Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

More Is Less: A Disinhibited Prefrontal Cortex Impairs Cognitive Flexibility

Aaron J. Gruber, Gwendolyn G. Calhoon, Igor Shusterman, Geoffrey Schoenbaum, Matthew R. Roesch and Patricio O'Donnell
Journal of Neuroscience 15 December 2010, 30 (50) 17102-17110; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4623-10.2010
Aaron J. Gruber
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gwendolyn G. Calhoon
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Igor Shusterman
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Geoffrey Schoenbaum
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Matthew R. Roesch
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Patricio O'Donnell
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Size-discounting task. Sketch of task apparatus (top) and time course (bottom) of task events for the two blocks used during recordings sessions.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    NVHL rats can learn the task. A, B, Number of trials (A) and number of response errors (B) for NVHL (red) and sham (black) rats during the initial training sessions in which only the two forced-choice trials were presented. Delays between task events were increased across trials to the values used in the full task during recording sessions. There were no differences in the number of trials and response errors between groups (ANOVA, p > 0.05, n = 11). C, Illustration of histologically verified damage to the ventral hippocampus in adult NVHL rats indicated on a standard atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2005). Gray and dark regions show the largest and smallest extent, respectively, of damage across rats.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Loss of field-potential modulation in NVHL rats during the task. A, Histologically identified electrode tracks in the medial PFC for control and NVHL rats. Prelimbic (PL) and infralimbic (IL) cortical areas are labeled according to a standard atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2005). B, Task-averaged time-frequency spectrograms of field potentials reveal reduced power for NVHL rats (bottom; n = 6) and sham controls (top; n = 5). Data are aligned to odor port entry, normalized to the 0.5 s interval before port entry, and include only field potentials recorded during correct trials. C, Loss of field potential (FP) power in NVHL rats is observed across several frequency bands. Summed field potential power within conventional frequency bands for NVHL (red) and control (gray) rats shows reduced modulation in NVHL rats during odor presentation (yellow boxes, p < 0.01 by Bonferroni corrected t test indicated by *) for all frequency bands <30 Hz. D, Field potential power spectra for NVHL (red) and control (gray) rats during baseline and odor presentation epochs, revealing less oscillation amplitude in NVHL rats in gamma and other frequency ranges (p < 0.05 by jackknifed U-statistic indicated by a blue line below the plot). Shaded regions represent mean ± SEM. n.s., Not significant.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Single-unit responses during the task. A, Raster plot and perievent time histogram for a putative pyramidal neuron recorded from a control rat performing the decision-making task. Colored bars indicate odor cue (green, free choice; red, forced right; blue, forced left) and triangles indicate reward delivery (red, right; blue, left). B, Bar plots showing the percentage of units from control (black; n = 86) and NVHL (red; n = 159) rats responding with significant increases of firing during each region of interest labeled above the bars. †Differences between the number of units, computed by χ2. C, Group averages of raw firing rates (top) and z scores (bottom) for PFC single units that showed a significant increase in activity during task epochs for NVHL (dashed red line) and control (solid black line) rats. Data for each epoch are aligned to the task event indicated by labels above vertical lines. Baseline firing was not different between the two groups; however, NVHL rats showed increased firing during the task. *p < 0.05 by t test of mean cell firing during regions of interest marked by blue bars above plots. Lines indicate mean and shaded regions represent ±SEM.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Bar plot showing the proportion of units that had a dependence on reward size for sham (black) and NVHL (gray) rats over multiple task epochs. Reward dependence was assessed by computing firing in each epoch for large and small reward trials to the preferred response direction for each unit, and testing for significant differences of firing in these conditions using the Wilcoxon rank sum test. *p < 0.05 by χ2 test between groups for each epoch.

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Behavioral deficits in NVHL rats. A, Plot of group-averaged response (resp.) choices during sequential free-choice trials within behavioral blocks, showing that both groups of rats develop a bias against the well giving small rewards as blocks progress, but NVHL rats (gray, n = 6) showed less bias than control rats (black, n = 5). B, Bar plot of response errors on forced-choice trials showing that rats made more response errors on forced trials for small rewards and that NVHL rats made more response errors than controls. C, Bar plot of reaction times showing that rats had faster reaction times for big rewards than small rewards, and NVHL rats had faster reaction times than controls for both reward sizes. D, Bar plot of premature exits from the odor port showing that NVHL rats had a nonstatistically significant trend to make more premature exits from the odor port. E, Bar plot of mean change in responses away from the discounted reward well on the first five free-choice trials in each block compared with the preblock bias, showing response perseveration in NVHL rats on the second block compared with controls. For all plots, significance at the 95% level by main effects of two-way ANOVA (†), Tukey–Kramer post hoc test (*) and t test (**) are indicated. Error bars indicate ±SEM. F, Changes in field-potential power during odor presentation correlate with performance in the task. Error rate during forced-choice trails plotted against average power during odor presentation normalized by the preodor value for the 1–30 Hz frequency band in control rats. The line was generated by linear regression of data points. n.s., Not significant.

  • Figure 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 7.

    Pharmacological modulation of task performance in NVHL and naive rats. A, Schema illustrating the modified task used for pharmacology experiments. B, C, Responding for small rewards on free-choice trials and error rate when forced to respond for small rewards were reduced in NVHL rats (n = 4; B) but not sham rats (n = 4; C) following systemic administration of the mGluR2/3 agonist LY379268, showing that reducing glutamate transmission improves behavior. D, Systemic administration of the D2 antagonist eticlopride (Etic) impaired behavior in naive rats (n = 5). Error rate when forced to respond for small rewards and premature response rate increased with eticlopride compared with saline (SAL) treatment in the same naive rats. E, The same treatment did not alter behavior in NVHL rats (n = 5). *p < 0.05 by paired t test. n.s., Not significant.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Number of neurons that increased, decreased, or did not change their rate during behaviorally relevant task epochs

    Odor deliveryExit odor portReward wellExit reward well
    NVHL (n = 156) (Increased, decreased, did not change)37,* 93, 2640,* 91, 2549, 70,* 37*46, 49,* 61*
    Control (n = 87) (Increased, decreased, did not change)12, 52, 2311, 53, 2320, 22, 4523, 16, 48
    • ↵*Significant differences of between-group frequencies (χ2; p < 0.05).

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 30 (50)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 30, Issue 50
15 Dec 2010
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
  • 2010 Author and Subject Indices
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
More Is Less: A Disinhibited Prefrontal Cortex Impairs Cognitive Flexibility
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
More Is Less: A Disinhibited Prefrontal Cortex Impairs Cognitive Flexibility
Aaron J. Gruber, Gwendolyn G. Calhoon, Igor Shusterman, Geoffrey Schoenbaum, Matthew R. Roesch, Patricio O'Donnell
Journal of Neuroscience 15 December 2010, 30 (50) 17102-17110; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4623-10.2010

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
More Is Less: A Disinhibited Prefrontal Cortex Impairs Cognitive Flexibility
Aaron J. Gruber, Gwendolyn G. Calhoon, Igor Shusterman, Geoffrey Schoenbaum, Matthew R. Roesch, Patricio O'Donnell
Journal of Neuroscience 15 December 2010, 30 (50) 17102-17110; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4623-10.2010
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Memory Retrieval Has a Dynamic Influence on the Maintenance Mechanisms That Are Sensitive to ζ-Inhibitory Peptide (ZIP)
  • Neurophysiological Evidence for a Cortical Contribution to the Wakefulness-Related Drive to Breathe Explaining Hypocapnia-Resistant Ventilation in Humans
  • Monomeric Alpha-Synuclein Exerts a Physiological Role on Brain ATP Synthase
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
  • Generation of Intensity Selectivity by Differential Synaptic Tuning: Fast-Saturating Excitation But Slow-Saturating Inhibition
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.