Abstract
In two recent papers (Rheims et al., 2009; Holmgren et al., 2010), Zilberter and coworkers argue that the well known depolarizing GABA actions that take place at the cellular and network level in the neonatal hippocampus and neocortex in vitro are pathophysiological phenomena, attributable to deficient mitochondrial energy metabolism. In their experiments, supplementing the glucose-containing solution with weak-acid substrates of mitochondrial energy metabolism (such as β-hydroxy-butyrate, lactate, or pyruvate) abolished the spontaneous network events (giant depolarizing potentials; GDPs) and the underlying depolarizing actions of GABA. In this study, we made electrophysiological recordings of GDPs and monitored the mitochondrial membrane potential (Ψm) and intracellular pH (pHi) in CA3 neurons in neonatal rat hippocampal slices. Supplementing the standard physiological solution with l-lactate did not produce a change in Ψm, whereas withdrawal of glucose, in the presence or absence of l-lactate, was followed by a pronounced depolarization of Ψm. Furthermore, d-lactate (a poor substrate of mitochondrial metabolism) caused a prompt inhibition in GDP frequency which was similar to the effect of l-lactate. The suppression of GDPs was strictly proportional to the fall in pHi caused by weak carboxylic acids (l-lactate, d-lactate, or propionate) or by an elevated CO2. The main conclusions of our work are that the inhibitory effect of l-lactate on GDPs is not mediated by mitochondrial energy metabolism, and that glucose at its standard 10 mm concentration is an adequate energy substrate for neonatal neurons in vitro. Notably, changes in pHi appear to have a very powerful modulatory effect on GDPs.
Introduction
Activation of GABAA receptors has been shown to lead to depolarizing and sometimes even functionally excitatory responses in a wide variety of immature neurons (for review, see Cherubini et al., 1991; Ben-Ari et al., 2007; Blaesse et al., 2009). This type of GABA action is generally thought to be a crucial mechanism in the generation of spontaneous network events in the developing brain (Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Sipilä et al., 2005). However, in two recent publications, Zilberter and coworkers (Rheims et al., 2009; Holmgren et al., 2010) have challenged the physiological relevance of the above observations, suggesting instead that depolarizing GABA actions and the consequent network events are pathophysiological manifestations of an energy deficiency in neonatal neurons kept under standard in vitro conditions. The main argument put forward in the above studies is that, although glucose is the primary metabolic substrate in the adult brain, immature neurons rely on the supply of additional substrates, such as ketone bodies, lactate, and pyruvate, to drive mitochondrial ATP production. Hence, Zilberter and colleagues suggested that, when using preparations from neonatal rodents, the composition of the exogenous energy substrate pool is inadequate if only glucose is present.
All the conclusions by Zilberter's team regarding the state of energy metabolism in neonatal neurons are based on recordings of electrophysiological parameters such as the resting membrane potential, EGABA, and giant depolarizing potentials (GDPs), which are promoted by depolarizing GABAA actions (Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Sipilä et al., 2005). The metabolic state of mitochondria, which Zilberter and colleagues proposed to be the locus of action of the additional (nonglucose) substrates, was not monitored in their studies. In the present study, we examined whether an enriched energy substrate pool suppresses network activity via changes in mitochondrial functions. Our main focus was on l-lactate, a well known substrate of oxidative energy metabolism both in neonatal and adult neurons (Erecinska et al., 2004; Raichle and Mintun, 2006). According to Holmgren et al. (2010), addition of 5 mm l-lactate into the standard physiological solution should provide in vitro conditions where energy metabolism is not compromised (see Discussion).
Our data, based on experiments with l- and d-lactate, propionate, and elevated CO2, show that inhibition of GDPs with application of l-lactate is not related to changes in the metabolic state of mitochondria. Instead, the fall in neuronal intracellular pH (pHi) induced by the weak acids is sufficient to account for the suppression of GDPs.
Materials and Methods
Experiments were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the University of Helsinki. Most experiments were performed on horizontal hippocampal slices (400 μm) from postnatal day (P) 6–8 Wistar rat pups, as described previously (Sipilä et al., 2006a), with some additional observations at P4–P5. Data are reported as mean ± SEM with the accompanying n value referring to the number of slices from which data were obtained, unless otherwise stated. Statistical analyses were performed with Student's paired t test. Least-squares cubic linear fit (York, 1966) and weighted correlation coefficient (r) were calculated with lsqubic.m Matlab algorithm to determine the correlation between GDP frequency and intracellular acidification.
Electrophysiological recordings.
The standard physiological solution contained the following (in mm): 124 NaCl, 4 KCl, 1.3 MgSO4, 2 CaCl2, 1.1 NaH2PO4, 10 d-glucose, and 25 NaHCO3 (solutions equilibrated with 5% CO2/95% O2; pH 7.4 at the experimental temperature of 32°C). The slightly higher GDP frequency attained with 4 mm versus 3.5 mm extracellular K+ (Sipilä et al., 2005) was considered advantageous in experiments where a reduction of GDP frequency was expected to take place. Field-potential electrodes were filled with 150 mm NaCl and recordings were done from submerged slices (double-sided perfusion at 4 ml/min, chamber volume 1 ml) from the CA3c-b area. Recordings were low-pass filtered at 2 kHz and digitized at 5 kHz using Strathclyde Electrophysiology WinEDR (provided by John Dempster) and band-passed at 0.2–25 or 2–25 Hz for the illustrations. Only slices with a stable baseline frequency of GDPs >0.05 Hz were used in the experiments. GDP frequency was quantified with an in-house program using a sliding bin of 120 s and a sliding window of 15 s.
l- and d-Lactate (Sigma) and propionate (propionic acid sodium-salt; Fluka) were used at 5 mm concentration unless otherwise stated, and 3-(aminosulphonyl)-5-(butylamino)-4-phenoxybenzoic acid (bumetanide; Sigma) was applied at 10 μm. In glucose depletion experiments, 10 mm d-glucose was replaced with an equimolar amount of sucrose. In experiments where the CO2 content was increased from 5% to 8%, 15 mm NaHCO3 (final HCO3− concentration 40 mm) was added to maintain the extracellular pH constant, with an equimolar amount of Na-d-gluconate (15 mm) in the control solution to keep the osmolarity constant.
Intracellular pH measurements.
The fluorescence imaging system consisted of an OptoLED light source (excitation wavelengths, 500 ± 10 and 436 ± 10 nm; Cairn Research), an upright microscope (60×; water-immersion objective; numerical aperture, 0.9; Zeiss) and a PCO SensiCam CCD camera. Images were captured (at 0.1–0.2 Hz) and analyzed with WinFluor software (provided by John Dempster). Slices were perfused at 2.5 ml/min (chamber volume, 1.5 ml). BCECF-AM (10 μm; Invitrogen) was injected into the CA3c-b area via patch pipette and the fluorescent signal was measured and calibrated as before (Ruusuvuori et al, 2004). After loading, slices were left to recover for 20–30 min before recordings from the somatic region of CA3 pyramidal neurons were started.
Measurements of mitochondrial membrane potential.
Hippocampal slices were bath loaded with 5 μm rhodamine 123 (Rh123; Molecular Probes) for 25 min at room temperature. Before recordings were started, slices were washed for ≥30 min. The fluorescence imaging system consisted of a Colibri light source (excitation wavelength, 510 ± 8 nm; emission, >530 nm; Zeiss), a Zeiss AxioExaminerZ1 upright microscope (60×, water-immersion objective; numerical aperture, 0.9), and an AxioCam CCD camera. Images were captured at 0.1–0.03 Hz and analyzed with ImageJ software (National Institutes of Health). Slices were perfused at 2.5 ml/min (chamber volume, 1.0 ml; recording temperature, 32 ± 0.5°C). The regions of interest enclosed 4–8 CA3 pyramidal cell somata. Mean fluorescence intensity is expressed as f/f0, where f0 is the baseline fluorescence intensity, and f the fluorescence intensity at a defined time point. The protonophore carbonyl cyanide4-(trifluoromethoxy) phenylhydrazone (FCCP, 4–10 μm; Sigma) dissipated mitochondrial membrane potential (Ψm) (Duchen, 1992; Ortsäter et al., 2002), as indicated by an increase in Rh123 fluorescence.
Results
GDPs are suppressed by both l- and d-lactate
Extracellular field potential recordings in the CA3 stratum pyramidale of P6–P8 hippocampal slices showed that bath application of 5 mm l-lactate produced a fast reduction in the frequency of GDPs to 51.4 ± 4.9% (n = 12; p < 0.001) from the normalized baseline values. The maximum effect was reached within ∼2 min (Fig. 1A). Notably, a similar rapid decrease in GDP frequency was caused by d-lactate (to 37.2 ± 5.6%; n = 9; p < 0.001) (Fig. 1A). In the continuous presence of the weak acids, the decrease in GDP frequency was followed by a recovery toward its original level (after 20 min in l-lactate, GDP frequency was 87.6 ± 4.0% of the control value and, in d-lactate, 86.1 ± 5.6% of the control value). Removal of l- and d-lactate after the recovery induced a rebound increase in GDP frequency (to 157.0 ± 29.0%, n = 4 and 133.0 ± 15.0%, n = 9, respectively). The transient suppression of GDPs cannot be related to an improvement in mitochondrial energy metabolism because the effects of the poorly metabolized isomer d-lactate were qualitatively and quantitatively similar to those obtained with l-lactate. Decreasing K+ from 4.0 to 3.5 mm did not alter the effect of lactate (Holmgren et al., 2010, their supplemental Fig. 1). In experiments with 3.5 mm K+, 5 mm l-lactate application on P4–P5 slices reduced GDP frequency to 67 ± 12% (p < 0.05; n = 5).
Propionate has been used as a standard tool to manipulate pHi in neurons and various other cell types (Roos and Boron, 1981). In accordance with the experiments with l- and d-lactate, bath application of 5 mm propionate caused a peak decrease in the frequency of the GDPs (to 19.5 ± 6.0%; n = 4; p < 0.001) from the normalized baseline values. The more pronounced decrease in GDPs with 5 mm propionate can be explained on the basis of the differences in the pKa values of the weak acids used. With an identical total concentration of the two weak acids at a pH of ∼7.4, the membrane-permeable protonated form of propionate (pKa, 4.87) is present at ∼10× higher concentration than that of lactate (pKa, 3.86), which leads to a much higher initial rate of net influx of H+ (Roos and Boron, 1981; Mason et al., 1990). In agreement with this, a rapid and pronounced reduction of GDP frequency (to 37.2 ± 8%, n = 6) (Fig. 1A) was seen in the presence of only 1 mm propionate.
In the immature CA3 pyramidal neurons, the driving force for GABAA receptor-mediated depolarizing currents is provided by the Na-K-2 Cl cotransporter isoform 1 (NKCC1)-mediated Cl−-uptake (Yamada et al., 2004; Sipilä et al., 2006b). The fact the GDP generation was not blocked in the presence of l-lactate suggests that GABA remains depolarizing following a prolonged exposure to these weak acids. We examined this more closely in experiments with bumetanide (10 μm), a specific blocker of NKCC1 that has been shown to inhibit GDPs in the neonatal rat hippocampus (Dzhala et al., 2005; Sipilä et al., 2006b). As expected, bumetanide completely blocked the occurrence of GDPs in the continuous presence of 5 mm l-lactate (lactate application for >40 min, n = 4) (Fig. 1B).
GDPs are suppressed upon glucose depletion
Glucose depletion resulted in a near-complete block of GDPs. The maximum effect on frequency (4.3 ± 1.8% of baseline; n = 9) was attained with a delay of 5–20 min. When glucose depletion was done in the continuous presence of 5 mm l-lactate, there was a similar run-down of GDPs (n = 7) (Fig. 1C). The latter result shows that, unlike glucose, 5 mm l-lactate alone is not able to support GDPs. During the onset of glucose depletion, GDP frequency decreased steadily with no signs of increased frequency or amplitude such as might be predicted if energy deficiency would cause pathophysiological hyperactivity (Holmgren et al., 2010).
Mitochondrial membrane potential is not affected by l-lactate application in the presence of glucose
It is well established that neuronal energy substrate utilization undergoes marked changes during development (Erecinska et al., 2004). However, findings of this kind do not justify the conclusion (Rheims et al., 2009; Holmgren et al., 2010) that glucose would be an inadequate energy substrate for neurons in in vitro experiments (Tyzio et al., 2010). This is an empirical question that needs to be tested. Hence, we looked at the consequences of glucose on GDPs in the absence and presence of l-lactate.
If, as argued by Zilberter and colleagues, neonatal neurons were energy depleted in standard physiological solution, addition of weak-acid substrate of oxidative energy metabolism would be expected to cause a change in the mitochondrial membrane potential, ψm. Therefore, we performed measurements of ψm from CA3 pyramidal neurons with the fluorescent indicator Rh123. In seven of seven slices, addition of 5 mm l-lactate to the standard solution did not cause a detectable change in ψm within CA3 neurons. The normalized Rh123 fluorescence after 5 mm l-lactate was 99.7 ± 0.2% of the control value (p = 0.18) (Fig. 2A). Furthermore, in the continuous presence of 10 mm glucose, substituting 5 mm l-lactate with the poorly metabolizable d-lactate had no effect on ψm (99.4 ± 0.4%, p = 0.23, n = 4) (Fig. 2A,B). In striking contrast to the findings with l-lactate, glucose depletion produced a progressive, slowly developing increase in the Rh123 fluorescence, reflecting a gradual depolarization of ψm (mean increase of the normalized fluorescence to 109.7±.1.1%, p = 0.001, n = 5) (Fig. 2A,C). The depolarization fully recovered when glucose concentration was restored. The obvious conclusion from the above data is that, in neurons of neonatal hippocampal slices, mitochondrial energy production can be fully supported by glucose. This conclusion gained further support in experiments where glucose depletion caused an increase in Rh123 fluorescence even in the continuous presence of 5 mm l-lactate (to 107.1 ± 0.4%, p < 0.001, n = 6) (Fig. 2A).
We found no evidence suggesting that neonatal hippocampal neurons in vitro are under metabolic stress in the absence of l-lactate, or that adding l-lactate would have any beneficial effect on mitochondrial function.
Modulation of intraneuronal pH with weak acids
In the standard solution, the baseline pHi of the P6–P8 CA3 pyramidal neurons was 7.20 ± 0.01 (n = 20 cells from seven slices). Exposure to 5 mm l- or d-lactate resulted in a rapid acidification, which achieved its maximal value within 2 min (fall in pHi for d-lactate, 0.022 ± 0.004, n = 6 cells from three slices; l-lactate, 0.028 ± 0.008, n = 6 cells from three slices) (Fig. 3A,B). Application of 1 mm propionate evoked an intracellular acidosis comparable to that obtained with l- and d-lactate (0.024 ± 0.002, n = 11 cells from four slices), whereas the pHi change upon exposure to 5 mm propionate was 0.056 ± 0.003 (n = 12 cells from four slices) (Fig. 3A,B).
We next asked whether the fall in neuronal pHi caused by the weak acids could account for their inhibitory action on GDP activity. Using changes in CO2 content is another standard way to manipulate cellular acid–base parameters (Roos and Boron, 1981; Kaila and Ransom, 1998; Dulla et al., 2005). Elevating CO2 from 5% to 8% at a constant extracellular pH caused a prompt intraneuronal acidosis (0.064 ± 0.004, n = 9) (Fig. 3A–C). In field potential recordings, 8% CO2 caused a transient block of GDPs (n = 5) (Fig. 3C). Notably, in all experimental paradigms, the fall in pHi was paralleled by a decrease in GDP frequency, both of which gradually recovered toward their original levels. As shown in Figure 3D, the suppression of GDP frequency by the three weak carboxylic acids and elevated CO2 shows a striking quantitative dependence on the fall of pHi, with a weighted correlation coefficient of −0.97. Although these data do not imply that the soma (where the pHi responses are measured) is the subcellular compartment that mediates the effect of the acidosis on GDPs, it is evident that the suppression of GDPs shows a strict dependence on the intracellular acid load (see Discussion).
Discussion
In their recent studies, Zilberter and coworkers (Rheims et al., 2009; Holmgren et al., 2010) claimed that depolarizing GABA responses recorded in neonatal neurons in vitro are caused by an insufficient energy substrate pool. They showed that addition of substrates such as β-hydroxy-butyrate, pyruvate, or lactate to a glucose-containing solution blocked GDPs. According to the results presented in Holmgren et al. (2010), addition of exogenous lactate at 5 mm concentration is sufficient to provide in vitro conditions where energy metabolism of the neonatal neurons is not compromised.
It is obvious from the available literature that d-lactate and propionate are not efficient substrates for mitochondrial energy production in the mammalian brain (Poole and Halestrap, 1993; Ros et al., 2001; Nguyen et al., 2007; Gibbs and Hertz, 2008). However, both d- and l-lactate as well as propionate caused a significant reduction in the frequency of GDPs, which was followed by a gradual recovery in the continuous presence of the weak acid. After the recovery from l-lactate-induced GDP suppression, bumetanide, an NKCC1-specific antagonist, fully blocked GDPs. These results indicate that, in contrast to the conclusions by Holmgren et al. (2010), the depolarizing GABA actions needed to drive GDPs are not abolished in the presence of l-lactate. Furthermore, direct measurements of the Ψm showed that exogenous l-lactate is not needed for maintaining Ψm in a standard glucose-containing physiological solution. In contrast to this, glucose depletion caused a gradual dissipation of the Ψm and a near-complete block of GDPs, regardless of the absence or presence of l-lactate. Even in its initial phase, the energy–metabolic crisis that was evident after glucose depletion did not lead to neuronal hyperexcitability, as would be predicted on the basis of the hypothesis by Holmgren at al. (2010).
The effects of weak acids on GDPs can be readily explained on the basis of pHi changes. We found a quantitatively strict dependence (Fig. 3D) of the suppression of the GDP frequency on the intraneuronal acidosis brought about by the weak carboxylic acids and by elevating the CO2 level from 5% to 8%. In addition to their effect on neuronal acid-base parameters (Chesler, 2003), carboxylic acids and elevated CO2 are not likely to share any other mechanism of action to influence neuronal functions. Depending on the local pH-modulatory mechanisms, experimentally induced changes in pHi show variation among distinct subcellular compartments and microdomains (Willoughby and Schwiening, 2002), and the specific technique used for acidifying the neurons will influence the magnitude of the pH changes evoked in the soma and elsewhere. The present pHi measurements were done in the neuronal soma. This does not imply that the subcellular target of the pH-modulatory action on GDPs is the neuronal cell body, but our data demonstrate that the suppression of GDPs is strictly related to the net acid load imposed by the three carboxylic acids (Fig. 3). The full block achieved by elevated CO2 is in agreement with this quantitative relation. Identifying the molecular targets of the GDP-suppressing effect of the fall in pHi is beyond the scope of the present study. However, likely candidates include voltage-dependent Na+ and Ca2+ channels and acid-sensing K+ channels (Tombaugh and Somjen, 1998; Dulla et al., 2005, 2009; Enyedi and Czirják, 2010).
In summary, our results show that the energy demands of the neonatal pyramidal neurons in hippocampal slices are fully met when glucose, used at the conventional in vitro concentration of 10 mm, is the only exogenous energy substrate. Moreover, the acute suppression of GDP frequency upon weak-acid application has nothing to do with a change in oxidative energy metabolism as shown by direct measurements of Ψm. The present results imply that the lactate-induced decrease in GDP frequency observed by Holmgren et al. (2010) is attributable to an intraneuronal acidosis. The pronounced suppression of GDPs by a very small acid load (as seen with 1 mm propionate) indicates that neonatal neurons are exquisitely sensitive to changes in pHi. This finding is likely to have important implications on the generation of endogenous network events under physiological and pathophysiological conditions (Helmy et al., 2010) in the developing brain.
Footnotes
-
This study was supported by grants from the Academy of Finland, the Sigrid Jusélius Foundation, and the Jane and Aatos Erkko Foundation. K.K. is a member of the Finnish Center of Excellence in Molecular and Integrative Neuroscience Research. We thank Prof. Karl E. Åkerman and Dr. Alexander Surin for their advice in the work on mitochondria, and Prof. Juha Voipio for his valuable comments on an early draft of this paper.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Prof. Kai Kaila, Department of Biosciences, University of Helsinki, FI-00014 Helsinki, Finland. Kai.Kaila{at}Helsinki.Fi