Abstract
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition with multiple causes, comorbid conditions, and a wide range in the type and severity of symptoms expressed by different individuals. This makes the neuroanatomy of autism inherently difficult to describe. Here, we demonstrate how a multiparameter classification approach can be used to characterize the complex and subtle structural pattern of gray matter anatomy implicated in adults with ASD, and to reveal spatially distributed patterns of discriminating regions for a variety of parameters describing brain anatomy. A set of five morphological parameters including volumetric and geometric features at each spatial location on the cortical surface was used to discriminate between people with ASD and controls using a support vector machine (SVM) analytic approach, and to find a spatially distributed pattern of regions with maximal classification weights. On the basis of these patterns, SVM was able to identify individuals with ASD at a sensitivity and specificity of up to 90% and 80%, respectively. However, the ability of individual cortical features to discriminate between groups was highly variable, and the discriminating patterns of regions varied across parameters. The classification was specific to ASD rather than neurodevelopmental conditions in general (e.g., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder). Our results confirm the hypothesis that the neuroanatomy of autism is truly multidimensional, and affects multiple and most likely independent cortical features. The spatial patterns detected using SVM may help further exploration of the specific genetic and neuropathological underpinnings of ASD, and provide new insights into the most likely multifactorial etiology of the condition.
Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a highly heterogeneous neurodevelopmental condition with multiple causes and courses, a wide range in symptom severity, and several associated comorbid disorders (Amaral et al., 2008). It has thus been suggested that ASD should be thought of as “the autisms” rather than a single autistic phenotype (Geschwind, 2007; Geschwind and Levitt, 2007). This makes the neuroanatomy of autism inherently difficult to describe.
Although several autistic “core” structures have repeatedly been highlighted by previous studies, such as abnormalities in frontal, parietal, and limbic regions, as well as the basal ganglia and the cerebellum [e.g., McAlonan et al. (2002), Waiter et al. (2004), McAlonan et al. (2005), and Rojas et al. (2006)], reports of region-specific differences in ASD are highly variable [for review, see Toal et al. (2005) and Amaral et al. (2008)]. Such variable findings may simply be explained by confounds such as clinical heterogeneity between studies, or analytical techniques. Alternatively, variability in findings may indicate that differences in brain anatomy in ASD are relatively subtle and spatially distributed, and are difficult to detect using mass-univariate (i.e., voxelwise) approaches. Last, given the multiple etiology of ASD, it is likely that its neuroanatomy is not confined to a single morphological parameter but affects multiple cortical features.
There is already evidence to suggest that several aspects of cerebral morphology are different in people with ASD—including both volumetric (i.e., cortical thickness, regional area) and geometric (i.e., cortical shape) features (Levitt et al., 2003; Nordahl et al., 2007); and that different morphological features may have different neuropathological and genetic underpinnings (Panizzon et al., 2009). For instance, cortical thickness is likely to reflect dendritic arborization (Huttenlocher, 1990), while cortical surface area has been linked to the number of minicolumns in the cortical layer (Rakic, 1988). Geometric features such as cortical folding pattern, on the other hand, may reflect an abnormal pattern of intrinsic as well as extrinsic connectivity (Van Essen, 1997). Thus, examining the relationship between such multiple cortical features could provide invaluable insights into the multifactorial etiology of ASD.
So far, individual aspects of the cortex are generally explored in isolation (i.e., in separate statistical models). Hence it is not yet known (1) whether they equally contribute to differentiating individuals with ASD from controls; and (2) what the relationship between such multiple cortical abnormalities in ASD is. In the present study, we therefore used a multiparameter classification approach using a support vector machine (SVM) [e.g., Mourão-Miranda et al. (2005), Davatzikos et al. (2008), and Klöppel et al. (2008)] to investigate brain anatomy in adults with ASD. Here, a variety of morphological features, including both volumetric and geometric parameters, were used simultaneously to classify control and ASD individuals, as well as a neurodevelopmental control group of individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). We aimed to demonstrate that the neuroanatomical patterns discriminating individuals with autism from controls are truly multidimensional, comprising multiple and most likely independent cortical features. If so, this may guide further exploration of the specific genetic and neuropathological underpinnings of ASD.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
Twenty control adults were recruited locally by advertisement and 20 adults with ASD were recruited through a clinical research program at the Maudsley Hospital/Institute of Psychiatry (London). All volunteers (see Table 1) gave informed consent (as approved by the Institute of Psychiatry and Bethlem and Maudsley Hospital Trust research ethics committee), and had a full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) >75 [WASI (Wechsler, 1999)]. All volunteers were right-handed males, and were between 20 and 68 years of age. None had a history of major psychiatric disorder or medical illness affecting brain function (e.g., psychosis or epilepsy). All participants underwent a psychiatric interview and physical examination. Blood tests were used to exclude biochemical, hematological, or chromosomal abnormalities (including fragile X syndrome). All participants with ASD were diagnosed with autism according to ICD 10 research criteria (World Health Organization, 1992) by a trained and qualified clinician in our Adult Autism Specialist Clinic at the South London and Maudsley Hospital. The initial clinical diagnosis was then confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [ADI-R (Lord et al., 1994)] whenever possible (n = 17); that is, where parents agreed to take part. In three out of the 20 included cases, ADI scores could not be obtained due to informants being unavailable. In these three cases, the clinical diagnosis was confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule [ADOS (Lord et al., 1989)]. Both ADI-R and ADOS scores were available for two participants. All cases had to reach the ADOS or ADI algorithm cutoffs in the three domains of impaired reciprocal social interaction, communication and repetitive behaviors and stereotyped patterns, but we did allow failure to reach cutoff in one of the domains (by one point). Sixteen ASD individuals did not have a delay in development of phrase speech at the age of 36 months (and so may be subtyped by some as having the autistic subtype of Asperger's syndrome); the remaining four individuals had a history of delayed phrase speech (and so may be subtyped by some as having the autistic subtype of high functioning autism). However, due to the small sample size it was not possible to reliably investigate putative differences between people with high functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome, and all subjects were analyzed in a combined group of individuals with ASD.
Furthermore, we recruited a group of 19 individuals with ADHD from the Adult ADHD services at the Maudsley Hospital, London. This group served as a neurodevelopmental control group and was matched to the ASD group in gender, age (mean ± SD = 31 ± 8.5), FSIQ (mean ± SD = 107 ± 16), and handedness. Of these, eight individuals fulfilled the criteria for ADHD combined type and 11 for ADHD inattentive type. The diagnosis of ADHD was made by a trained and qualified clinician based on a structured clinical interview according to DSM-IV criteria. ADHD symptoms were also assessed using Barkley Current and Childhood Symptoms Scales (Barkley, 2006) and the Wender-Utah Rating Scale [WURS-25 (Ward et al., 1993)].
MRI data acquisition.
MRI data were acquired using a 1.5 T GE Signa Neuro-optimized System (General Electric Medical Systems) fitted with 40 mT/m high-speed gradients at the Maudsley Hospital, London. Whole-brain spoiled gradient recalled acquisition in the steady-state T1-weighted series were collected in the coronal plane with repetition time = 13.8 ms, echo time = 2.8 ms, yielding 124 contiguous 1.5 mm2 axial slices of 256 × 192 voxels with an in-plane resolution of 1 mm2. Similar T1-weighted MRI scans were acquired for the ADHD group on a 1.5 T GE Signa Neuro-optimized System at the Maudsley Hospital, London, using repetition time = 10.72 ms, echo time = 4.86 ms, yielding 146 contiguous 1.1 mm2 axial slices. A quadrature birdcage head coil was used for radio-frequency transmission and reception. Foam padding and a forehead strap were used to limit head motion. All scans were visually inspected by a radiologist to assess (1) image contrast, (2) movement artifacts, and (3) the existence of clinical abnormalities. Scans displaying low image quality or clinical abnormalities were excluded from this study.
Image processing.
Cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation were performed with the Freesurfer image analysis suite, which is documented and freely available for download online (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). The technical details of these procedures are described in prior publications (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999, 2002, 2004a,b; Fischl and Dale, 2000; Ségonne et al., 2004; Jovicich et al., 2006). Briefly, this processing includes motion correction and averaging of multiple volumetric T1-weighted images (when more than one is available), removal of nonbrain tissue using a hybrid watershed/surface deformation procedure (Ségonne et al., 2004), automated Talairach transformation, segmentation of the subcortical white matter and deep gray matter volumetric structures (including hippocampus, amygdala, caudate, putamen, and ventricles) (Fischl et al., 2002, 2004b), intensity normalization, tessellation of the gray matter white matter boundary, automated topology correction (Fischl et al., 2001; Ségonne et al., 2007), and surface deformation following intensity gradients to optimally place the gray/white and gray/CSF borders at the location where the greatest shift in intensity defines the transition to the other tissue class (Dale et al., 1999). Once the cortical models are complete, a number of deformable procedures were performed for further data processing and analysis, including surface inflation (Dale et al., 1999), registration to a spherical atlas, which used individual cortical folding patterns to match cortical geometry across subjects (Fischl et al., 1999), parcellation of the cerebral cortex into units based on gyral and sulcal structure (Fischl et al., 2004a; Desikan et al., 2006), and creation of a variety of surface-based data, including maps of curvature and sulcal depth (see below). This method uses both intensity and continuity information from the entire three-dimensional MR volume in segmentation and deformation procedures to produce representations of cortical thickness, calculated as the closest distance from the gray/white boundary to the gray/CSF boundary at each vertex on the tessellated surface (Fischl and Dale, 2000). The maps are created using spatial intensity gradients across tissue classes and are therefore not simply reliant on absolute signal intensity. The maps produced are not restricted to the voxel resolution of the original data thus are capable of detecting submillimeter differences between groups. Procedures for the measurement of cortical thickness have been validated against histological analysis (Rosas et al., 2002) and manual measurements (Kuperberg et al., 2003; Salat et al., 2004). Freesurfer morphometric procedures have been demonstrated to show good test–retest reliability across scanner manufacturers and across field strengths (Han et al., 2006). All reconstructed surfaces were visually inspected for gross-anatomical topological defects. Brains that did not reconstruct or showed geometric inaccuracies were excluded from the study.
A set of five morphometric parameters per vertex were used as input to the multimodal classifier. Three parameters [1–3: average convexity or concavity, mean (radial) curvature, and metric distortion] accounted for geometric features at each cerebral vertex and two parameters (4, 5: cortical thickness and surface area, respectively) measured volumetric features.
(1) The average convexity or concavity (C) was used for quantifying the primary folding pattern of a surface. C captures large-scale geometric features and is insensitive to noise in the form of small wrinkles in a surface (Fischl et al., 1999). At each vertex, C indicates the depth/height above the average surface and measures sulcal depth or gyral height, respectively. Differences in sulcal depth have previously been investigated as one important aspect of the cerebral geometry implicated in ASD (Nordahl et al., 2007), and have been suggested to reflect abnormal pattern of cortical connectivity.
(2) Mean (radial) curvature was used to assess folding of the small secondary and tertiary folds in the surface. The selection of this feature was motivated by early findings of polymicrogyria (i.e., excessive number of small convolutions on the surface of the brain) in autism (Piven et al., 1990). These would not be captured by the average convexity measure reflecting large-scale geometric features only.
(3) Metric distortion (i.e., Jacobian) indicating the degree of cortical folding was calculated as degree of displacement and convolution of the cortical surface relative to the average template (Fischl et al., 1999; Wisco et al., 2007). While sulcal depth and radial curvature measure specific aspects of the cortical geometry, metric distortion is a wider measure of the overall degree of cortical folding, and thus captures geometric distortions otherwise not specified. There is a large body of evidence to suggest that individuals with ASD display abnormal patterns of cortical gyrification reflecting cerebral development and connectivity [e.g., Levitt et al. (2003) and Hardan et al. (2004)].
(4) Cortical thickness (see above) and (5) pial area (i.e., the area of a vertex on the gray matter surface, calculated as the average of the area of the triangles touching that vertex) were used to quantify volumetric differences. Although these two features are generally combined to measure regional brain volume, a recent study has shown that cortical thickness and surface are influenced by distinct genetic mechanism (Panizzon et al., 2009), and we hence added as separate features into the model. A summary of the set of parameters is displayed in Figure 1.
Group differences in intracranial volume, total brain volume, and gray matter volume as estimated by FreeSurfer were assessed using t tests for independent samples before classification. There were no significant differences between groups in any of these parameters at a level of p < 0.05 (see Table 2). There were also no significant differences in total brain volume between controls and individuals with ADHD, nor between ASD and ADHD group on p < 0.05. Bartlett's test for homogeneity of variances was used to examine parameter variability across homolog regions in different hemispheres using a threshold of p < 0.0014 (corrected for multiple comparisons).
Classification and support vector machine.
A linear SVM was used to classify between patient and control group on the basis of their brain morphology. SVM has previously been applied to MRI data (Mourão-Miranda et al., 2005; Davatzikos et al., 2008; Klöppel et al., 2008), and a detailed description was given by Burges (1998) and Schoelkopf and Smola (2002). Briefly, SVM is a supervised multivariate classification method that treats each image as a point in a high dimensional space. If SVM is applied to images coming from different modalities, as in the current study, the number of dimensions equals the number of voxels/vertices per image multiplied by the number of modalities. Input data were then classified into two classes (e.g., individuals with ASD and controls) by identifying a separating hyperplane or decision boundary. The algorithm is initially trained on a subset of the data 〈x, c〉 to find a hyperplane that best separates the input space according to the class labels c (e.g., −1 for patients, +1 for controls). Here, x represents the input data (i.e., feature vector). The feature vector was generated by concatenating the five image modalities for each subject. Once the decision function is learned from the training data, it can be used to predict the class of a new test example. SVM (Vapnik, 1995) is a maximum margin classifier, which identifies the optimal hyperplane by finding the hyperplane with the maximum margin (i.e., maximal separation between classes). The margin is the distance from the separating hyperplane to the closest training examples. The training examples that lie on the margin are called support vectors. The hyperplane is defined by a weight vector and an offset. The weight vector is a linear combination of the support vectors and is normal to the hyperplane. The linear kernel SVM used in the present study allows direct extraction of the weight vector as an image (i.e., the SVM discrimination map). A parameter C, which controls the trade-off between having zero training errors and allowing misclassifications, was fixed at C = 1 for all cases (default value). The LIBSVM toolbox for Matlab was used to perform the classifications (http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/∼cjlin/libsvm/).
Discrimination maps.
The weight vector has the same dimension as the feature vector and is normal to the hyperplane. It can be thought of as a spatial representation of the decision boundary, and thus represents a map of the most discriminating regions. Here, the feature vector had n × m dimensions, where n equals the number of vertices and m denotes the number of modalities (i.e., five). Given two groups (ASD vs controls), with the labels +1 and −1, respectively, a positive value in the discrimination map (red/yellow color scale) indicates relatively higher parameter values in patients than in controls with respect to the hyperplane, and a negative weight (blue/cyan color scale) means relatively higher parameter values in controls than in patients with respect to the hyperplane. Because the classifier is multivariate by nature, the combination of all voxels as a whole is identified as a global spatial pattern by which the groups differ (i.e., the discriminating pattern). To enable visualization of the discriminating pattern for each image modality, the weight vector was cut into its constituent parts, which were then mapped back onto the average white matter surface. In the present study, we colored all voxels that have values higher than 30% of the maximum value of the discrimination map (Mourão-Miranda et al., 2005, 2006). This threshold, although ultimately arbitrary, eliminates noise components predominantly (<30%), thus enabling a better visualization of the most discriminating regions (31–100%).
Intraregional morphometric profiles.
To identify the relative contribution of specific parameters in discriminating between groups at different locations on the cortical surface, we displayed so called intraregional morphometric profiles. These profiles were derived by calculating the average weights of vertices within regions of interest (ROIs) for the five different parameters. ROIs were based on contiguous weight clusters from the overall discrimination maps. The choice of the ROIs, although ultimately arbitrary, was motivated by (1) the relevance of a region to the current literature [e.g., sulcal depth differences in intraparietal sulcus (Nordahl et al., 2007)], and (2) generally high parameter weights for a specific morphometric feature (e.g., high weights for cortical thickness in medial temporal sulcus). The ROI analysis aimed to illustrate that different regions can display distinct differences in one or more parameters, rather than displaying the same pattern of feature weights. As all weights were scaled, means and SDs are directly comparable across regions and parameters.
Cross-validation and permutation testing.
The performance of the classifier was validated using the commonly used leave-two-out cross-validation approach. This validation approach provides robust parameter estimates particularly for smaller samples. In each trial observations from all but one subject from each group were used to train the classifier. Subsequently, the class assignment of the test subjects was calculated during the test phase. This procedure was repeated S = 20 times (where S is the number of subjects per group), each time leaving observations from a different subject from each group out. The accuracy of the classifier was measured by the proportion of observations that were correctly classified into patient or control group. We also quantified the sensitivity and specificity of the classifier defined as Sensitivity = TP/(TP + FN) and Specificity = TN/(TN + FP), where TP is the number of true positives, i.e., the number of patient images correctly classified; TN is the number of true negatives, i.e., number of control images correctly classified; FP is the number of false positives, i.e., number of controls images classified as patients; and FN is the number of false negatives, i.e., number of patients images classified as controls.
Classifications were made first, by including all five morphological modalities into the feature vector, and, second, on the basis of each individual parameter. Different classifiers were trained for each hemisphere. This enabled us to assess the overall classification accuracies for individual parameters and hemispheres. Classifier performance was evaluated using basic receiver operating characteristics (ROC) graphs as well as permutation testing. Permutation testing can be used to evaluate the probability of getting specificity and sensitivity values higher than the ones obtained during the cross-validation procedure by chance. We permuted the labels 1000 times without replacement, each time randomly assigning patient and control labels to each image and repeated the cross-validation procedure. We then counted the number of times the specificity and sensitivity for the permuted labels were higher than the ones obtained for the real labels. Dividing this number by 1000 we derived a p value for the classification.
Last, the established classifier was used to predict group membership of individuals with ADHD. To achieve this, the classifier was first retrained using all ASD–control pairs to provide the best possible prediction model for discriminating the two groups. This trained classifier was then applied to the multiparameter data from the ADHD group and predictions for group membership for these subjects were derived.
To validate the binary classification of the subject groups on a quantitative level, and to identify the degree to which the classification is driven by autistic symptoms rather than confounds unrelated to autism, the test margin for each subject coming from the all included classifier was correlated with the level of symptom severity measured by the ADI subscales. A similar approach has previously been employed by Ecker et al. (2010).
Results
Overall classifier performance
Classification accuracies as well as sensitivity and specificity for each classifier are listed in Table 3. On the whole, strong hemispheric asymmetry was observed with regards to the overall classification accuracy. The left hemisphere provided consistently higher and above chance prediction accuracies across all morphometric parameters. Here, individuals with ASD were correctly assigned to the appropriate diagnostic category in 85.0% of all cases when all parameters were considered simultaneously (Fig. 2C). The sensitivity of the multiparameter classification in the left hemisphere was 90.0%; i.e., if a volunteer had a clinical diagnosis of ASD, the probability that this participant was correctly assigned to the ASD category was 0.9. The specificity was 80.0%—meaning that 80.0% of the controls subjects were correctly classified as controls. High classification accuracies were also obtained when individual morphological parameters were considered. Figure 2A shows the receiver operator characteristics or ROC graph for the six classifiers in the left hemisphere. In general, one point in ROC space is better than another if it is to the northwest (TP rate is high, FP rate is low, or both) with the point (0, 1) representing perfect classification. Classifiers appearing on the left-hand side of the ROC graph, near the x-axis, may be considered “conservative” (i.e., make positive classifications only with strong evidence but often have low TP rates), whereas classifiers on the upper right-hand side of the graph may be thought of as “liberal” (i.e., make positive classifications with weak evidence so they classify nearly all positives correctly but often have high FPs). The diagonal line y = x represents the strategy of randomly guessing a class. Best discrimination was obtained when cortical thickness measures were used to classify between groups with sensitivity and specificity values as high as 90.0%, followed by a classification on the basis of the metric distortion parameter (sensitivity 80.0%, specificity 80.0%). The average convexity, pial area, and mean curvature displayed accuracies in the range of 70.0–80.0%. The classification p value resulting from the permutation test was very low across, as well as within, modalities (<0.004). The probability of obtaining specificity and sensitivity values higher than the ones obtained during cross-validation procedure by chance is thus extremely low.
A very similar profile of parameter importance was observed in the right hemisphere—despite it having generally lower classification accuracies. Here, individuals with ASD were correctly assigned to the appropriate diagnostic category in 65.0% of all cases (sensitivity 60.0%, specificity 70.0%, p < 0.03) when all parameters were considered simultaneously (see Fig. 2B,D). As in the left hemisphere, cortical thickness as well as metric distortion displayed the best classification performance—although only cortical thickness measures reached statistical significance (p < 0.01). The discrepancy in overall classification accuracy between the two hemispheres was not due to differences in parameter variability, as there were no significant differences in parameter variance between homolog regions in different hemispheres (calculated across the whole sample) (see supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).
Thus, while all parameters provided statistically significant predictions in the left hemisphere, only the full model and cortical thickness displayed significant predictions in the right hemisphere. Highest classification accuracy (90.0%) might thus be obtained by using cortical thickness measures exclusively in the left hemispheres.
Correlation coefficients between ADI-R subscales and the test margin coming from the combined model are listed in Table 4. The test margin in the left hemisphere was positively correlated with the ADI scores for individuals with ASD in the social (r = 0.414, p < 0.05) and communication domain (r = 0.62, p < 0.01). Therefore, individuals with higher values on these ADI subdomains are located on the extreme right relative on the hyperplane while the individuals with a lower level of impairment are mostly located in close proximity to the hyperplane overall.
Classification of individuals with ADHD using the established ASD classifier
To establish the degree of clinical specificity of the discrimination algorithm to ASD, rather than to neurodevelopmental disorders in general, the established classifier was used to predict group membership of individuals with ADHD. To predict group membership, the model including all parameters was chosen as it provided best classification accuracy in the right hemisphere, and high accuracy in the left hemisphere. On the basis of the neuroanatomical information available for the left hemisphere, 15 out of the 19 individuals with ADHD were allocated to the control group (78.9%), and 4 individuals with ADHD were allocated to the ASD group (21%). Using the right hemisphere, the classifier allocated cases with ADHD with approximately equal frequencies (47% allocated to the ASD category; 52% allocated to the control category). The classification plots for individuals with ADHD are shown in Figure 3 for both hemispheres.
Discrimination maps of ASD-specific abnormalities
The spatial maps of brain regions identified as outlined above are shown in Figure 4 and a summary description can be found in Table 5. Each of these maps is a spatial representation of the SVM weight vector, and while they do not directly quantify the information content of each region, each map forms a spatially distributed pattern showing the relative contribution of each voxel to the decision function. Although “knock-out” techniques may be used to directly quantify the information content of particular regions, this approach may be hampered by substantial information redundancy across brain regions. Nonetheless, the spatial distribution of the weight vector does provide information about which brain regions contributed to classification and in this case is suggestive of a distributed pattern of relative deficit or excess in ASD with respect to controls. We emphasize that due to the multivariate character of SVM (i.e., it considers interregional correlations), each region in the discrimination maps should be interpreted in the context of the entire discriminating pattern and should not be considered in isolation. Further, individual regions may display high classification weights for several reasons, e.g., there is a large difference in volume between groups in that region, or the region is highly intercorrelated with other components of the network (i.e., pattern). This is of particular relevance to ASD, as individuals with the disorder most likely have abnormalities in the development of neural systems, in addition to differences in isolated regions. Thus, it is important to highlight that individual network components are not necessarily different between groups, but should be considered as constituent parts of a neuroanatomical network discriminating between groups.
As can be seen in this Figure 4, the five investigated parameters resulted in different spatially distributed patterns of regions with highest contribution to the discrimination. While cortical thickness provided best classification accuracy and maximum weight values, differences between groups were observed in both volumetric as well as geometric features.
Cortical thickness
In both hemispheres, the discriminative pattern for cortical thickness in ASD comprised regions in all four lobes of the cortex (Fig. 4A). Regional details are summarized in Table 5. The “excess pattern” (i.e., ASD > controls) comprised several temporal regions, including the right superior temporal sulcus [Brodmann area 22 (BA22)], medial and superior temporal gyrus (BA21/BA22), and the parahippocampal gyrus (BA36), fusiform gyrus (BA20), and entorhinal cortex. In addition, the excess pattern included the inferior/superior parietal lobe (BA39), BA18 of the occipital lobe, and the anterior and posterior cingulate gyrus.
While the excess pattern comprised predominantly occipitotemporal regions, the pattern displaying a relative thinning of the cortex in ASD versus controls (i.e., “deficit pattern”) included mainly frontal and parietal regions such as the middle frontal gyrus (BA46), the medial/superior frontal gyrus (BA10), and BA46 in the medial frontal gyrus. In addition, the deficit pattern contained the superior parietal cortex (BA40/7) and the anterior cingulate gyrus.
Surface area
Only a few surface-area (Fig. 4D) differences were observed in relatively small clusters with generally low weights. Details are summarized in Table 4.
Regional geometric characteristics
Apart from volumetric differences, features describing regional geometric characteristics also displayed high discrimination weights, which were summarized in Table 6. In both hemispheres, individuals with ASD displayed a pattern of relative increase in sulcal depth (Fig. 4B) in the intraparietal sulcus as well as in the superior frontal cortex. The discriminative pattern for cortical folding (Fig. 4C), as indicated by the metric distortion (i.e., Jacobian), included mainly bilateral parietal regions such as the inferior parietal lobe (BA39/40), and several regions of the right frontal lobe (e.g., supramarginal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, and orbitofrontal regions). In addition, the discriminative pattern in cortical folding comprised the precuneus.
Morphometric profiles
The discrimination maps show that different morphometric parameters elicited different spatial patterns of weights with some overlap between particular parameter pairs. The weights for individual parameters are thus not uniform across the cortex but vary from region to region (i.e., vertex to vertex). This can also be seen on the basis of the morphometric profile for individual regions, which are displayed in Figures 5D, 6D, 7D, and 8C and listed in Table 7. These profiles visualize the mean parameter weights for individual features within regions of interest.
In the intraparietal sulcus for instance, high discrimination weights were observed for differences in sulcal depth with lower weights for volumetric features such as cortical thickness or surface area (see Fig. 5). A similar profile was also seen in the inferior parietal lobe (BA39). Here, the weights for volumetric parameters such as cortical thickness or surface area were very low in comparison to the weights associated with the pattern of cortical folding (i.e., metric distortion) (see Fig. 6).
Other regions, however, displayed high weights for volumetric parameters exclusively. For instance, in the temporal sulcus (BA21), high weights were observed for cortical thickness exclusively in the light of low weights in all other parameters (see Fig. 8). There were also regions with equally high weights in volumetric as well as geometric features—e.g., the anterior cingulate gyrus. Here, cortical thickness as well as local cortical folding displayed high parameter weights.
In summary, we observed a spatially distributed pattern of regions that can be used to differentiate individuals with ASD from controls. Overall, highest weights were observed for measures of cortical thickness, followed by geometric features such as sulcal depth and metric distortion, which were predominantly observed in parietal regions. This suggests that both volumetric and geometric features play an important role in distinguishing between ASD and control group. Last we demonstrated that the parameter weights for individual cortical features were region dependent.
Discussion
Autism affects multiple aspects of the cerebral anatomy, which makes its neuroanatomical correlates inherently difficult to describe. Here, we used a multiparameter classification approach to characterize the complex and subtle gray matter differences in adults with ASD. SVM achieved good separation between groups, and revealed spatially distributed and largely non-overlapping patterns of regions with highest classification weights for each of five morphological features. Our results confirm that the neuroanatomy of ASD is truly multidimensional affecting multiple neural systems. The discriminating patterns detected using SVM may help further exploration of the genetic and neuropathological underpinnings of ASD.
There is good evidence to suggest that several aspects of cerebral morphology are implemented in ASD—including both volumetric and geometric features (Levitt et al., 2003; Nordahl et al., 2007). However, these are normally explored in isolation. Here, we aimed to establish a framework for multiparameter image classification to describe differences in gray matter neuroanatomy in autism in multiple dimensions, and to explore the predictive power of individual parameters for group membership. This was achieved using a multiparameter classifier incorporating volumetric and geometric features at each cerebral vertex. In the left hemisphere, SVM correctly classified 85.0% of all cases overall at a sensitivity and specificity as high as 90.0% and 80.0%, respectively, using all five morphological features. This level of sensitivity compares well with behaviorally guided diagnostic tools whose accuracies are on average ∼80%. Naturally, one would expect lower sensitivity values than the test used for defining the “autistic prototype” itself (i.e., ADI-R). Thus, if a classifier is trained on the basis of true positives identified by diagnostic tools, the maximal classification accuracy that could be reached is only as good as the measurements used to identify true positives.
The significant predictive value of pattern classification approaches may have potential clinical applications. Currently, ASD is diagnosed solely on the basis of behavioral criteria. The behavioral diagnosis is however often time consuming and can be problematic, particularly in adults. Also, different biological etiologies might result in the same behavioral phenotype [the “autisms” (Geschwind, 2007)], which is undetectable using behavioral measures alone. Thus, the existence of an ASD biomarker such as brain anatomy might be useful to facilitate and guide the behavioral diagnosis. This would, however, require further extensive exploration in the clinical setting, particularly with regards to classifier specificity to ASD rather than neurodevelopmental conditions in general.
To address the issue of clinical specificity, the established ASD classifier was used to classify individuals with ADHD—a neurodevelopmental control group. Bilaterally, the ASD classifier did not allocate the majority of ADHD subjects to the ASD category. This indicates that it does not perform equally well for other neurodevelopmental conditions, and is more specific to ASD. To further demonstrate that the classification is driven by autistic symptoms, the test margins of individuals with ASD were correlated with measures of symptom severity (Ecker et al., 2010). We found that larger margins were associated with more severe impairments in the social and communication domain of the ADI-R. The classifier therefore seems to use neuroanatomical information specifically related to ASD rather than simply reflecting nonspecific effects introduced by any kind of pathology. However, due to a recent scanner upgrade, ADHD scans were acquired with different acquisition parameters, while manufacturer, field strength, and pulse sequence remained the same. FreeSurfer has been demonstrated to show good test–retest reliability particularly within scanner-manufacturer and field strength (Han et al., 2006), but we cannot exclude the possibility that systematic differences in regional contrast may have affected the ADHD classification. Future research is thus needed to validate the ADHD findings on an independent sample.
The overall classification accuracy varied across hemispheres (79.0% left vs 65.0% right) in the absence of interhemispheric differences in parameter variability. Hemisphere laterality is an area, which remains relatively unexplored in autism. While our data suggest that the left hemisphere is better at discriminating between groups (i.e., is more “abnormal”), it is unclear whether this discrepancy is due to quantitative differences in parameters or to qualitative aspects of the discriminating patterns (i.e., additional regions). Furthermore, it is also not possible to identify whether individuals with ASD display a higher (lower) degree of cortical asymmetry relative to controls. There is some evidence to suggest that individuals with ASD show a lower degree of “leftward” (i.e., left > right) cortical symmetry than controls (Herbert et al., 2005), which may explain differences in classification accuracy. There is also evidence to suggest that the left hemisphere is under tighter genetic control than the right hemisphere (Thompson et al., 2001), which may be of relevance to a highly heritable condition such as ASD. However, a direct numerical comparison between hemispheres is needed to address this issue directly.
The classification accuracy not only varied across hemispheres but also across morphometric parameters. Bilaterally, cortical thickness provided the best classification accuracy and highest regional weights. Differences in cortical thickness have been reported previously in ASD for both increases (Chung et al., 2005; Hardan et al., 2006) as well as decreases (Chung et al., 2005; Hadjikhani et al., 2006), and in similar regions as reported here (i.e., parietal, temporal, and frontal areas). The overlap with previous studies indicates that these regions display high classification weights due to a quantitative (i.e., “true”) difference rather than high intercorrelations with thickness measures in other brain regions.
Certain geometric features such as average convexity and metric distortion provided above chance classifications as well, particularly in parietal, temporal, and frontal regions, and in areas of the cingulum. Average convexity and metric distortion measure different aspects of cortical geometry (see Materials and Methods) and have previously been linked to ASD, as has sulcal depth (Nordahl et al., 2007). Such geometric features were suggested to reflect abnormal patterns of cortical connectivity. There have also been reports of abnormal patterns of gyrification (Piven et al., 1990; Hardan et al., 2004) and large-scale displacements of the major sulci (Levitt et al., 2003). Thus, our study provides further evidence to support the hypothesis that the “autistic brain” is not just bigger or smaller but is also abnormally shaped.
While we demonstrated that the neuroanatomy of ASD is multidimensional, the etiology of such multivariate differences remains unclear. Here, little or no spatial overlap was observed between the discriminating patterns for individual parameters. Such region dependency was also observed in the regional morphometric profiles displaying the distribution of weights across multiple cortical features in a region of interest. If one assumes that different cortical features reflect different neuropathological processes, such region- and parameter-dependent variations may reflect the multifactorial etiology of ASD. For example, evidence suggests that cortical thickness and surface area reflect different neurobiological processes and are associated with different genetic mechanisms (Panizzon et al., 2009). Cortical thickness is likely to reflect dendritic arborization (Huttenlocher, 1990) or changing myelination at the gray/white matter interface (Sowell et al., 2004). In contrast, surface area is influenced by the division of progenitor cells in the embryological periventricular area, and is associated with the number of minicolumns (Rakic, 1988). Instead, geometric differences are predominantly linked with the development of neuronal connections and cortical pattern of connectivity, and are thus a marker for cerebral development (Armstrong et al., 1995; Van Essen, 1997). It is therefore likely that the reported maps reflect multiple genetic and/or neurobiological etiologies, which need further investigation. Thus, our findings should be interpreted in the context of a number of methodological limitations.
First, the classification algorithm is highly specific to the particular sample used for “training” the classifier, namely high-functioning adults with ASD. The advantage of this approach is that the classifier offers high specificity with regard to this particular subject group, but is less specific to other cohorts on the spectrum. Due to the small sample size, it was also not possible to reliably investigate differences between high-functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome. Evidence (Howlin, 2003) suggests that by adulthood these groups are largely indistinguishable at the phenotypic level. However, the extent to which these groups differ at the level of brain anatomy is unknown, and may be investigating using SVM in the future. Second, 85% of ASD participants in our sample were diagnosed using the ADI-R, and 15% were diagnosed using the ADOS. As both diagnostic tools measure autistic symptoms at different developmental stages, the classifier may be biased toward individuals with an early diagnosis of ASD. Although it is not expected that classifier performance on the basis of ADOS and ADI differ drastically, diagnostic heterogeneity may be a potential limitation. Last, SVM is a multivariate technique and hence offers a limited degree of interpretability of specific network components. Additional analysis such as “searchlight” or “virtual lesions” approaches (Averbeck et al., 2006; Kriegeskorte et al., 2006; Pessoa and Padmala, 2007) may therefore be combined with SVM in the future to establish the relative contribution of individual regions/parameters to the overall classification performance.
Nevertheless, while classification values and specific patterns we report must be considered as preliminary, our study offers a “proof of concept” for describing the complex multidimensional gray matter differences in ASD.
Footnotes
-
This work was undertaken with the support of the A.I.M.S. Consortium (Autism Imaging Multicentre Study), which is funded by the Medical Research Council UK (G0400061), and a programme grant on the diagnosis and treatment of young adults with autism funded by the National Institute for Health Research (RP-PG-0606-1045). We would also like to thank the Wellcome Trust, which funded J.M.-M.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Christine Ecker, Section of Brain Maturation, PO50, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. Christine.ecker{at}kcl.ac.uk
This article is freely available online through the J Neurosci Open Choice option.