F < 1). Importantly, participants in the cTBS AG condition generated the trained sequence above baseline in the inclusion test (F(1,21) = 8.77, p < 0.01) and were able to exclude the trained sequence from responses on the exclusion test (F(1,21) = 0.35, p = 0.56) (i.e., E trained = B). Conversely, participants in the cTBS M1HAND condition were unable to avoid generating the trained sequence above baseline on the exclusion test (F(1,21) = 15.02, p < 0.001), whereas generation of the trained sequences on the inclusion test was equivalent to baseline (F(1,21) = 1.28, p = 0.28). Participants in the control condition generated the trained sequence above baseline on the inclusion test (F(1,21) = 8.77, p < 0.01) and were able to refrain from generating the trained sequence on the exclusion test (F < 1). Collectively, these results reveal that sequence knowledge in the manual cTBS M1HAND condition was implicit (I = E trained; E trained > B), whereas sequence knowledge was available to conscious, intentional control in the cTBS AG and control conditions (I trained > E trained; I trained > B; E trained = B). All planned comparisons were significant for Bonferroni–Holm corrected p values.
Recognition test confidence ratings and recognition priming.
Mean start times on the recognition test in the cTBS M1HAND, cTBS AG, and control conditions were 45 min 7 s, 45 min 34 s, and 46 min 2 s, respectively. Results from the mean recognition confidence ratings to trained and untrained sequences were consistent with the conclusions drawn from the cued-generation task (Fig. 3b, lower ratings correspond to greater confidence that the sequence was “old”). A 3 (site: M1HAND, AG, control) × 2 (sequence: trained, untrained) mixed-factorial ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of sequence (F(1,21) = 7.26, p < 0.05) and a significant interaction between site and sequence (F(1,21) = 3.78, p < 0.05). No other effects were significant. Planned comparisons based on mean confidence ratings revealed that participants were able to discriminate between trained and untrained sequences in the cTBS AG and control conditions (F(1,21) = 8.49, p < 0.01 and F(1,21) = 5.87, p < 0.05, respectively), whereas in the cTBS M1HAND condition, participants were unable to recognize the trained sequence (F < 1).
Examination of RTs to trained and untrained sequences can provide insight into whether there is evidence of priming of the trained sequence in the absence of recognition (Shanks et al., 2006) (Fig. 4). A 3 (site: M1HAND, AG, control) × 2 (sequence: trained, untrained) × 6 (position: 1–6) mixed-factorial ANOVA performed on the mean recognition RTs at each position revealed significant main effects of sequence (F(1,21) = 8.41, p < 0.01) and position (F(5,105) = 186.84, p < 0.0001). Interactions between site and sequence (F(2,21) = 8.47, p < 0.01), between site and position (F(10,105) = 2.74, p < 0.01), and between site, sequence, and position (F(10,105) = 2.70, p < 0.01) were all significant. No other main effects or interactions were significant. Because positions 3–6 were predictable from the preceding targets (i.e., for trained sequences) and positions 1–2 were not predictable, position 3 is the first trial on which priming of the trained sequence can be detected as faster latency of response (Shanks et al., 2006). Figure 4b shows the results of the priming score calculated by subtracting the mean RTs to trained sequences from untrained sequences for positions 3–6; these values were averaged across positions 3 and 4 and across positions 5 and 6 (Shanks et al., 2006). Repeated measures t tests performed against chance (0) revealed significant priming in the cTBS M1HAND condition for positions 3–4 (24 ms) and 5–6 (27 ms) (t(7) = 5.62 and 3.50, respectively, p values <0.01), whereas priming was absent in both collapsed locations for cTBS AG and control conditions (p values >0.05).
In summary, both the cued-generation task and recognition test were in agreement in revealing evidence of sequence knowledge that was available to conscious awareness in the cTBS AG (and control) condition but implicit knowledge in the cTBS M1 condition. These data also indicate that, even though concurrent tasks can limit the availability of conscious knowledge (Shanks and Channon, 2002), the vigilance task did not preclude the acquisition of above-baseline knowledge and conscious awareness in the cTBS AG and control condition of experiment 1.
Experiment 2
Participants in all three between-subject conditions achieved >95% accuracy on the vigilance task, which indicates that participants were able to attend consistently to the sequence of target stimuli and could reliably discriminate LDTs from standard targets using covert reorienting of visuospatial attention. Mean end times in the cTBS AG, cTBS M1HAND, and control conditions were 39 min 23 s, 38 min 53 s, and 38 min 7 s, respectively.
Effects of cTBS over M1 and AG on posttraining direct tests of learning and awareness
Subjective measures of sequence knowledge.
Learning on the perceptual SRT task did not lead to accurate sequence knowledge above the subjective threshold: participants exhibited little or no metaknowledge about the target sequence on the awareness questionnaire in the cTBS M1HAND (mean ± SE, 1.5 ± 0.03), cTBS AG (mean ± SE, 1.3 ± 0.03), and control (mean ± SE, 1.1 ± 0.03) conditions, and the level of awareness was equivalent across conditions (Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA, χ2(2) = 0.51, p = 0.78). On the cued-generation task, participants were again more confident in their ability to generate a novel sequence on the exclusion test (mean ± SE, 51.1 ± 4.45) than generate the trained sequence on the inclusion test (mean ± SE, 27.4 ± 4.28) (F(1,21) = 15.47, p = 0.001), and this was consistent across between-subject conditions (F(2,21) = 1.17, p = 0.33).
Cued-generation task.
Mean start times in the cTBS AG, cTBS M1HAND, and control conditions were 47 min 33 s, 48 min 17 s, and 46 min 39 s, respectively. Figure 5a shows the mean proportions of trained SOCs generated under each site condition for the inclusion and exclusion tests. A 3 (site: cTBS M1HAND, cTBS AG, control) × 2 (test: inclusion, exclusion) × 2 (sequence: trained, baseline) mixed-factorial ANOVA performed on the mean proportions of second-order triplets generated revealed a significant main effect of test (F(1,21) = 46.24, p < 0.001) and significant interactions between site and sequence (F(2,21) = 6.84, p < 0.01), between test and sequence (F(1,21) = 17.01, p < 0.001), and between site, test, and sequence (F(2,21) = 7.07, p < 0.01). In the perceptual cTBS AG condition, although there was a significant difference in the proportion of trained triplets generated on the inclusion relative to exclusion test (F(1,21) = 7.62, p < 0.01), this difference was undermined by the failure to exceed baseline performance on the inclusion [0.41 (trained) vs 0.48 (baseline)] and exclusion [0.33 (trained) vs 0.38 (baseline)] tests (F(1,21) = 8.98, p < 0.01 and F(1,21) = 2.38, p = 0.14, respectively). In the cTBS M1HAND condition, conscious control when responding with trained sequences (i.e., I > E) (F(1,21) = 13.09, p < 0.01) was accompanied by above-baseline performance on the inclusion test but not on the exclusion test (F(1,21) = 8.98, p < 0.01 and F(1,21) = 1.42, p = 0.25, respectively). Participants in the control condition were also able to exert conscious control when responding with the trained sequence (i.e., I trained > E trained) (F(1,21) = 28.45, p < 0.0001), and performance was above and equivalent to baseline on the inclusion and exclusion tests (F(1,21) = 35.93, p < 0.0001 and F(1,21) = 1.59, p = 0.22, respectively). Collectively, these results indicate that sequence knowledge in the perceptual cTBS AG condition was absent [I (I trained < B) > E (E trained = B)], whereas knowledge was available to support conscious generation of the trained sequence in the cTBS M1HAND and control conditions (I trained > E trained; I trained > B; E trained = B). All planned comparisons were significant for Bonferroni–Holm corrected p values.
Recognition confidence ratings.
Mean start times in the cTBS AG, cTBS M1HAND, and control conditions were 61 min 54 s, 60 min 66 s, and 59 min 38 s, respectively. Mean recognition confidence ratings to trained and untrained sequences recognition for each site of stimulation condition are shown in Figure 5b. A 3 (site: M1HAND, AG, control) × 2 (sequence: trained, untrained) mixed-factorial ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between site and sequence (F(2,21) = 3.08, p < 0.05). Importantly, planned comparisons revealed that participants in the control condition could discriminate between trained and untrained sequences (F(1,21) = 6.07, p < 0.05), whereas participants in the AG and M1HAND conditions were at chance (F(1,21) = 0.44, p = 0.51 and F(1,21) = 2.05, p = 0.17, respectively). Hence, the results from the recognition test revealed that perceptual learning led to explicit knowledge in the control condition. Although results from the cued-generation task and recognition test were not in agreement in the cTBS M1HAND condition, it is important to note that functional dissociations between subjective and objective measure of learning and awareness have been reported in other studies [e.g., forced-choice questionnaire vs free-generation and recognition (Destrebecqz and Cleeremans, 2003)] and are variously argued to reveal gradations in the awareness with which sequence knowledge is held (Norman et al., 2006) or differences in the nature of knowledge applied when responding on different measures (Dienes et al., 1995). None of these interpretations, however, is at variance with the notion that explicit knowledge on the cued-generation task in the cTBS M1HAND condition provides evidence of conscious awareness.
Discussion
We investigated the cortical substrate underlying manual and perceptual probabilistic sequence learning by administering cTBS over M1 and the right AG. Four novel results were obtained: (1) cTBS over M1 and AG led to a functional-temporal dissociation in manual sequence learning: learning was disrupted across 75% trials in the cTBS M1 condition, whereas in the cTBS AG condition, disruption was confined to a midpoint during training; (2) cTBS over M1 modulated awareness associated with manual sequence learning: posttraining direct tests revealed that sequence knowledge was implicit in the cTBS M1 condition, whereas sequence knowledge was explicit in the cTBS AG and control conditions; (3) covert reorienting of visuospatial attention to visual stimuli supported probabilistic sequence learning; and (4) perceptual sequence learning was abolished by cTBS administered over the right AG.
Functional-temporal dissociation between M1 and AG during manual sequence learning
cTBS over M1 led to interference with learning for at least nine training blocks (23 min 52 s), whereas the effect of cTBS over the AG led to loss of sensitivity to improbable trials that peaked at a midpoint during training. Interestingly, the chronometry of the effect in the M1 condition corresponds with the timing of early activation associated with consolidation mechanisms implicated in manual skill and sequence learning (Muellbacher et al., 2002; Seidler et al., 2005), and is broadly in line with the duration of the suppressive effect of cTBS over M1 on motor-evoked potentials (20 s cTBS/20 min effect) (Huang et al., 2005; Reis al., 2008). TBS-induced functional effects after stimulation over the parietal cortex have thus far been associated with tonic, rather than phasic, effects lasting up to 30 min (Nyffeler et al., 2008).
Importantly, given that the SRT task error rate was comparable across conditions, the effects were specific to learning and not the result of impairment with response execution or a speed–accuracy tradeoff and are likely to have involved disruption of effector-independent and effector-dependent learning (Bapi et al., 2000). Results in the cTBS M1 condition are thus consistent with evidence showing that procedural aspects of learning on the manual SRT task exhibit relative insensitivity to posttraining TMS over M1 (Goedert and Willingham, 2002; Press et al., 2005). Disentangling learning from performance is important because changes in motor performance alone can lead to changes in the activity of neural areas implicated in learning. Compensatory mechanisms involving the recruitment of other parts of the motor system could, however, have masked interference with response execution (Lee et al., 2003). Indeed, coactivation and/or inhibition of topographically and functionally related regions, such as the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), supplementary motor area (SMA), cingulate motor cortex, thalamus, putamen, and contralateral M1 (Chouinard et al., 2003; Bestmann et al., 2004), are possible because TMS induces effects proximal and distal to the site of stimulation (Hallett, 2000).
Although subregions such as PMd do not have a readily identifiable casual role in manual sequence learning beyond a contribution to the selection of movements according to learned arbitrary rules (Thoenissen et al., 2002; Perez et al., 2007), sequence learning is routinely associated with coherent preferential activation of cortico-cerebellar and cortico-striatal networks in early and late phases (and delayed recall) of manual learning, respectively (Doyon and Ungerleider, 2002; Doyon et al., 2003). Subregions that support early motor skill learning exhibit a temporal gradient in activation: a decrease in cerebellar activation with learning is accompanied by an increase in activation of the basal ganglia, M1, and the SMA during later stages (Hazeltine et al., 1997; Grafton et al., 1998b). Interestingly, the production, rather than learning, of sequences is identified with the SMA (Perez et al., 2007; Schwarb and Schumacher, 2009), whereas M1 has been implicated in procedural consolidation (Muellbacher et al., 2002), whereby a learned skill becomes less susceptible to interference (Robertson et al., 2004). Given this role, it was important to assess the implications of cTBS-induced interference on the nature of newly acquired sequence knowledge using the declarative tests.
Motor-response-dependent contribution of M1 to the state of awareness associated with sequence knowledge
Learning in the cTBS M1 condition was characterized by a lack of awareness of the learning process and its contents, thereby meeting the conservative criterion for implicit knowledge (Cleeremans, 1993). Participants failed to detect the sequence on the questionnaire but generated comparable, above-baseline, trained sequence elements on the cued-generation tests and two elements primed responses on the recognition test without recognition. In contrast, performance in the control and cTBS AG conditions was consistent with explicit learning: conscious generation and recognition of the trained sequence accompanied awareness on the questionnaire. M1 thus has a causal role in developing experience-dependent functional attributes relevant to conscious sequence knowledge, and this role appears to be motor response dependent; in experiment 2, there was a null effect of cTBS over M1 on the state of awareness associated with perceptual sequence knowledge, albeit after a longer duration of cTBS over M1 (30 vs 20 s).
Our results are in agreement with TMS studies that have documented an effector-specific role for the M1 in manual sequence learning (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994, 1996). Previous studies indicate that M1 is involved with the representation of parameters beyond movement dynamics and kinematics, such as serial order or movement context (Grafton et al., 1998b; Carpenter et al., 1999; Matsuzaka et al., 2007). Multiple additional sources of information contribute to manual sequence learning (Verwey and Wright, 2004), such as coding for the movement or goal of the movement (Grafton et al., 1998a). Explicit motor response information is hypothesized to involve the integration and organization of action-effect codes into an ordered plan of actions (Hazeltine, 2002; Tubau et al., 2007). Importantly, learning-related changes in the motor cortices involve modulation from prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) in forming abstract goal and/or response-based representations (Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008). Such abstract representations imply a level of global access in which information can be deployed flexibly in response to varying contextual demands, which is, arguably, a fundamental feature of consciousness (Baars, 2002). Hence, the modulation in awareness was arguably attributable to a loss of, or impairment with, effector and/or response-specific information that had an impact on the formation of such representations and on perceptual-motor integration processes involved with developing knowledge that can be deployed flexibly.
The angular gyrus had a critical role in perceptual but not manual sequence learning
In the perceptual cTBS M1 and control conditions, rapid—occurring within 100 ms of a predictive cue (Lambert, 2003)—and sustained covert reorienting of visuospatial attention rather than action supported probabilistic sequence learning. Perceptual knowledge was expressed through the conscious generation and recognition of the trained sequence. These data serve as a counterpoint to accounts in which motor responses are a condition for sequence learning (Deroost and Soetens, 2006) but are consistent with the view that perceptual learning involves conscious access (Knee et al., 2007) and elaborate on previous work by demonstrating that perceptual learning can proceed without an intentional orientation during training. Importantly, perceptual learning was abolished by cTBS over the AG: there was no evidence of sequence knowledge on the cued-generation and recognition tests. In contrast, performance on the manual cued-generation and recognition test was consistent with explicit knowledge. Involvement of the right AG in sequence learning was thus sensitive to the mode of response.
Failure to detect learning in the perceptual cTBS AG condition did not emerge because perception of visual stimuli was impaired; performance on the vigilance task was at ceiling in all conditions. This is an important consideration because TMS administered over the right posterior parietal cortex can interfere with spatial orienting and attentional processes and induce neglect-like behavior (Hilgetag et al., 2001; Rosenthal et al., 2006; Rounis et al., 2007). Although the right AG has a particular role in the reorienting of spatial attention (Chambers et al., 2004), visuospatial orienting of attention depends on a network of frontal and parietal areas in the right hemisphere that extends to bilateral hemispaces (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Husain and Nachev, 2007). Because focal lesions within this network lead to discrete functional deficits, it will be important to examine the effect that disruption has on perceptual learning. For example, the lateral prefrontal cortex is instrumental in orienting attention, particularly in relation to the selection of task-relevant information (Brass and von Cramon, 2004), as part of an anterior striatum basal ganglia loop involved in visuospatial representation (Hikosaka et al., 2002), whereas neurons within the lateral intraparietal area in nonhuman primates have been shown to integrate probabilistic evidence regarding spatial positions (Yang and Shadlen, 2007).
Although the foci, and lateralization, of activation within the parietal cortex during manual sequence learning vary across functional MRI studies (Hazeltine et al., 1997; Grafton et al., 1998b; Willingham et al., 2002; Schendan et al., 2003; Bischoff-Grethe et al., 2004; Olson et al., 2006), activation within the parietal cortex is reported consistently. Manual learning might also be predicted to be susceptible to parietal stimulation on the grounds that changes in ipsilateral M1 are associated with TMS over the PPC (Koch et al., 2007). Nonetheless, our enhanced direct and indirect test protocol revealed comparable manual learning between the cTBS AG and control conditions. Hence, the contribution of the right AG to sequence learning per se did not operate independently of response mode. Previous investigations have similarly reported a null effect of TMS over parietal cortex on manual sequence learning (Robertson et al., 2001), and intact learning on the manual SRT task has been reported in patients with lesions involving the right parietal lobe (Berryhill et al., 2008).
Activation within the right IPL on the manual SRT task thus appears to be a consequence of more generalized processes involving the covert allocation and redirection of attention rather than sequence learning per se. Interestingly, deficits in spatiotemporal organization on tasks involving the predictive remapping of sequential movements occur in patients with ideomotor limb apraxia resulting from lesions within the left parietal cortex (Harrington and Haaland, 1992; Rushworth et al., 1997). Although activation within left inferior parietal cortex during the manual SRT task is associated with changes in performance rather than sequence learning per se (Willingham et al., 2002), disruption of the left parietal lobe contribution to motor attention might influence manual sequence learning (Rushworth et al., 2001). In particular, activation within the left supramarginal gyrus is associated with the covert preparation of movements, which, in the manual SRT task, is likely to be related to constant redirections of motor attention (Rushworth et al., 2003).
Commonalities and differences between motor and perceptual sequence learning
Comparable training parameters were implemented across the manual and perceptual SRT tasks, but differences along dimensions such as stimulus–response (S-R) compatibility are likely to influence learning. Lower relative S–R compatibility in the manual—attributable to greater response selection demands (Sternberg, 1969)—relative to the perceptual SRT task is likely to facilitate sequence learning (Koch and Hoffman, 2000). Sensitivity to changes in S–R compatibility has been observed within the PPC, including the IPL, left SMA, premotor cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex (Nee et al., 2007). It will be important to explore whether there are any differences between manual and perceptual learning in the effect of cTBS on the superior parietal cortex because this area, along with the PMd and dorsal prefrontal cortex, mediates spatial response selection (Dassonville et al., 2001; Jiang and Kanwisher, 2003; Schumacher et al., 2007) and the formation of spatial cue-to-response associations (Wise et al., 1997) and is consistently activated on the manual SRT task (Grafton et al., 2002; Olson et al., 2006).
Conclusion
The present results point to a functional-temporal dissociation between M1 and AG during manual sequence learning that had direct implications for the conscious accessibility of sequence knowledge. Training of covert attention to spatial locations led to probabilistic sequence learning, which was dependent on intact function within the IPL. Differences between the manual and perceptual SRT tasks along dimensions such as response-to-stimulus learning and integrated spatial/stimulus–response-based information were sufficient to modulate the contributions of M1 and AG to sequence learning. We conclude that the AG had a critical role in perceptual sequence learning but disruption of the AG does not affect the conscious expression of sequence knowledge in manual sequence learning, whereas M1 had a causal role in developing experience-dependent functional attributes relevant to conscious sequence knowledge on manual but not perceptual sequence learning.
Footnotes
This research was supported by a grant from the Wellcome Trust. We thank two anonymous reviewers for thoughtful comments on a previous version of this manuscript.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Clive Rosenthal, Department of Clinical Neurology, University of Oxford, Level 6, West Wing, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK. clive.rosenthal{at}clneuro.ox.ac.uk
This article is freely available online through the J Neurosci Open Choice option.