Abstract
Stimulus-specific response potentiation (SRP) is a robust form of experience-dependent plasticity that occurs in primary visual cortex. In awake mice, visual evoked potentials (VEPs) recorded in layer 4 of binocular visual cortex undergo increases in amplitude with repeated presentation of a sinusoidal grating stimulus over days. This effect is highly specific to the experienced stimulus. Here, we test whether the mechanisms of thalamocortical long-term potentiation (LTP), induced with a theta burst electrical stimulation (TBS) of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus, are sufficient to account for SRP. First, we demonstrate that LTP similarly enhances the amplitude of VEPs, but in a way that generalizes across multiple stimuli, spatial frequencies, and contrasts. Second, we show that LTP occludes the subsequent expression of SRP. Third, we reveal that previous SRP occludes TBS-induced LTP of the VEP evoked by the experienced stimulus, but not by unfamiliar stimuli. Finally, we show that SRP is rapidly and selectively reversed by local cortical infusion of a peptide that inhibits PKMζ, a constitutively active kinase known to maintain NMDA receptor-dependent LTP and memory. Thus, SRP is expressed by the same core mechanisms as LTP. SRP therefore provides a simple assay to assess the integrity of LTP in the intact nervous system. Moreover, the results suggest that LTP of visual cortex, like SRP, can potentially be exploited to improve vision.
Introduction
Long-term potentiation (LTP) is a form of synaptic plasticity elicited in the brain by patterned electrical stimulation (Bliss and Lømo, 1973). The mechanisms of LTP have been elucidated in numerous studies motivated by the belief that this phenomenon reveals a core process of experience-dependent information storage that may go awry in psychiatric disorders and be targeted by therapies designed to correct them (Martin et al., 2000; Malenka and Bear, 2004; Neves et al., 2008). Although LTP-like changes have been shown to occur after several types of behavioral training (Doyère et al., 1995; Rioult-Pedotti et al., 2000; Schafe et al., 2005; Gruart et al., 2006; Whitlock et al., 2006; Tye et al., 2008), it has yet to be determined that these synaptic modifications are sufficient to account for the resulting memory (Neves et al., 2008). This is a difficult determination to make, particularly in the case of memories that are encoded by sparse, iterative, and widely distributed synaptic modifications deep inside the brain (i.e., many synapses away from the sensory periphery). Consequently, there is no established, noninvasive paradigm in which one can be confident that a measured change in electrophysiological response or behavior is attributable specifically to induction of LTP.
In the current study, we examine the possibility that the mechanism of LTP may be sufficient to account for a surrogate electrophysiological measure of perceptual learning in the mouse visual system. In multiple sensory modalities and species, it has been shown that repeated presentation of a sensory stimulus results in sensory gains that may be highly specific to the familiar stimulus (Poggio et al., 1992; Fahle and Morgan, 1996). The lack of transfer in perceptual learning from one stimulus to another has led to the suggestion that the underlying plasticity occurs in primary sensory cortex (Karni and Bertini, 1997; Gilbert et al., 2001). Imaging studies in humans support this hypothesis (Furmanski et al., 2004) and recording directly from mouse primary visual cortex reveals that local plasticity does occur after repeated exposure to visual stimuli (Frenkel et al., 2006). Stimulus-specific response potentiation (SRP) of the cortical visual evoked potential (VEP) occurs in response to brief daily presentation of a particular visual stimulus. SRP is input-specific and long-lasting, and is prevented by blockade of NMDA receptors and by cortical delivery of a peptide that inhibits AMPA receptor insertion. All these properties resemble those of canonical LTP.
Here, we use the VEP as an assay to determine whether electrically induced geniculocortical LTP has equivalent functional effects to SRP. Our experiments reveal that LTP and SRP mimic and mutually occlude one another. Additionally, we find that established SRP is rapidly erased by injection of a peptide that has been shown to reverse LTP in the hippocampus. The mechanisms of LTP therefore are necessary and sufficient to account for SRP. These findings strengthen the hypothesis that LTP is a mechanism for perceptual learning and suggest that SRP is a simple assay to assess the integrity of LTP in the intact nervous system.
Materials and Methods
Animals.
All animals were male C57BL/6 mice (Charles River Laboratories). Animals were implanted on postnatal day 30 (P30). Mice were group housed with food and water available ad libitum and maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle.
Electrode implantation.
Mice were anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of 50 mg/kg ketamine and 10 mg/kg xylazine. The 1% lidocaine hydrochloride anesthetic was injected locally under the scalp of the mouse before incision. The skull was then cleaned with iodine and 70% ethanol. A steel headpost was then affixed to the skull just anterior to bregma using cyanoacrylate glue for subsequent head restraint. Small (<0.5 mm) burr holes were drilled in the skull overlying the binocular visual cortex (3.2 mm lateral of lambda). Tapered tungsten microelectrodes (FHC), 75 μm in diameter at their widest point, were inserted in each hemisphere and positioned 470 μm below the cortical surface in the stereotaxic dorsal–ventral axis. Silver wire (A-M Systems) reference electrodes were placed bilaterally over prefrontal cortex. All electrodes were then secured in place using cyanoacrylate glue. Circular plastic walls (cut from pipette tips) were glued into place to create a 3 × 3 mm exposed aperture above the skull for subsequent placement of a stimulating electrode into the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) (see Fig. 1 A). A permanent marker was used to demarcate the site of entry for LGN stimulation (2.1 mm posterior to bregma, 2.2 mm lateral to midline). Dental cement was applied to cover the remaining skull and enclose all electrodes into a stable, protective head cap. Animals were monitored postoperatively for signs of infection or discomfort and were allowed at least 24 h recovery before habituation to the restraint apparatus.
VEP recording.
VEP recordings were conducted in awake, head-restrained mice. Animals were habituated to the restraint apparatus before recording by sitting in situ in front of a gray screen. The animals remained still but alert during subsequent presentation of discrete visual stimuli. VEP amplitude was quantified by measuring trough–peak response amplitude, as described previously (Sawtell et al., 2003; Frenkel and Bear, 2004). For most experiments, animals were presented with 400 phase reversals of each oriented stimulus, delivered binocularly, on a given day. For acuity/contrast sensitivity experiments, in which a large number of different stimuli were presented in each session, the number of phase reversals averaged for each stimulus was reduced to 200.
ZIP infusions.
Animals underwent the normal VEP electrode implantation surgery under ketamine/xylazine anesthesia but were also implanted bilaterally with 26 gauge guide cannulae (Plastics One). Guide cannulae were positioned just lateral to the recording electrode at a 45° angle to the skull so as to target the VEP recording site, and lowered 0.1 mm below surface. Guide cannulae were then fixed in place with cyanoacrylate glue and dental cement. Dummy cannulae were inserted. Animals were allowed to recover and habituated to the recording apparatus for 2 d before experiments. On the first day of VEP recording, dummy cannulae were removed to determine whether physical displacement would, in any way, affect the VEP. They were repositioned at the end of the recording session. Solutions were prepared on the day of injection. Syringes and guide tubing attached to 33 gauge injection cannulae were filled with distilled water, which was separated from the injected solution with an air bubble. Dummies were again removed, and injection cannulae were inserted through guides and allowed to sit in place for 5 min before injection. Slight positive pressure was maintained on the syringe while inserting the injection cannulae, using a KD Scientific infusion pump, to prevent blockage. Myr-SIYRRGARRWRKL-OH (ZIP) (Invitrogen), the specific pseudosubstrate inhibitor of PKMζ, was dissolved in artificial CSF to a concentration of 10 nmol/μl. One microliter was infused over the course of 10 min using the infusion pump. Scrambled control (myr-RLYRKRIWRSAGR-OH) was prepared to the equivalent concentration and infused in the same manner.
Visual stimuli.
Visual stimuli consisted of full-field, 100% contrast, sinusoidal gratings generated by a VSG2/2 card (Cambridge Research System) and presented, linearized by γ correction, on a computer monitor. VEPs were elicited by vertical (90°), or oblique (45 or 135°) bars, phase reversing at a frequency of 0.5 Hz. For acuity experiments, stimuli ranged across 0.05, 0.15, 0.3, 0.45, 0.6, 0.8, and 1 cycle/°, and for contrast sensitivity experiments stimuli ranged across 0, 1.5, 3.125, 6.25, 12.5, 25, 50, and 100% contrast. The display was positioned 20 cm in front of the mouse and centered on the midline, thereby occupying 92 × 66° of the visual field. Mean luminance, determined by a photodiode placed in front of the computer screen, was 27 cd/m2.
Thalamic stimulation.
Animals were kept in the dark for 2 h before in vivo LTP experiments. Mice were transiently anesthetized under isoflurane (1%) while positioned in the VEP recording apparatus (body temperature was maintained using a small heating blanket). A burr hole was drilled according to previous demarcation (2.1 mm anteroposterior, 2.2 mm mediolateral), and a stainless-steel concentric bipolar stimulating electrode (FHC), 200 μm in diameter with a 25 μm Pt/Ir inner pole, was advanced slowly from surface. Stimuli (0.2 ms pulse width) were applied every 10 s while recording evoked field potentials through chronically implanted recording electrodes. Approximately 2.4–2.5 mm from surface, a monosynaptically driven field potential was detected, indicating arrival at the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN), and depth was finely adjusted to optimize the response amplitude. Given the proximity of the dLGN to the overlying hippocampus, it was essential that we distinguish between volume-conducted electrical responses in visual cortex elicited by hippocampal stimulation and synaptic field potentials resulting from thalamic stimulation. In initial experiments, we recorded light flash-evoked multiunit activity through the stimulating electrode to determine when we were positioned in the dLGN. Such evoked activity does not occur in hippocampus. This enabled us to determine that a signature of hippocampal-mediated, volume-conducted responses is their relatively long latency and characteristic positive-going morphology that does not change regardless of recording electrode depth. The optimal short-latency field potential was observed when the electrode was in contact with the optic radiation fibers emitted from the anterior pole of the dLGN. Field potentials were evoked at 60% maximum using a stimulus intensity ranging from 0.1 to 0.7 mA. Once stable, field potentials were evoked every 30 s over a half-hour baseline before inducing LTP with theta burst stimulation (TBS). TBS consisted of bursts of six pulses delivered at 100 Hz, repeated six times at a frequency of 10 Hz, all of which was repeated six times, separated by 20 s intervals. This whole tetanus was repeated three times with an interval of 5 min between each to saturate LTP. Field potentials were then followed for 1 h before removal of the stimulating electrode, application of dental cement to cover the craniotomy, and recovery of the animal from isoflurane anesthesia in the home cage. LTP data were normalized, averaged, and reported as mean ± SEM. Changes in synaptic strength were measured by comparing the average response amplitude 50–60 min after the final TBS stimulation to the pretetanus baseline response.
Histology.
Formalin perfusion and histology were performed after all experiments. Chronically implanted recording electrode position was determined by making an electrolytic lesions and staining fixed, sectioned brains with cresyl violet. The position of acutely placed stimulating electrodes in the dLGN was also confirmed in this manner.
Statistical analyses.
LTP data are presented as a percentage of average pre-TBS baseline. Each presented sample point is an average of four recorded data points over 2 min. VEP data are presented either raw or normalized (as described in the text and figure legends) and each data point is an average of 400 or 200 individual VEPs. All data are reported as mean ± SEM. Comparison of electrically evoked potentials in LTP experiments were evaluated using a paired t test. Single-day comparisons of either raw or normalized VEP amplitudes were made with unpaired t tests for comparisons between two groups and paired t tests for comparisons within animals. One-way ANOVA was applied for comparisons between three or more groups of VEPs and repeated-measures ANOVAs for comparisons across days. Fisher's least significant difference test was applied post hoc to determine which comparisons yielded significant difference.
Results
Orientation selectivity of SRP
As previously reported (Frenkel et al., 2006), repeated presentation of a phase-reversing, sinusoidal grating stimulus to a head-fixed, awake mouse (Fig. 1 A) results in a pronounced increase in the trough-peak amplitude of averaged VEPs recorded from layer 4 of binocular visual cortex. This phenomenon is described as stimulus-specific because the enhanced VEP is not driven by stimuli of different orientations. Here, we further investigated the specificity of SRP by varying the orientation, spatial frequency, or contrast of test stimuli after SRP had been saturated to a 0.5 Hz phase-reversing, full-field, 0.05 cycle/°, 100% contrast stimulus of a particular oblique orientation (henceforth termed X°, because the orientation was varied from animal to animal) presented binocularly (for experimental time line, see Fig. 1 B). SRP developed gradually over the course of 5 d. The amplitude of VEPs recorded on day 1 (144.8 ± 14.6 μV) approximately doubled by day 5 (269.1 ± 20.9 μV, n = 7; one-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (4,6) = 22.4, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 1 C). On day 5, once SRP had been saturated, several different novel orientations of stimuli were presented, randomly interleaved, to determine the degree of orientation specificity of SRP. The amplitude of VEPs driven by these novel orientations are presented in Figure 1 D. A stimulus just 5° different in orientation from the familiar X° stimulus evoked VEPs of significantly lower amplitude (136.4 ± 12.6% day 1 VEP amplitude) than the familiar stimulus (F (6,42) = 2.799, p < 0.05; Fisher's post hoc test for comparison of familiar X° orientation with X + 5° orientation, p < 0.05). Thus, for orientation at least, SRP can be described as exquisitely specific.
SRP at a single spatial frequency or contrast does not result in a shift in acuity or contrast sensitivity
We continued to investigate the specificity of SRP by presenting familiar X° and novel X + 90°-oriented stimuli at varying spatial frequency or contrast. These results are presented in Figure 1, E and F, respectively. At a fixed contrast of 100%, stimuli of 0.05, 0.15, 0.3, 0.45, and 0.6 cycle/° evoked VEPs discernible over noise (as determined with a static gray screen). At the lowest spatial frequency of 0.6 cycle/°, VEPs were significantly greater amplitude (43.6 ± 5.1 μV) than trough–peak measurements for a gray screen (27.8 ± 2.4 μV; paired t test, t (6) = 5.02; p < 0.005). All unfamiliar spatial frequencies evoked VEPs of equivalent magnitudes at the two orientations (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,3) = 0.328, p = 0.805 for the interaction effects of stimulus orientation and spatial frequency). The familiar spatial frequency of 0.05 cycle/°, at which the familiar 100% contrast X° orientation evoked VEPs (256.2 ± 20.7 μV) almost double the amplitude of those driven by the novel X + 90° stimulus (147.6 ± 17.5 μV) was significantly enhanced, because of SRP (paired t test, t (6) = 7.81, p < 0.0005). The same was true for contrast. Stimuli presented at contrast as low as 6.25% evoked VEPs (37.5 ± 2.7 μV) significantly discernible above trough–peak measurements for a gray screen (27.8 ± 2.4 μV; paired t test, t (6) = 2.52; p < 0.05; denoted by the dotted line in Fig. 1 E,F). However, VEPs were of equivalent magnitude when driven by both familiar and novel stimuli at all contrast values other than the familiar 100% contrast (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,3) = 1.647, p = 0.147 for the interaction effects of stimulus orientation and contrast. Note that the familiar stimulus was both 100% contrast and 0.05 cycle/°, so VEP amplitudes and statistic are the same as those already given for the familiar X° orientation). Thus, repeated presentation of a stimulus at one spatial frequency or contrast did not result in a general enhancement of spatial acuity or contrast sensitivity.
Repeated presentation of multiple stimuli enhances spatial acuity and contrast sensitivity
To determine whether SRP, despite its specificity, could be used to enhance spatial acuity or contrast sensitivity, we then repeatedly presented a group of mice (n = 9) with a single X°-oriented stimulus at a range of spatial frequencies and contrasts. After 5 d of repeated presentation of 100% contrast stimuli at 0.05, 0.15, 0.3, 0.45, 0.6, 0.8 cycle/°, VEPs showed a varying degree of potentiation relative to day 1 (Fig. 2 A,B). Notably, VEPs that were not discernible above noise levels (22.2 ± 1.6 μV; dotted lines) at 0.8 cycle/° on day 1 (30.6 ± 2.0 μV, n = 9; paired t test, t (8) = 1.21; p = 0.26) were small, but present on day 5 (39.4 ± 5.4 μV; paired t test, t (8) = 3.31; p < 0.05). The emergence of visual cortical activity driven by stimuli that did not evoke VEPs before visual experience suggests that spatial acuity can be modestly enhanced by SRP. Normalization of VEP amplitude on day 5 to day 1 (Fig. 2 B) reveals that SRP increased visual cortical activity across all spatial frequencies (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,5) = 23.62, p < 0.0001 for the interaction effects of orientation and spatial frequency). Similarly, after repeated presentation of 0.05 cycle/° stimuli across contrast values ranging from 0 to 100%, VEPs emerged at 3.125% contrast (41.6 ± 7.1 μV) compared with noise levels measured to a gray screen (27.8 ± 2.4 μV; paired t test, t (8) = 3.73; p < 0.01) (Fig. 2 C,D). On day 1, this contrast did not drive VEPs (26.2 ± 1.6 μV) above noise levels, again suggesting that SRP could be used to enhance contrast sensitivity. Normalization of the amplitude of VEPs on day 5 to those on day 1 (Fig. 2 D) reveals that SRP resulted in significant gains in visual cortical activity at 100, 50, 25, 12.5, 6.25, and 3.125% contrast (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,6) = 11.903, p < 0.0001 for the interaction effects of orientation and contrast). Thus, SRP across multiple stimuli may serve as a means of enhancing visual acuity and contrast sensitivity.
Application of TBS to dLGN induces thalamocortical LTP in the intact mouse
It has previously been shown that thalamocortical LTP can be induced in vivo at dLGN–visual cortical synapses in the rat (Heynen and Bear, 2001). Here, we show that LTP can be induced in the mouse in a similar fashion (Fig. 3 A). Responses recorded in layer 4 of binocular primary visual cortex (V1) underwent LTP after delivery of TBS (n = 20). Stability of the response was determined over a 30 min baseline period before induction of saturated LTP with three spaced bouts of TBS (5 min intertetanus interval). Responses were then evoked at the baseline interval of one every 30 s over a period of 1 h to assess the degree of LTP. Over the period of 50–60 min after the final TBS, field potential amplitude was 121 ± 6.6% of baseline, indicating significant LTP (paired t test for average value across last 10 min of baseline and 50–60 min after final tetanus; t (19) = 3.99; p < 0.001).
Thalamocortical LTP mimics SRP in potentiating VEP amplitude
We then tested whether thalamocortical LTP would impact the VEP itself. A subgroup of mice (n = 13) that underwent LTP as described above were allowed to recover overnight. VEPs were sampled the following day, evoked by 100% contrast, 0.05 cycle/° stimuli. In the hemisphere that had undergone LTP, VEPs were significantly greater amplitude 1 d after TBS (187.7 ± 23.8 μV) than in the control hemisphere that had not received TBS (100.6 ± 11.5 μV, n = 13; paired t test, t (12) = 4.17, p < 0.005), indicating that LTP had increased synaptic drive within layer 4 of visual cortex (Fig. 3 C). We also determined whether or not TBS would exert a sustained effect over VEP amplitude by presenting a novel orthogonal stimulus 5 d after LTP in the same mice. Once again, averaged VEP amplitude in the previously tetanized hemisphere (165.6 ± 12.9 μV) was significantly greater than in the control hemisphere (119.6 ± 8.0 μV, n = 12; paired t test, t (11) = 2.23, p < 0.05), although the difference was less than it had been 5 d beforehand. Therefore, LTP mimics SRP in enhancing the amplitude of the VEP and can do so over the course of at least 5 d.
LTP enhances spatial acuity and contrast sensitivity
Having established that VEP amplitude can be increased through thalamocortical LTP, we then tested whether it would have a general effect on visual acuity and contrast sensitivity. One day after inducing saturated LTP in one hemisphere, mice were presented with a range of novel, obliquely oriented stimuli across a range of spatial frequencies and contrasts. VEPs driven across 0.05, 0.15, 0.3, 0.45, 0.6, 0.8, and 1 cycle/° were significantly greater amplitude in the TBS hemisphere (Fig. 4 A) than in the control hemisphere (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,6) = 12.21, p < 0.005 for the interaction effects of orientation and spatial frequency). The overall enhancement of visual acuity through TBS stimulation is illustrated by normalizing VEP amplitude in the TBS hemisphere to control (Fig. 4 B). Even at as high a spatial frequency as 1 cycle/°, discernible VEPs emerged in the TBS hemisphere (31.6 ± 1.3 μV). These were 1.32 ± 0.08 times greater than evoked activity in the control hemisphere (24.0 ± 1.8 μV; paired t test, t (10) = 5.04, p < 0.001), which was equivalent to noise (23.9 ± 1.2 μV). Contrast sensitivity was also markedly enhanced (Fig. 4 C), resulting in a general increase in VEP amplitude in the tetanized hemisphere (Fig. 4 D) (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,6) = 7.89, p < 0.05 for the interaction effects of orientation and contrast). TBS stimulation of the dLGN, therefore, serves as a means to enhance spatial acuity and contrast sensitivity.
SRP is occluded by thalamocortical LTP
Having shown that LTP can potentiate VEPs, we then continued to test VEP amplitude in the same group of mice (n = 13) over the following 4 d, a typical period for the emergence of asymptotic SRP (Frenkel et al., 2006) (Fig. 5 A). On day 1, as reported above, VEPs recorded post-LTP were significantly greater amplitude in the TBS (183.4 ± 21.4 μV) than the control hemisphere (100.6 ± 11.0 μV; t (12) = 3.16; p < 0.01). By day 5, however, binocular VEPs evoked in the TBS hemisphere were no longer significantly different (253.9 ± 29.2 μV) from those in the control hemisphere (221.2 ± 22.6 μV; t (12) = 1.44; p = 0.18) (Fig. 5 B,C). Thus, the ceiling level of potentiation after induction of SRP and LTP combined was equivalent to that attained through SRP alone. Normalization of day 5 VEP amplitude to day 1 (Fig. 5 D) revealed that there was significantly less SRP in the tetanized hemisphere (146.3 ± 12.9% of day 1) than the untetanized hemisphere (230.1 ± 10.4% of day 1; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,3) = 18.37; p < 0.0005 for the interaction effects of hemisphere treatment over days). This finding demonstrates that thalamocortical LTP significantly occludes the subsequent expression of SRP.
Saturation of SRP to a single orientation occludes LTP in a stimulus-specific manner
Having established that thalamocortical LTP could both mimic and occlude SRP, we proceeded to test whether the reverse is also true; that SRP occludes LTP. A group of P30 mice (n = 13) were implanted and habituated as described previously, before undergoing SRP to a 0.05 cycle/°, 100% contrast stimulus over 5 d. On day 5, VEPs driven by the familiar X° stimulus and a novel X + 90° stimulus were recorded in both hemispheres. Afterward, mice underwent TBS stimulation of the dLGN in one hemisphere only. They were then allowed to recover overnight before again recording VEPs in awake mice, driven by either the familiar X° stimulus or a second novel X + 45° stimulus, to test the contribution of TBS-induced LTP to increases in VEPs driven by either stimulus (for experimental design, see Fig. 6 A,B).
Before LTP induction, VEPs driven by the familiar X° stimulus were of significantly greater amplitude (309.2 ± 21.0 μV; n = 13) than those driven by the novel X + 90° stimulus (183.2 ± 18.5 μV; one-way ANOVA, F (5,72) = 10.99; p < 0.0001; Fisher's post hoc test, p < 0.0001), reflecting SRP. A day later, after LTP, VEPs driven by the same X° stimulus were no greater in amplitude (302.6 ± 25.3 μV) than they had been the day before (Fisher's post hoc test, p = 0.83). However, the second novel X + 45° stimulus, otherwise equivalent in all properties to both the familiar and the novel X + 90° stimulus presented the day before, evoked significantly larger VEPs than the novel X + 90° stimulus had the previous day (242.8 ± 24.5 μV; Fisher's post hoc test, p < 0.005) (Fig. 6 C). If these data are normalized to pre-TBS baseline values (Fig. 6 D), the VEPs driven by the novel stimuli in the TBS hemisphere (134.0 ± 5.8%) show a significant potentiation after TBS (one-way ANOVA, F (2,36) = 14.46; p < 0.0001) relative to the VEPs recorded from the control hemisphere (106.9 ± 5.4 μV; Fisher's post hoc test, p < 0.001) and the VEPs driven by the familiar X° stimulus within the TBS hemisphere (97.3 ± 3.7 μV; Fisher's post hoc test, p < 0.0001). Therefore, LTP was limited to stimuli that had not previously been experienced, suggesting that SRP occludes LTP. The enhancing effect of TBS on VEPs driven by novel stimuli was limited to the TBS hemisphere as there was no similar increase in the magnitude of VEPs driven by the same novel stimuli in the control hemisphere before (152.5 ± 17.0 μV) and a day after TBS (161.3 ± 24.5 μV; Fisher's post hoc test, p = 0.77) (Fig. 6 C), demonstrating that TBS does not significantly enhance VEPs across hemispheres (Fig. 6 D). The data comprising the summary plots presented in Figure 6, C and D, are broken down into families of 13 lines for clarity (Fig. 6 E). Therefore, not only does thalamocortical LTP mimic and occlude experience-dependent plasticity (SRP), but the reverse is also true: SRP occludes thalamocortical LTP.
SRP maintenance is reversed by local inhibition of PKMζ
Maintenance of NMDA receptor-dependent LTP depends on PKMζ, an atypical, constitutively active isoform of the calcium-dependent kinase PKC (Pastalkova et al., 2006). Given the dependence of SRP on NMDA receptor activation (Frenkel et al., 2006) and the involvement of expression mechanisms shared with many forms of LTP, we tested whether local infusions of ZIP into the binocular zone of mouse primary visual cortex would erase SRP. Mice (P30) were implanted as described previously with the addition of bilateral infusion cannulae positioned proximal to the recording site (n = 9) (Fig. 7 A). After 2 d of recovery and habituation, an X° stimulus was presented repeatedly over 5 d, resulting in saturated SRP (248.5 ± 27.9% of averaged day 1 value). On day 5, a novel X + 90° stimulus was also presented, which elicited significantly smaller VEPs (135.8 ± 12.5%; t (8) = 3.89; p < 0.005) demonstrating the stimulus selectivity of SRP (Fig. 7 C,D).
X + 90° was then presented over 4 additional days, resulting in SRP equivalent to that observed in phase one (287.9 ± 24.9%; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,4) = 0.168; p = 0.95 for the interaction effects of orientation over days). On day 9, a third X + 45° stimulus was presented, interleaved with the X + 90° stimulus. As on day 5, VEPs evoked by the familiar stimulus (287.9 ± 24.9%) were greater amplitude than for the novel stimulus (147.3 ± 12.5%; t (8) = 7.781; p < 0.0001). After the sequential establishment of SRP to two different stimuli, 1 μl of ZIP (10 nmol/μl) was infused using a microinfusion pump over 10 min while the animal remained in the recording apparatus. One hour later, VEPs evoked by the novel X + 45° stimulus (140.9 ± 7.0%) were not significantly different in amplitude from those recorded 1 h earlier (147.3 ± 12.5%; t (8) = 0.94; p = 0.93), indicating no effect of the infusion or inhibitor on baseline VEPs. In contrast, VEPs evoked by the familiar X + 90° stimulus had dropped significantly in amplitude (150.2 ± 7.9% day 1 values, t (8) = 5.31; p < 0.001) and were now not significantly different from those evoked by the novel stimulus (t (8) = 1.10; p = 0.31), indicating that ZIP had erased recently saturated SRP (Fig. 7 C,D).
SRP can be reinstated after ZIP infusion but does not spontaneously recover
If SRP can be entirely erased without long-lasting insult to visual cortex, then it should be possible to reinduce SRP to the same stimulus. To test this possibility, both the previously familiar X + 90° and novel X + 45° stimuli were presented, interleaved, over the next 4 d. Four days after ZIP application, on overall day 13 of the experiment, VEPs evoked by X + 90° (232.6 ± 14.8%) were significantly greater in amplitude than when first recorded after ZIP application (150.2 ± 7.9%; t (8) = 4.38; p < 0.005). Therefore, we conclude that SRP could be reinduced after saturated SRP had previously been erased by ZIP. Moreover, the degree and rate of SRP reacquisition was equivalent to the original acquisition (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (1,4) = 1.36; p = 0.26 for interaction effect between ZIP treatment and day), suggesting that there were no savings after ZIP-mediated erasure of SRP (Fig. 7 C,D). VEPs evoked by the novel X + 45° stimulus on day 13 (218.9 ± 10.5%) had undergone an equivalent degree and rate of SRP to the other two stimuli (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F (4,16) = 1.75; p = 0.06 for interaction effect between orientation and day) demonstrating that the application of ZIP does not impact subsequent SRP.
VEPs driven by the original X° stimulus, which the animals had not seen over the previous week, were also interleaved among these recordings on day 13. These VEPs (142.8 ± 12.0%) were significantly lower amplitude than they had been on day 5 (248.5 ± 27.9%; t (8) = 4.51; p < 0.005). They were also significantly lower amplitude than those evoked by X + 90° (232.6 ± 14.8%) and X + 45° stimuli on the same day (218.9 ± 10.5%; F (2,27) = 19.36; p < 0.001; Fisher's post hoc test, p < 0.001 for comparisons between X° and either other orientation; p > 0.05 for comparison between X + 90° and X + 45°) after SRP, but equivalent in amplitude to those evoked by X + 90° (150.2 ± 7.9%) and X + 45° before SRP (147.3 ± 12.5%, respectively; F (2,27) = 0.98; p > 0.05). Under normal circumstances, SRP is known to be maintained for at least a week without a requirement for additional presentation of visual stimuli (Frenkel et al., 2006). Thus, SRP to the original X° stimulus had been erased on day 9 by ZIP application and showed no signs of spontaneous recovery in the intervening week (Fig. 7 C,D).
A final test of the specificity of ZIP action was provided at this stage by infusing 1 μl of a control peptide (10 nmol/μl) with the same constituent amino acids as ZIP but in scrambled order, as used in previous studies of ZIP action (Pastalkova et al., 2006; Shema et al., 2007). One hour after infusion of this peptide, VEPs evoked by the X° stimulus (118.7 ± 9.1%) were still significantly smaller than those evoked by the X + 90° (201.2 ± 14.8%) or X + 45° stimulus (186.8 ± 9.5%; F (2,27) = 17.10; p < 0.001; Fisher's post hoc test, p < 0.001 for comparisons between X° and either other orientation; p > 0.05 for comparison between X + 90° and X + 45°). Therefore, the actions of ZIP in erasing SRP are not a general result of infusion but specific to the inhibition of PKMζ. These data demonstrate a critical role for PKMζ in maintaining SRP (Fig. 7 C,D).
Discussion
We show that TBS of the dLGN can induce LTP of VEPs driven by a wide range of stimuli, including those with high spatial frequencies and low contrasts. This LTP occludes SRP, an experience-dependent form of plasticity and a surrogate measure of perceptual learning. Conversely, SRP prevents TBS from further potentiating the VEPs evoked by the experienced, but not novel, stimuli in vivo. This mutual occlusion suggests that LTP and SRP share core expression mechanisms. Consistent with this conclusion, we find that local inhibition of PKMζ, a constitutively active kinase known to maintain NMDA receptor-dependent LTP and memory (Serrano et al., 2005; Pastalkova et al., 2006), reverses SRP in the visual cortex.
TBS of the dLGN induces LTP at mature visual cortical synapses
In the slice preparation, it has been reported that somatosensory thalamocortical LTP cannot be induced past postnatal day 9 in rats (Crair and Malenka, 1995; Barth and Malenka, 2001). This window may close later in the visual system (Kirkwood et al., 1995; Wang and Daw, 2003), but it is nevertheless reported to be difficult to induce LTP at thalamocortical synapses in slices taken from animals around P30 or later. These findings contrast with the observations that substantial LTP can be induced in the visual (Heynen and Bear, 2001; Dringenberg et al., 2004; Kuo and Dringenberg, 2009), auditory (Hogsden and Dringenberg, 2009), and somatosensory cortices (Lee and Ebner, 1992) by stimulation of the thalamus or white matter in intact adult rodents. One possible explanation for this discrepancy has been that the inhibitory tone of the cortex is altered in ex vivo preparations. Indeed, in the cortical slice, LTP that is not normally present can emerge in the presence of a GABAA receptor antagonist (Artola and Singer, 1987; Kirkwood and Bear, 1994; Wang and Daw, 2003) (but see Jiang et al., 2007). Metaplasticity of the LTP threshold offers another potential explanation as we allow the mice 2 h of dark exposure before the application of an electrical tetanus to dLGN. Sensory deprivation before the delivery of thalamocortical tetanic stimulation has been shown to facilitate LTP induction (Kuo and Dringenberg, 2009).
Enhancement of thalamocortical transmission by high-frequency electrical stimulation can also reflect changes in the excitability of the thalamus, rather than synaptic LTP in the cortex (Hirata and Castro-Alamancos, 2006). However, the mutual occlusion of SRP and LTP indicate that, in substantial part, the potentiation of VEPs by TBS results from changes in the strength of cortical synapses rather than altered thalamic excitability, since SRP is disrupted by manipulations local to the visual cortex (Frenkel et al., 2006), including injection of the ZIP peptide.
Although the data indicate that cortical synapses are potentiated by thalamic TBS, it remains to be determined which synapses are involved. The VEP is a local field potential composed of events within a small population of synapses surrounding the recording electrode (Katzner et al., 2009). When the recording electrode is positioned in layer 4 of the cortex, a substantial component of the VEP represents direct thalamocortical synaptic events (Liu et al., 2007; Yoon et al., 2009; Khibnik et al., 2010). However, later components of the field potential likely reflect intracortical synaptic events. We cannot discount the possibility that LTP induced by dLGN stimulation may result from strengthening of intracortical synapses. Future studies will determine how plasticity at different populations of synapses within visual cortex contribute to the observed LTP of VEPs.
LTP mechanisms are sufficient to account for SRP
Several previous studies have demonstrated learning-related potentiation of synaptic strength in the hippocampus (Doyère et al., 1995; Sacchetti et al., 2002; Gruart et al., 2006; Whitlock et al., 2006) and the amygdala (Rogan et al., 1997; Rumpel et al., 2005; Schafe et al., 2005; Tye et al., 2008). Most pertinent to the study described here, learning can also induce LTP-like changes in synaptic strength in the neocortex (Rioult-Pedotti et al., 1998, 2000). In these latter studies, learning occluded ex vivo cortical LTP. However, mutual occlusion, whereby LTP also prevents subsequent learning, was not demonstrated. This leaves open the possibility that experience induces an alteration of LTP threshold through metaplasticity (Abraham, 2008). Previous studies in the hippocampus have demonstrated that LTP can disrupt subsequent learning (Barnes et al., 1994; Moser et al., 1998; Madroñal et al., 2007) (but see Jeffery and Morris, 1993). However, the experiments presented here are the first to show that occlusion effects are limited to previously activated synapses without preventing additional change at neighboring synapses. SRP in the mouse visual system provides a perfect opportunity to address this issue of specificity because the potentiating effect is limited to a clearly defined stimulus. If inputs onto the same population of postsynaptic cells are altered during SRP, as is suggested by the eye specificity of the phenomenon, then cell-wide metaplastic changes cannot account for the stimulus-specific occlusion of LTP by SRP. We therefore conclude that SRP uses the same core mechanisms to potentiate synapses in the visual cortex as support potentiation by thalamocortical LTP.
We find that SRP is disrupted by the ZIP peptide that inhibits PKMζ. ZIP erases LTP in the hippocampus (Serrano et al., 2005; Pastalkova et al., 2006) and memory of various forms in the hippocampus (Pastalkova et al., 2006; Serrano et al., 2008; Hardt et al., 2010), amygdala (Serrano et al., 2008), and neocortex (Shema et al., 2007, 2009). The effect of ZIP on SRP appears specific to the inhibition of PKMζ because a scrambled control does not significantly affect already established SRP. Crucially, we demonstrate that SRP can reemerge to a familiar stimulus at the same rate as it emerges for the first time to a novel stimulus. Moreover, we demonstrate that no spontaneous recovery of synaptic potentiation occurs after initial erasure by ZIP and that SRP stays reversed for at least 1 week. These findings are consistent with SRP being maintained in the visual cortex using mechanisms similar to LTP in CA1 (Ling et al., 2002; Serrano et al., 2005) and the dentate gyrus (Pastalkova et al., 2006). We know from previous work that SRP is dependent on the NMDA receptor for induction, and the insertion of GluR1 AMPA receptor subunits for expression (Frenkel et al., 2006). These are mechanisms shared with the canonical form of CA1 LTP and we now extend the list of commonalities by demonstrating the involvement of PKMζ in the maintenance of SRP.
Given the mutual occlusion that exists between the two phenomena, it is fair to describe SRP as a biomarker of LTP. SRP therefore could serve as a naturalistic means of assessing synaptic plasticity. Relatively unchallenging electrophysiological methods are required to record and analyze SRP and it is an extremely reliable phenomenon. It seems likely that SRP is a general phenomenon in sensory cortex because similar effects have been observed in other animal species (Clapp et al., 2006; Hua et al., 2010), including humans (Clapp et al., 2005b; McNair et al., 2006; Ross et al., 2008). Thus, SRP provides a means to interrogate the integrity of neocortical LTP mechanisms, and could be used, for example, to assess the effectiveness of experimental therapeutic interventions in animals and humans.
Sensory gains through SRP and LTP
It has been suggested that SRP reveals a core mechanism for perceptual learning in the visual system (Frenkel et al., 2006). Perceptual learning is of great interest not just in the context of basic research but also from a clinical perspective. Amblyopia is a disorder resulting from deprivation of vision early in life that afflicts as many as 1–2% of people in the world (Webber and Wood, 2005). Disruption of cortical development through deprivation cannot be rescued after ∼8 years of age in humans even if visual function is entirely recovered, because plasticity mechanisms are altered in adulthood. Perceptual learning can occur in adults and has been used to recover visual function in amblyopics (Li et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2008; Polat et al., 2009).
One issue with the use of perceptual learning to recover visual function is that it is so specific to individual stimuli that a complex and lengthy training schedule would be required to significantly recover visual function. Our results here demonstrate that dLGN TBS-induced LTP can dramatically enhance both visual acuity and contrast sensitivity, at least at the level of the VEP, across a wide range of stimulus orientations. Recent work has suggested that it is possible to noninvasively induce LTP in primary sensory cortices of rats (Uhlrich et al., 2005; Manning et al., 2007) and humans (Clapp et al., 2005a,b) using photic or auditory tetani. Another alternative may be the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation, either on its own or in conjunction with visual stimulation, to noninvasively deliver a potentiating tetanus (for review, see Cooke and Bliss, 2006). Our results suggest that, should we identify a successful noninvasive technique in the future, a broader-based LTP of synaptic transmission in the visual cortex may be sufficient to recover visual function in an immediate and general fashion in amblyopics.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute and the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (M.F.B.), NIH–National Eye Institute Grant R01 EY018323-01. We thank Arnold Heynen for helpful advice and discussions, and Erik Sklar, Kathleen Oram, and Suzanne Meagher for assistance.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Mark F. Bear, The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 46-3301, 43 Vassar Street, Cambridge, MA 02139. mbear{at}mit.edu
References
- Abraham, 2008.↵
- Artola and Singer, 1987.↵
- Barnes et al., 1994.↵
- Barth and Malenka, 2001.↵
- Bliss and Lømo, 1973.↵
- Chen et al., 2008.↵
- Clapp et al., 2005a.↵
- Clapp et al., 2005b.↵
- Clapp et al., 2006.↵
- Cooke and Bliss, 2006.↵
- Crair and Malenka, 1995.↵
- Doyère et al., 1995.↵
- Dringenberg et al., 2004.↵
- Fahle and Morgan, 1996.↵
- Frenkel and Bear, 2004.↵
- Frenkel et al., 2006.↵
- Furmanski et al., 2004.↵
- Gilbert et al., 2001.↵
- Gruart et al., 2006.↵
- Hardt et al., 2010.↵
- Heynen and Bear, 2001.↵
- Hirata and Castro-Alamancos, 2006.↵
- Hogsden and Dringenberg, 2009.↵
- Hua et al., 2010.↵
- Jeffery and Morris, 1993.↵
- Jiang et al., 2007.↵
- Karni and Bertini, 1997.↵
- Katzner et al., 2009.↵
- Khibnik et al., 2010.↵
- Kirkwood and Bear, 1994.↵
- Kirkwood et al., 1995.↵
- Kuo and Dringenberg, 2009.↵
- Lee and Ebner, 1992.↵
- Li et al., 2007.↵
- Ling et al., 2002.↵
- Liu et al., 2007.↵
- Madroñal et al., 2007.↵
- Malenka and Bear, 2004.↵
- Manning et al., 2007.↵
- Martin et al., 2000.↵
- McNair et al., 2006.↵
- Moser et al., 1998.↵
- Neves et al., 2008.↵
- Pastalkova et al., 2006.↵
- Poggio et al., 1992.↵
- Polat et al., 2009.↵
- Rioult-Pedotti et al., 1998.↵
- Rioult-Pedotti et al., 2000.↵
- Rogan et al., 1997.↵
- Ross et al., 2008.↵
- Rumpel et al., 2005.↵
- Sacchetti et al., 2002.↵
- Sawtell et al., 2003.↵
- Schafe et al., 2005.↵
- Serrano et al., 2005.↵
- Serrano et al., 2008.↵
- Shema et al., 2007.↵
- Shema et al., 2009.↵
- Tye et al., 2008.↵
- Uhlrich et al., 2005.↵
- Wang and Daw, 2003.↵
- Webber and Wood, 2005.↵
- Whitlock et al., 2006.↵
- Yoon et al., 2009.↵