Skip to main content

Umbrella menu

  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

User menu

  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Spatially Global Representations in Human Primary Visual Cortex during Working Memory Maintenance

Edward F. Ester, John T. Serences and Edward Awh
Journal of Neuroscience 2 December 2009, 29 (48) 15258-15265; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4388-09.2009
Edward F. Ester
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
John T. Serences
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Edward Awh
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Additional Files
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    WM task. On each trial of the task, observers were shown a sample grating in the upper left or right visual field for 6 s. A subsequent change in the color of the fixation point (to red or green; shown here as white) informed observers whether they should remember the sample orientation (memory trials) or simply wait for the next trial to begin (no-memory trials). On memory trials, the cue was followed by a 14 s delay period and the presentation of a 1 s test grating in the same spatial location as the sample. Observers made a two-alternative, forced-choice response indicating whether the sample and test gratings shared the same orientation. On no-memory trials, the 14 s delay period was followed by an additional 1 s blank interval (i.e., no test grating was shown and no response was required). Trials were separated by a 2 s intertrial interval (ITI).

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Multivoxel pattern analysis of WM data. The results of a multivoxel pattern analysis using the 60 most spatially selective voxels in each V1 ROI during a time period extending from 6 to 16 s after the offset of the sample stimulus. The horizontal line at 0.5 denotes chance classification accuracy. ROIs both contralateral and ipsilateral to the sample stimulus discriminated its orientation but only when active WM maintenance was required. Qualitatively similar results were obtained across a range of pattern sizes (50 and 75 voxels) (supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) and also when a linear discriminant algorithm was used to perform classification. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Event-related analysis of WM data. The mean response of the 60 most spatially selective voxels in V1 (collapsed across hemispheres) is shown as a function of memory condition and ROI. Data are collapsed across stimulus orientation (i.e., 45° vs 135° trials) because no differences in mean response amplitude attributable to this factor were observed. Dashed vertical lines at 0 and 22 s denote the onset of the sample and test stimuli, respectively. Shaded regions represent ±1 SEM.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Global orientation-selective patterns are similar during PM and WM maintenance. A classification algorithm was trained to recognize orientation-selective patterns using the 75 most spatially selective voxels from each V1 ROI during PM (both left and right visual field trials were included in this analysis; top panels depict only RVF trials merely for illustrative purposes). As illustrated in the top two panels, this analysis was restricted to conditions in which the sample stimulus appeared in the same cortical ROI during PM and WM maintenance (for an identical analysis that focuses on conditions in which the sample stimulus appears in different spatial locations during each task, see supplemental Fig. 7, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). This algorithm was then used to infer the orientation of the sample stimulus during each trial of the WM task. The horizontal line at 0.5 denotes chance classification accuracy. Above-chance classification accuracy was observed in ROIs both contralateral and ipsilateral to the sample stimulus, indicating that orientation-selective patterns are similar during PM and WM maintenance. As in Figure 2, we failed to observe sustained above-chance classification accuracy on no-memory trials, indicating that above-chance classification of data from WM trials is not attributable to a lingering sensory effect. Qualitatively similar results were obtained when this analysis was applied to the 65 most spatially selective voxels in each ROI. Error bars represent ±1 SEM.

Tables

  • Figures
  • Additional Files
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Mean ± SEM discrimination thresholds and accuracy for 45° and 135° trials during PM and WM maintenance

    PM taskWM task
    45° trials135° trials45° trials135° trials
    Threshold18.53 ± 1.4918.40 ± 1.639.59 ± 1.269.89 ± 0.94
    Accuracy0.74 ± 0.010.72 ± 0.020.74 ± 0.020.74 ± 0.01

Additional Files

  • Figures
  • Tables
  • Supplemental Data

    Files in this Data Supplement:

    • supplemental material - Supplemental Material
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 29 (48)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 29, Issue 48
2 Dec 2009
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Spatially Global Representations in Human Primary Visual Cortex during Working Memory Maintenance
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Spatially Global Representations in Human Primary Visual Cortex during Working Memory Maintenance
Edward F. Ester, John T. Serences, Edward Awh
Journal of Neuroscience 2 December 2009, 29 (48) 15258-15265; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4388-09.2009

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Spatially Global Representations in Human Primary Visual Cortex during Working Memory Maintenance
Edward F. Ester, John T. Serences, Edward Awh
Journal of Neuroscience 2 December 2009, 29 (48) 15258-15265; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4388-09.2009
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Influence of Reward on Corticospinal Excitability during Movement Preparation
  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2021 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.