Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

The Role of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis in Learning to Fear

Anna K. Radke
Journal of Neuroscience 9 December 2009, 29 (49) 15351-15352; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5194-09.2009
Anna K. Radke
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Determining how the brain regulates fear and anxiety is critical to developing appropriate treatments for the wide range of human anxiety disorders. The extended amygdala is a macrostructure in the forebrain that includes three key players in fear and anxiety-like behaviors: the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA), central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA), and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST). The BLA sends excitatory projections to both CeA and BNST, which in turn project to brainstem structures involved in the expression of fearful behaviors. In Pavlovian fear conditioning, animals learn to associate a previously neutral conditioned stimulus (CS) with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) (e.g., shock). Lesion or temporary inactivation of either the BLA or the CeA, but not the BNST, before conditioning disrupts the expression of fear-related behaviors such as potentiation of the startle reflex, freezing, and increased autonomic responses during a recall test (Walker et al., 2003).

Although the BNST does not seem to be involved in learning to fear an explicit stimulus (e.g., a tone or a light presented only in the presence of the aversive stimulus), it is involved in learning to fear more general, long-lasting cues (Walker et al., 2003; Davis et al., 2009). For example, disruption of the BNST impairs learning to fear the context in which shock was presented (Sullivan et al., 2004). In addition, Walker and Davis (2008) have recently shown that rats can be trained to fear an 8 min auditory CS for its full duration, but that fear to the late, sustained component of CS presentation is blocked by inactivation of the BNST. Based on such experiments, it has been suggested that the BNST preferentially responds to diffuse long-duration stimuli resulting in anxiety or “sustained” fear (Walker and Davis, 2008; Davis et al., 2009).

In a recent study in The Journal of Neuroscience, Duvarci et al. (2009) sought to further define the role of the BNST in fear and anxiety behaviors by examining whether this structure contributes to fearful learning about a neutral conditioned stimulus that is never directly paired with an aversive stimulus (CS−). To this end, Duvarci et al. (2009) trained Lewis rats in a differential auditory fear conditioning paradigm in which rats were first presented with two auditory stimuli, one that was paired with shock (CS+) and another that was not (CS−). Testing for contextual fear memory took place in the same chamber as training (context A). Further testing for fear of the auditory stimuli was done in a second context (context B). During both tests, time spent freezing was used as the measure of learning.

As would be expected in this type of experiment, freezing was consistently observed both in context A and in the presence of the CS+, indicating that animals had learned to fear both these stimuli. Interestingly, the animals showed a considerable amount of variability in their responses to the CS−. Although it was never paired with shock, some animals learned to fear the CS− as much as the CS+. These same animals also showed more context conditioning and increased anxiety on the elevated plus maze. To investigate the role of the BNST in these behaviors, Duvarci et al. (2009) performed the same experiments in a group of rats with bilateral BNST lesions. When compared to animals with sham BNST lesions, lesion animals demonstrated reduced freezing to context A but normal levels of freezing when the CS+ was presented, consistent with previously published experiments (Walker et al., 2003; Sullivan et al., 2004). Additionally, virtually none of the BNST lesion animals learned to fear the CS−, suggesting that the BNST is involved in learning this inappropriate behavior.

What do these results tell us about the role of the BNST in auditory cue fear conditioning? Duvarci et al. (2009) suggest that, in the subset of animals that learned to fear the CS−, increased activity in the BNST during fear conditioning decreased cue selectivity in the amygdala, causing the animals to learn to fear both the CS+ and the CS−. Learning about the CS− therefore occurred because of stimulus generalization; that is, the animals did not discriminate between the CS+ and the CS− during the training and/or test. According to this account, neural projections from the BNST to the amygdala determine how selective the amygdala is during the learning process.

In addition to the conclusion presented by Duvarci et al. (2009), another interpretation of the data exists. Animals responding to the CS− may have done so because fearful learning about this stimulus occurred in the BNST. In this case, fear of the CS− was not due to the disruption of learning processes in the amygdala by the BNST, but instead to separate instances of learning about the CS+ and the CS− in the amygdala and BNST, respectively. Although it may seem counterintuitive that learning about the CS−, a short-duration cue, occurred in the BNST, this could be possible if the CS− was perceived as a diffuse, contextual cue. When one considers that the majority of the relatively limited experience the animals had with the CS− occurred in the context where shock was presented and that the shock and CS− were presented during the same training session, it is easy to see how this cue could become associated with the context itself.

This alternative interpretation is consistent with the results of Duvarci et al. (2009). Importantly, lesion of the BNST eliminated freezing to the CS−. In addition, the positive correlation between fear of the CS− and fear of the context implies that these behaviors are mediated by similar substrates. As discussed above, it has already been established that context conditioning is a BNST-mediated behavior (Walker et al., 2003; Sullivan et al., 2004). BNST-mediated CS− learning is also consistent with the hypothesis of Davis and colleagues that the BNST responds to generalized, nonspecific stimuli (Walker and Davis, 2008; Davis et al., 2009).

Regardless of which interpretation of the data proves true, the results of Duvarci et al. (2009) are relevant to the study of anxiety in rodents and humans. For example, another way to view the inappropriate fear to the CS− expressed by some animals is as a failure to inhibit fear during CS− presentation, or as a failure to learn that the CS− signaled a period of safety from shock. The results of Duvarci et al. (2009) are therefore relevant to literature on conditioning of safety signals (i.e., conditioned inhibition) (Rescorla, 1969). Learning to view a cue as a safety signal is an active process that is mediated by suppression of neural activity in the lateral amygdala (Collins and Paré, 2000; Rogan et al., 2005). The fact that BNST lesions correct the failure to learn in CS− responders could be interpreted to mean that BNST activity interferes with this learning process. Because the current study used only five training trials, it is impossible to know whether all of the animals would have eventually learned to inhibit fear to the CS−. Repeating this experiment with more trials would reveal whether BNST activity is completely preventing or merely slowing conditioning of the CS− as a safety signal.

Because clinical anxiety is often thought of as an inability to appropriately inhibit fear, and because interventions that facilitate the ability of patients to inhibit fear offer an effective strategy for the treatment of anxiety (Davis et al., 2006), the findings of Duvarci et al. (2009) might also be important for understanding the origins of anxiety in humans. Increased freezing to the CS− in anxious rats offers a striking parallel to the finding that posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) patients exhibit increased fear to the context and to a neutral CS− (Grillon and Morgan, 1999). A similar pattern has also recently been described for patients with panic disorder (Lissek et al., 2009). The individual differences in anxiety behavior in the inbred strain of Lewis rats described by Duvarci et al. (2009) may therefore prove to be a useful model for studying the neural substrates of PTSD and panic disorder. Finally, it is clear that future treatments targeting the BNST may be particularly helpful in the treatment of human anxiety disorders.

In summary, the differential fear conditioning paradigm used by Duvarci et al. (2009) is poised to offer fresh insights about the role of the BNST in inappropriate learning about a neutral CS−. Duvarci et al. (2009) propose that the BNST interferes with the ability of the amygdala to discriminate between the CS+ and CS−. The alternative conclusion presented here hypothesizes that the CS− is perceived as a retrieval cue for the context in which shock occurred and the BNST therefore directly mediates its association with shock presentation. Careful testing should be done to discriminate between these two hypotheses, to shed light on a behavior that may be a hallmark of some human anxiety disorders.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short, critical reviews of recent papers in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

  • This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant T32-HD007151. I thank Dr. Jonathan Gewirtz for insightful discussion of this article.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Anna K. Radke, Department of Psychology, N-218 Elliott Hall, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455. aradke{at}umn.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Collins DR,
    2. Paré D
    (2000) Differential fear conditioning induces reciprocal changes in the sensory responses of lateral amygdala neurons to the CS+ and CS−. Learn Mem 7:97–103.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Davis M,
    2. Ressler K,
    3. Rothbaum BO,
    4. Richardson R
    (2006) Effects of D-cycloserine on extinction: Translation from preclinical to clinical work. Biol Psychiat 60:369–375.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Davis M,
    2. Walker DL,
    3. Miles L,
    4. Grillon C
    (2009) Phasic vs. sustained fear in rats and humans: Role of the extended amygdala in fear vs. anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology, Advance online publication. Retrieved September 28, 2009. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.109.
  4. ↵
    1. Duvarci S,
    2. Bauer EP,
    3. Paré D
    (2009) The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis mediates inter-individual variations in anxiety and fear. J Neurosci 29:10357–10361.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Grillon C,
    2. Morgan CA 3rd.
    (1999) Fear-potentiated startle conditioning to explicit and contextual cues in gulf war veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 108:134–142.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Lissek S,
    2. Rabin SJ,
    3. McDowell DJ,
    4. Dvir S,
    5. Bradford DE,
    6. Geraci M,
    7. Pine DS,
    8. Grillon C
    (2009) Impaired discriminative fear-conditioning resulting from elevated fear responding to learned safety cues among individuals with panic disorder. Behav Res Ther 47:111–118.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Rescorla RA
    (1969) Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychol Bull 72:77–94.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    1. Rogan MT,
    2. Leon KS,
    3. Perez DL,
    4. Kandel ER
    (2005) Distinct neural signatures for safety and danger in the amygdala and striatum of the mouse. Neuron 46:309–320.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Sullivan GM,
    2. Apergis J,
    3. Bush DE,
    4. Johnson LR,
    5. Hou M,
    6. Ledoux JE
    (2004) Lesions in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis disrupt corticosterone and freezing responses elicited by a contextual but not by a specific cue-conditioned fear stimulus. Neuroscience 128:7–14.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Walker DL,
    2. Davis M
    (2008) Role of the extended amygdala in short-duration versus sustained fear: a tribute to Dr. Lennart Heimer. Brain Struct Funct 213:29–42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Walker DL,
    2. Toufexis DJ,
    3. Davis M
    (2003) Role of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis versus the amygdala in fear, stress, and anxiety. Eur J Pharmacol 463:199–216.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 29 (49)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 29, Issue 49
9 Dec 2009
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The Role of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis in Learning to Fear
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
The Role of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis in Learning to Fear
Anna K. Radke
Journal of Neuroscience 9 December 2009, 29 (49) 15351-15352; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5194-09.2009

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
The Role of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis in Learning to Fear
Anna K. Radke
Journal of Neuroscience 9 December 2009, 29 (49) 15351-15352; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5194-09.2009
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Adult-Born Granule Cells Contribute to Dentate Gyrus Circuit Reorganization after Traumatic Brain Injury
  • The Oscillatory Nature of Movement Initiation
  • Lateral Preoptic Hypothalamus: A Window to Understanding Insomnia
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.