Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Neuroligin-1 Deletion Results in Impaired Spatial Memory and Increased Repetitive Behavior

Jacqueline Blundell, Cory A. Blaiss, Mark R. Etherton, Felipe Espinosa, Katsuhiko Tabuchi, Christopher Walz, Marc F. Bolliger, Thomas C. Südhof and Craig M. Powell
Journal of Neuroscience 10 February 2010, 30 (6) 2115-2129; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4517-09.2010
Jacqueline Blundell
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Cory A. Blaiss
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mark R. Etherton
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Felipe Espinosa
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Katsuhiko Tabuchi
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christopher Walz
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Marc F. Bolliger
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thomas C. Südhof
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Craig M. Powell
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Neuroligins (NLs) are a family of neural cell-adhesion molecules that are involved in excitatory/inhibitory synapse specification. Multiple members of the NL family (including NL1) and their binding partners have been linked to cases of human autism and mental retardation. We have now characterized NL1-deficient mice in autism- and mental retardation-relevant behavioral tasks. NL1 knock-out (KO) mice display deficits in spatial learning and memory that correlate with impaired hippocampal long-term potentiation. In addition, NL1 KO mice exhibit a dramatic increase in repetitive, stereotyped grooming behavior, a potential autism-relevant abnormality. This repetitive grooming abnormality in NL1 KO mice is associated with a reduced NMDA/AMPA ratio at corticostriatal synapses. Interestingly, we further demonstrate that the increased repetitive grooming phenotype can be rescued in adult mice by administration of the NMDA receptor partial coagonist d-cycloserine. Broadly, these data are consistent with a role of synaptic cell-adhesion molecules in general, and NL1 in particular, in autism and implicate reduced excitatory synaptic transmission as a potential mechanism and treatment target for repetitive behavioral abnormalities.

Introduction

Neuroligins (NLs) are a family of neuronal postsynaptic cell adhesion molecules (Ichtchenko et al., 1995, 1996). NLs are differentially localized to excitatory and inhibitory synapses (Ichtchenko et al., 1995, 1996; Song et al., 1999; Graf et al., 2004; Varoqueaux et al., 2004; Budreck and Scheiffele, 2007). Neuroligin-1 (NL1) is enriched preferentially at excitatory synapses (Song et al., 1999), neuroligin-2 (NL2) is enriched at inhibitory synapses (Graf et al., 2004; Varoqueaux et al., 2004), and neuroligin-3 (NL3) appears to be present at both (Budreck and Scheiffele, 2007). Expression of NLs in vitro has been shown to induce presynaptic specializations and increase synaptic density (Scheiffele et al., 2000; Dean et al., 2003; Graf et al., 2004; Prange et al., 2004; Boucard et al., 2005; Chih et al., 2005; Chubykin et al., 2005, 2007; Levinson et al., 2005; Nam and Chen, 2005). However, the synapse-increasing activities of NLs in culture do not reflect a requirement for NLs in initial synapse formation in vivo but rather a role of NLs in synapse specification and modulation (Varoqueaux et al., 2006; Chubykin et al., 2007).

Understanding NL function in vivo is not only critical for a basic understanding of synapse function but is also relevant to human autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). Indeed, mutations in members of the NL family and its associated binding partners, including the NL1 binding partners neurexin-1 and shank3, have been implicated in human autism and mental retardation (Jamain et al., 2003; Zoghbi, 2003; Chih et al., 2004; Comoletti et al., 2004; Laumonnier et al., 2004; Yan et al., 2005; Feng et al., 2006; Durand et al., 2007; Szatmari et al., 2007; Yan et al., 2008a,b). Chromosomal rearrangements in regions that harbor the NL1 and NL2 genes and single nucleotide polymorphisms in the gene encoding NL1 have been associated directly with human ASDs (Konstantareas and Homatidis, 1999; Zoghbi, 2003; Yan et al., 2004; Ylisaukko-oja et al., 2005; Südhof, 2008). More recently, a genome-wide copy number variation analysis also implicated NL1 among several candidate genes in ASD susceptibility (Glessner et al., 2009), further suggesting a direct link between NL1 and human autism.

In light of the link between NLs and autism, we predicted that NL1 knock-out (KO) mice might exhibit autism- or mental retardation-relevant behavioral abnormalities. Consistent with our hypothesis, NL1 KO mice displayed deficits in hippocampus-dependent spatial memory along with impaired hippocampal long-term potentiation. NL1 KO mice also exhibited increased repetitive grooming behavior, which may be relevant to the increased repetitive behavior seen in autism (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), along with a reduced NMDA/AMPA ratio at corticostriatal synapses. Furthermore, we demonstrate that the autism-related repetitive grooming phenotype can be rescued by systemic d-cycloserine (DCS) in adult mice. Overall, these data are consistent with the hypothesis that NL1 dysfunction can lead to autism- and mental retardation-related behavioral abnormalities in part via alteration of NMDA receptor (NMDAR) function.

Materials and Methods

Genetic manipulations

NL1 KO mice were generated as described previously (Varoqueaux et al., 2006). To reduce genetic and experimental variability, the NL1 mice studied were sex-matched, littermate products of heterozygous mating on a hybrid 129S6/SvEvTac/C57BL/6J background. In all studies, experimenters were blind to genotype of the animals.

Western blot

Protein compositions were determined by immunoblotting on brain tissues homogenized in PBS, 10 mm EDTA, and proteinase inhibitors from four pairs of P40 littermate mice per genotype. Proteins (40 μg) were loaded per lane and blotted with antibodies for synaptic proteins and internal controls (β-actin or GDP dissociation inhibitor). Blots were reacted with 125I-labeled secondary antibodies, followed by PhosphoImager (STORM 860; GE Healthcare) detection.

Morphological analyses

NL1 KO and wild-type (WT) littermate control mice were anesthetized and perfusion fixed with 4% fresh paraformaldehyde and cryoprotected with 30% sucrose. Sections (30 μm) were blocked with 3% goat serum/0.3% Triton X-100 in PBS and incubated with anti-synaptophysin monoclonal antibody (Millipore Corporation), anti-vesicular glutamate transporter 1 (vGlut1) monoclonal antibody (Synaptic Systems), and/or anti-vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT) polyclonal antibody (Millipore Corporation) overnight at 4°C, followed by incubation with Alexa Fluor 488 or 633 goat anti-mouse IgG (Invitrogen). Sections were transferred onto SuperFrost slides and mounted under glass coverslips with Vectashield with 4′,6′-diamidino-2-phenylindole (Vector Laboratories). Sections of the CA1 and CA3 subfields of the hippocampus were imaged with a Leica TCS2 laser-scanning confocal microscope at 63× and magnified fivefold. For each experimental series, all images were acquired with identical settings for laser power, photomultiplier gain, and offset with a pinhole diameter. Images were imported into NIH ImageJ software, and synaptic densities and sizes were analyzed under fixed thresholds across all slides. Thresholds were chosen within the range that allowed outlining as many immunopositive puncta as possible throughout all images. The number and size of puncta were detected using the “analyze particle” module of the program. The average number and size of puncta were normalized with data from wild type to determine synaptic density and size, respectively. Statistical significance was determined by Student's t test. All of the data shown are means ± SEM.

Electrophysiological techniques

Hippocampus.

Transverse hippocampal slices were prepared from 4- to 8-week- old mice as described previously (Volk et al., 2007). In brief, mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and decapitated, and the brain was quickly isolated into ice-cold dissecting solution (in mm: 222 sucrose, 11 glucose, 26 NaHCO3, 1 NaH2PO4, 3 KCl, 7 MgCl2, and 0.5 CaCl2). Slices, 400 μm thick, were made using a Leica VT1200s and allowed to recover for at least 1.5 h in artificial CSF (ACSF) [in mm: 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 1.3 MgSO4 26 NaHCO3, 25 d-glucose, and 2.5 CaCl2, pH 7.4 (saturated with 95%O2/5%CO2)] before recording. The slices were then placed in a submerged chamber at 28–30°C and allowed to equilibrate for 15 min before recording. Extracellular recordings were performed in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Recording electrodes (3–5 MΩ) were filled with ACSF and placed in the stratum radiatum of area CA1. Basal field EPSPs (fEPSPs) were evoked at 0.067 Hz with a concentric bipolar electrode placed in the stratum radiatum. Input–output curves were constructed from the average of 10 traces at each stimulus intensity with the amplitude of the presynaptic fiber volley measured relative to the slope of the fEPSP.

For paired-pulse and long-term potentiation (LTP) protocols, stimulus strengths were adjusted to produce responses 40% of maximum fEPSP. The LTP protocol used was a theta burst stimulation (TBS) pattern consisting of five bursts of four pulses at 100 Hz with an interburst interval of 0.2 s. Baseline and post-induction responses were sampled at 0.033 Hz. Baseline recordings for LTP experiments were performed for >10 min, and slices were rejected if baseline was unstable.

Corticostriatal synapses.

Horizontal-oblique slices were prepared and recordings were performed as described previously (Ding et al., 2008). Briefly, wild-type and NL1 KO littermate mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and decapitated, and the brain was quickly isolated into dissecting solution. Slices 350–400 μm thick were prepared at ice-cold temperature from juvenile mice at postnatal day 15 (P15) to P38. Average age was 26.9 ± 1.8 d for wild-type and 26.1 ± 1.6 d for NL1 KO. The dissecting solution contained the following (in mm): 54 NaCl, 100 sucrose, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 10 Mg Cl2, 26 NaHCO3, 10 dextrose, and 0.5 CaCl2. The bathing solution contained the following (in mm): 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 1 MgCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 10 dextrose, and 2 CaCl2 (saturated with 95% O2/5% CO2). Slices were incubated in the bathing solution at 32°C for 1 h. Afterward, slices were kept at room temperature until transferred to a submersion-type recording chamber. Whole-cell patch recordings on medium spiny neurons were done using micropipettes (3–5 MΩ) made from 1.1/1.5 mm borosilicate glass (Sutter Instruments). Recording pipettes were filled with the following solution (in mm): 117 Cs-methanesulphonate, 15 CsCl, 8 NaCl, 10 tetraethylammonium-Cl, QX-314 [N-(2,6-dimethylphenylcarbamoylmethyl)triethylammonium chloride], 0.2 EGTA, 2 ATP-Mg, 0.3 GTP, and 10 HEPES-CsOH, pH 7.25 (290–295 mOsm). A theoretical junction potential of 12, calculated using the corresponding function in Clampfit, was used to correct voltages post hoc. Access resistance was frequently checked to be <25 MΩ and stable (<20% of variability). Recordings were obtained using the 700B Multiclamp amplifier (Molecular Devices), and neurons were visualized using a Carl Zeiss Axioexaminer D1 scope equipped with infrared differential interference contrast visualization through microscope and a CCD camera and DOT optics.

EPSCs were evoked by stimulating corticostriatal projections with 0.2 ms current injections (commonly between 0.1 and 1 mA) at the boundary between neocortical layer VI and the corpus callosum by using concentric bipolar electrodes (FHC Inc.) and an A365 battery-driven stimulus isolator (WPI). Evoked NMDAR/AMPAR ratios were determined using standard, published methods (Myme et al., 2003). In the presence of picrotoxin, AMPAR currents are measured at the peak and at a voltage of −80 mV, at which most NMDAR currents are expected to be blocked by Mg2+. In the same cell, NMDAR currents are measured in a 2 ms window 50 ms after spike onset at a voltage of +40 mV.

All responses were digitized at 10 kHz and filtered at 1 kHz. Data were analyzed offline using pClamp and Microsoft Excel. Student's t test was used to evaluate significance of all analyses. Experimenters were blind to genotype.

Behavioral overview

Mice were age/sex-matched littermate progeny of heterozygous/heterozygous (NL1 KO) matings tested behaviorally in four groups. Experimenters were blind to genotype. The first cohort of mice for behavioral studies included 23 NL1 KO littermate pairs (total of 46 mice), except when noted. For shock threshold, nesting, and visible water maze, there were 11 littermate pairs (22 mice total), and, for the nonsocial test of olfaction, there were 10 littermate pairs (20 mice total), because some of the mice were removed from the original cohort for histological studies. Less stressful behaviors were tested first, with more stressful procedures at the end. The order of tests for the first cohort of mice was as follows: locomotor, dark/light box, open field, elevated plus maze, accelerating rotarod, social interaction with a juvenile, social learning, social versus inanimate preference test, preference for social novelty test, social interaction with an adult caged conspecific, fear conditioning, Morris water maze, prepulse inhibition, startle amplitude, nesting, olfaction for a nonsocial stimulus (peanut butter cookie), and shock threshold. A second cohort of mice (22 littermate pairs, 44 mice total) was tested for grooming, interaction with a social smell, and marble burying. A third cohort of mice (17 littermate pairs, 34 mice total) was combined with the second cohort to test grooming behavior after administration of d-cycloserine (or vehicle). A fourth cohort of mice (21 littermate pairs, 42 mice total) was reexamined in anxiety-related tasks in a different order (elevated plus maze, dark/light, open field) and then tested for hotplate sensitivity.

The dark/light, open field, and elevated plus mazes were used as measures of anxiety-like behavior. Social interaction with a juvenile, social learning, the social versus inanimate preference test, the preference for social novelty test, and social interaction with an adult caged conspecific were all used as measures of social behavior, and nesting behavior was tested because of its association with social and affiliative behaviors. Interaction with a social smell and olfaction for a nonsocial stimulus (peanut butter cookie) were both used as controls to test for normal olfactory function. Locomotor activity, startle amplitude, prepulse inhibition, shock threshold, and hotplate sensitivity were tested to examine basic neurologic function, including sensitivity to auditory and sensory stimuli. Grooming was observed to examine repetitive, stereotyped behavior. The Morris water maze was conducted to examine spatial learning and memory, and fear conditioning was conducted to examine fear learning and memory.

Within each cohort, all mice ranged from 2 to 8 months of age during the behavioral testing. Mice were moved within the animal facility to the testing room and allowed to habituate to the new location for at least 1 h before behavioral testing. All statistical analyses were conducted using Statistica software (StatSoft), and significance was taken as p < 0.05 for all experiments.

Morris water maze

The Morris water maze and visible platform tests were performed essentially as described previously (Powell et al., 2004) except a probe trial was performed only on day 12. Briefly, a 4-feet-diameter, white, plastic, circular pool was filled to a depth of 13 inches with 22 ± 1°C water made opaque with gothic white, nontoxic, liquid tempera paint in a room with prominent extramaze cues. Mice were placed in one of four starting locations facing the pool wall and allowed to swim until they found a 10-inch-diameter, white platform submerged by 1 cm, or until a maximum of 60 s had elapsed. On finding the platform, mice remained on the platform for 15 s before being removed to the home cage. If mice did not find the platform within 60 s, they were guided to the platform by the experimenter where they remained for 15 s before being removed to the home cage. Latency to reach the platform, distance traveled to reach the platform, swim speed, and percentage thigmotaxis (time spent near the wall of the pool) were measured using automated video-tracking software from Noldus (Ethovision 2.3.19). Mice were trained with 4 trials/d with an intertrial interval of 1–1.5 min for 11 consecutive days between 8:00 A.M. and 1:00 P.M. A probe trial (free swim with the submerged platform removed) was performed as the first trial of the day on day 12. The percentage time spent in the target quadrant and the number of platform location crossings was calculated using Ethovision 2.3.19. Percentage time spent in all quadrants, latency to platform, distance to platform, swim speed, and percentage thigmotaxis were analyzed with a three-way mixed ANOVA. Training in the visible water maze task was conducted in the same manner as the main Morris water maze except that a visible cue (black foam cube) was placed on top of the platform, mice were placed in the same location for each trial, and the platform was moved to a new, random location for each trial. Mice were trained with 6 trials/d for 2 consecutive days, and the latency to reach the visible platform was analyzed with a three-way mixed ANOVA.

Social behavior

Direct social interaction with a juvenile took place in a novel, empty, clear, plastic mouse cage under red light, as described previously (Kwon et al., 2006; Tabuchi et al., 2007). After a 15 min habituation in the dark, the experimental and target mice were placed in the neutral cage for 2 min and allowed to directly interact. Time spent interacting with the juvenile was scored by an observer blind to genotype. Social learning was assessed 3 d later by allowing mice to interact with the same juvenile for an additional 2 min. Again, time spent interacting with the juvenile was scored. Data were analyzed with a three-way mixed ANOVA.

Caged adult social interaction tests were performed in a 48 × 48 cm2 white plastic arena under red light using a 6.0 × 9.5 cm wire mesh rectangular cage containing an unfamiliar adult mouse, allowing olfactory, visual, and minimal tactile interaction (Kwon et al., 2006; Tabuchi et al., 2007). Mice were first placed in the arena for 5 min with an empty wire mesh cage. Then mice were allowed to interact with a novel caged social target for another 5 min. Time spent in the interaction zone was obtained using Noldus software (Ethovision 2.3.19). The box was wiped with 70% ethanol and air dried between mice. Data were analyzed with a three-way mixed ANOVA.

Social versus inanimate preference and preference for social novelty analyses were modified from previous descriptions (Moy et al., 2004; Nadler et al., 2004) as described in detail previously (Kwon et al., 2006; Tabuchi et al., 2007). Data were analyzed with a three-way mixed ANOVA. Nesting behavior was performed as described previously (Lijam et al., 1997) and analyzed with a three-way mixed ANOVA.

Interaction with a social smell was performed similar to interaction with a caged adult. Initially, mice were placed in a 48 × 48 cm2 white plastic arena for 5 min with a slide containing a nonsocial smell (rubbed with distilled water). Immediately after, mice were allowed to interact with a slide containing a “social” smell (slide rubbed on the anogenital region of an unfamiliar C57BL/6J WT mouse) for another 5 min. Time in the interaction zone was obtained using Noldus software (Ethovision 2.3.19). The box was wiped with 70% ethanol and air dried between mice. Data were analyzed with a three-way mixed ANOVA.

Olfaction for a nonsocial stimulus was measured as described previously (Moretti et al., 2005) except animals were food deprived overnight before the test and a peanut butter cookie was used as the olfactory treat. Data were analyzed with a two-way ANOVA.

Grooming

Mice were habituated to a novel home cage for 10 min. Immediately thereafter, total time spent grooming the face, head, or body was measured for 10 min. Grooming behavior was analyzed using a two-way ANOVA.

Marble burying

Similar to a previous description (Deacon, 2006), empty home cages were filled with bedding up to 5 cm from the cage floor, and 20 black marbles were placed evenly throughout the cage. Mice were allowed to freely explore the cage (and marbles) for 30 min, and, afterward, the number of successfully buried marbles was counted. A marble was defined as “buried” when <25% of the marble was visible. Data were analyzed with a two-way ANOVA.

Fear conditioning

Cued and contextual fear conditioning was performed essentially as described previously (Powell et al., 2004). Briefly, mice were habituated to the shock context for 2 min, during which the level of “pretraining” freezing was measured. Then, a 30 s, 90 dB tone coterminating with a 2 s, 0.5 mA footshock was delivered twice with a 1 min interstimulus interval. Mice remained in the context for 2 min before returning to their home cage. Freezing behavior (motionless except respirations) was monitored at 10 s intervals by an observer blind to genotype. To test for contextual learning 24 h after training, mice were placed into the same training context for 5 min and scored for freezing behavior every 10 s. To assess cue-dependent fear conditioning, mice were placed in a novel environment 3 h after the context test. Freezing behavior was assessed during a 3 min baseline, followed by a 3 min presentation of the tone. Cue-dependent fear conditioning was determined by subtracting baseline freezing from freezing during the tone. Both cued and contextual fear conditioning data were analyzed with a two-way ANOVA.

Hotplate sensitivity

Mice were placed on a black, anodized, constant-temperature plate of 52°C (IITC model 39 hotplate) covered with a Plexiglas enclosure. Latency to lick or shake the hindpaw was measured, and mice were removed after the first lick or shake of a hindpaw or after 30 s if no response was elicited. The plate was cleaned with water between mice and allowed to return to baseline temperature. Data were analyzed with a two-way ANOVA.

Footshock sensitivity

Pain sensitivity during footshock was measured as described previously (Powell et al., 2004). Briefly, mice were placed in a conditioning chamber and allowed to habituate for 2 min. Then, a series of 2 s footshocks was delivered; the initial shock was delivered at 0.05 mA, and the current increased by 0.05 mA every trial with a 20 s intertrial interval. The current required to elicit flinching, jumping, and vocalizing was recorded by an observer. Data were analyzed with a two-way ANOVA.

Grooming plus d-cycloserine or vehicle

The same protocol for assessing grooming behavior was used as described above except that half the mice (NL1 KO and WT) were injected with DCS (20 mg/kg, i.p.; Sigma) and the other half were injected with vehicle (saline) 20 min before placement in the novel home cage. Treatment groups were counterbalanced for the total time spent grooming during a baseline observation. Data were analyzed with a three-way ANOVA.

Results

NL1 deletion results in a decrease in neurexin levels

To determine whether NL1 deletion causes changes in other synaptic proteins, we examined protein levels of 26 presynaptic and postsynaptic proteins in brains of NL1 KO and littermate control mice by Western blot (Table 1). For the majority of proteins examined, these experiments revealed only subtle changes in the gross levels of synaptic markers as a result of NL1 deletion. In particular, it should be noted that no significant change was observed in NMDAR subunit protein levels, which is of interest given the decreased NMDA/AMPA ratio observed in NL1 KO mice (Chubykin et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2008b). However, NL1 KO mice exhibited a 30% increase in the expression of NL3 as well as a 20% decrease in both α and β neurexin levels (Table 1). NL1 KO mice also exhibited a significant increase in the expression of synapsin 1a as well as decreases of ∼15–20% in the levels of several presynaptic proteins: liprin, CSP, and munc-18 (Table 1). Given the direct interactions between NL1 and neurexins as well as the link between neurexin-1 copy number and human cases of autism, the reduction of neurexin levels in NL1 KO mice further increases their potential relevance to autism.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Synaptic protein composition in NL1 KO brain

NL1 KO mice do not exhibit global behavioral deficits

Because NL1 is a ubiquitously expressed, excitatory synaptic cell adhesion molecule, one might expect widespread CNS dysfunction; on the contrary, NL1 KO mice showed normal anxiety-like behavior, locomotor activity, motor coordination/learning, auditory startle responses, and sensitivity to sensory stimuli. Anxiety-like behaviors were normal on three anxiety tests: elevated plus maze (supplemental Fig. 1A,B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), dark/light box (supplemental Fig. 1C,D, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), and open field (supplemental Fig. 1E,F, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Indeed, these tests of anxiety were repeated later in a naive cohort of 21 littermate pairs in a completely different testing order (elevated plus maze, dark/light box, followed by open field) with the same lack of effect observed (data not shown). Locomotor activity was also normal in NL1 KO mice when tested under four different conditions. First, locomotor activity in an open-field arena was normal in NL1 KO mice (supplemental Fig. 2A,B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). This was also true of locomotor activity in the dark/light apparatus (supplemental Fig. 2D, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), with only a small decrease in distance moved in the elevated plus maze (supplemental Fig. 2C, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) [two-way ANOVA, main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 4.09, p < 0.049; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 0.70, p = 0.41; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,42) = 0.42, p = 0.52]. NL1 KO mice also exhibited normal locomotor activity and habituated at a similar rate compared with controls in a 2 h novel-cage locomotor task (supplemental Fig. 2E, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). For the complete statistical analyses of all behavioral tests, see Table 2.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Additional statistical analyses for behavioral tests

On the accelerating rotarod, NL1 KO mice showed normal motor coordination and motor learning as measured over 27 trials (supplemental Fig. 2F, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Compared with their WT littermates, NL1 KO mice also exhibited normal prepulse inhibition (supplemental Fig. 2G, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) as well as a normal baseline startle amplitude in response to an acoustic tone (supplemental Fig. 2H, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).

NL1 KO mice also exhibited normal fear learning and memory. During a test for contextual fear memory, both genotypes spent a similar percentage of time freezing [NL1 KO (n = 23), 54.91 ± 3.52%; WT (n = 23), 63.60 ± 3.81%, mean ± SEM; two-way ANOVA, main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 2.71, p = 0.11; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 0.42, p = 0.52; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,42) = 0.01, p = 0.91]; NL1 KO and WT mice also exhibited comparable levels of cue-dependent fear memory [NL1 KO (n = 23), 32.50 ± 5.10%; WT (n = 23), 39.64 ± 4.17%, mean ± SEM; two-way ANOVA, main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 0.90, p = 0.35; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 0.09, p = 0.76; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,42) = 0.69, p = 0.41].

Sensitivity to painful sensory stimuli was measured in two different tests. In a test of footshock sensitivity, a series of footshocks were delivered through a metal grid floor at increasing currents. Both WT and NL1 KO mice required similar current thresholds to elicit flinching and jumping behaviors (Fig. 1A). Compared with their WT littermates, NL1 KO mice required a higher current threshold to elicit audible vocalizations (Fig. 1) [two-way ANOVA (n = 11 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,18) = 6.47, p < 0.20; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,18) = 9.29, p < 0.0069; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,18) = 1.78, p = 0.20], suggesting that, if anything, NL1 KO mice are slightly less sensitive to footshock. In a second test, mice were placed on a hotplate at 52°C, and NL1 KO mice exhibited a shorter latency to lick or shake their hindpaw compared with WT mice, suggesting that they are slightly more sensitive to heat [NL1 KO (n = 21), 11.79 ± 0.81 s; WT (n = 21), 14.33 ± 0.80 s, mean ± SEM; two-way ANOVA, main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,38) = 4.90, p < 0.04; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,38) = 2.26, p = 0.14; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,38) = 1.09, p = 0.30]. Although NL1 deletion appears to have mixed effects on nociception depending on the specific stimulus modality, the balance of the data suggest that, in general, NL1 deletion does not cause nonspecific, global behavioral dysfunction. For the complete statistical analyses of all behavioral tests, see Table 2.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

NL1 KO mice exhibit mixed responses to painful stimuli. When footshocks were delivered through a metal grid floor at increasing currents, the amount of current required to elicit flinching [two-way ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p = 0.78; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.54; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.29] and jumping [two-way ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p = 0.34; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.62; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.45] behaviors was similar for both WT and NL1 KO mice. Compared with WT mice, NL1 KO mice required a higher current threshold to elicit vocalizations [two-way ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), *p < 0.0.020; sex (between-subjects factor), p < 0.0069; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.20]. n = 11 littermate pairs. Data represent means ± SEM.

NL1 KO mice exhibit minimal deficits in social behavior

Because NL1 and neurexin 1 mutations in humans have been linked to autism spectrum disorders (Jamain et al., 2003; Chih et al., 2004; Comoletti et al., 2004; Laumonnier et al., 2004; Feng et al., 2006; Szatmari et al., 2007) and because there is a significant decrease in neurexin levels in NL1 KO mice, we tested NL1 KO mice in several tests of social behavior. NL1 KO mice exhibited a social interaction abnormality in only one of several tasks, showing decreased interaction with a caged, adult target mouse (Fig. 2A) [planned comparison (contrast analysis) of the effect of genotype within the social target only, F(1,42) = 4.64, p < 0.04; initial three-way mixed ANOVA (n = 23 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 4.76, p < 0.04; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 0.001, p = 0.97; main effect of target (within-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 1.00, p = 0.76; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,42) = 1.17, p = 0.29; genotype × target interaction, F(1,42) = 0.80, p = 0.38; sex × target interaction, F(1,42) = 0.79, p = 0.38; genotype × sex × target interaction, F(1,42) = 0.30, p = 0.59] in a task that has been validated as a measure of social approach/avoidance in several previous publications (Berton et al., 2006; Kwon et al., 2006; Tsankova et al., 2006; Krishnan et al., 2007; Tabuchi et al., 2007; Lutter et al., 2008). It is important to note that interaction with an inanimate, empty cage in the same apparatus, under the same conditions, was normal (Fig. 2A) [planned comparison (contrast analysis) of the effect of genotype within the inanimate target only, F(1,42) = 2.43, p = 0.13], indicating specificity for social interaction. In addition, the total distance moved during the test of interaction with a social target was similar between genotypes [trial with a social interaction target: NL1 KO, 2035.52 ± 129.71 cm; WT, 2490.96 ± 96.50 cm; trial with an inanimate interaction target: NL1 KO, 1581.90 ± 100.18 cm; WT, 1573.97 ± 78.26 cm, mean ± SEM; three-way mixed ANOVA (n = 23 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 2.74, p = 0.11; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 0.10, p = 0.76; main effect of target (within-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 112.26, p < 0.000001; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,42) = 0.12, p = 0.73; genotype × target interaction, F(1,42) = 10.36, p < 0.003; sex × target interaction, F(1,42) = 2.70, p = 0.11; genotype × sex × target interaction, F(1,42) = 4.97, p < 0.04].

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

NL1 KO mice exhibit minimal social behavior abnormalities. A, NL1 WT and KO mice were allowed to interact sequentially with a novel inanimate target (i.e., an empty cage) and a social target (i.e., a caged adult conspecific mouse) in an open field. An initial three-way mixed ANOVA found a significant main effect of genotype [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p < 0.04; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.97; target (within-subjects factor), p = 0.76; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.29; genotype × target interaction, p = 0.38; sex × target interaction, p = 0.38; genotype × sex × target interaction, p = 0.59]. Additional planned comparisons found that NL1 WT mice spent less time interacting with the social target than controls (left bars, *p < 0.04, planned comparisons). Both NL1 WT and KO mice spent similar amounts of time interacting with the novel, inanimate target (right bars, p = 0.13, planned comparisons). n = 23 littermate pairs. Legend in A applies to A–E. B, Time spent in interactions using the social versus inanimate preference test. Mice were simultaneously exposed to a novel inanimate target (i.e., an empty cage) and a novel social target (i.e., a novel, caged adult conspecific mouse). The time spent interacting with inanimate and social targets was not different between NL1 KO mice and controls, and neither WT or NL1 KO mice preferred the social target over the inanimate target [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p = 0.22; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.16; target (within-subjects factor), p = 0.08; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.27; genotype × target interaction, p = 0.97; genotype × sex × target interaction, p = 0.81]. ns, Not significant. n = 23 littermate pairs. C, Time spent in interactions using the social novelty preference test. Mice were simultaneously exposed to a novel social target (i.e., a novel, caged adult conspecific mouse) and a familiar social target (i.e., a familiar, caged adult conspecific mouse). A three-way mixed ANOVA found a main effect of target [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p = 0.39; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.50; target (within-subjects factor), p < 0.003; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.31; genotype × target interaction, p = 0.40; sex × target interaction, p = 0.87; genotype × sex × target interaction, p = 0.46]. However, planned comparisons within each genotype found that, unlike WT mice, NL1 KO mice did not exhibit a preference for the novel social target (**p < 0.006; ns, not significant; p = 0.10, planned comparisons). n = 23 littermate pairs. D, Time spent interacting with a freely moving juvenile mouse during a test of social learning [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p = 0.39; sex (between-subjects factor), p < 0.041; trial (within-subjects factor), p < 0.0007; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.56; genotype × trial interaction, p = 0.78; sex × trial interaction, p = 0.07; genotype × sex × trial interaction, p = 0.23]. Mice were initially allowed to interact with a juvenile target mouse for 2 min (Day 1), and NL1 KO mice showed normal direct social interaction with the juvenile. Seventy-two hours later (Day 4), mice were again allowed to interact with the same juvenile target mouse, and both WT and NL1 KO mice exhibited significant social learning (recognition memory), as indicated by a decrease in the amount of time spent interacting with the juvenile (*p < 0.021 for WT; p < 0.008 for KO, planned comparisons within each genotype). n = 23 littermate pairs. E, The thickness (i.e., the height) of nests built from cotton nesting material was measured over a 90 min observation period [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p < 0.044; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.35; time (within-subjects factor), p < 0.000013; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.69; genotype × time interaction, p = 0.15; sex × time interaction, p = 0.18; genotype × sex × time interaction, p < 0.032]. WT mice built thicker nests than NL1 KO mice (*p < 0.021, planned comparison between genotypes at 90 min). n = 11 littermate pairs. Data represent means ± SEM.

This isolated, task-specific abnormality in social behavior is not likely attributable to altered olfactory ability because time spent interacting with a “social smell” was normal in NL1 KO mice [NL1 KO, 52.17 ± 4.03 s; WT, 53.07 ± 3.81 s, mean ± SEM; two-way ANOVA (n = 22 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,40) = 0.30, p = 0.59; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,40) = 4.11, p < 0.049; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,40) = 2.01, p = 0.16]. Importantly, gross olfactory abilities were also normal in NL1 KO mice as measured by latency to find a buried treat in a neutral home cage [NL1 KO, 340.20 ± 53.30 s; WT, 387.90 ± 37.62 s, mean ± SEM; two-way ANOVA (n = 10 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,16) = 0.65, p = 0.43; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,16) = 2.58, p = 0.13; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,16) = 0.13, p = 0.72].

In three other social tasks, however, no differences were observed. In a test for social versus inanimate interaction, there was no difference between WT and NL1 KO in time spent interacting with either the social target or the inanimate cage, nor was there a significant preference for the social versus inanimate target for either genotype (Fig. 2B). In a test for familiar versus novel social interaction, there was also no significant difference between NL1 KO and WT littermates in time spent interacting with either the novel or the familiar social target (Fig. 2C). Unlike WT mice, NL1 KO mice did not show a statistically significant preference for the novel social target compared with the familiar, although a similar trend was apparent (Fig. 2C) [planned comparison (contrast analysis) of novel vs familiar target: WT, F(1,42) = 8.42, p < 0.006; NL1 KO, F(1,42) = 2.89, p = 0.10]. Likewise, no differences in social interaction or social learning were observed in a task of reciprocal social interaction with a juvenile conspecific (Fig. 2D). Interestingly, although not a strictly social behavior, NL1 KO mice displayed impaired nest building skills compared with WT (Fig. 2E) [initial three-way mixed ANOVA (n = 11 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,18) = 4.74, p < 0.044; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,18) = 0.93, p = 0.35; main effect of time (within-subjects factor), F(2,36) = 15.70, p < 0.000013; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,18) = 0.17, p = 0.69; genotype × time interaction, F(2.36) = 2.03, p = 0.15; sex × time interaction, F(2,36) = 1.81, p = 0.18; genotype × sex × time interaction, F(2,36) = 3.80, p < 0.032; planned comparisons (contrast analysis) comparing genotypes, 30 min, F(1,18) = 4.26, p = 0.054; 60 min, F(1,18) = 2.47, p = 0.13; 90 min, F(1,18) = 6.49, p < 0.021].

NL1 KO mice exhibit deficits in spatial memory

Because mental retardation is associated with many cases of ASDs (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and some ASD patients with neurexin mutations exhibit low IQ (Kim et al., 2008a), we tested learning and memory in NL1 KO mice using the Morris water maze task. NL1 KO mice exhibited significant abnormalities in spatial learning and memory. Despite a normal learning curve as measured using latency to reach the platform during training (Fig. 3B), NL1 KO mice exhibited a slight learning deficit using distance traveled before reaching the hidden platform, an analysis that eliminates swim speed as a concern (Fig. 3A) [three-way mixed ANOVA (n = 23 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 7.52, p < 0.0089; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 0.26, p = 0.61; main effect of day (within-subjects factor), F(10,420) = 20.95, p < 0.000001; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,42) = 0.011, p = 0.92; genotype × day interaction, F(10,420) = 0.78, p = 0.65; sex × day interaction, F(10,420) = 93, p = 0.50; genotype × sex × day interaction, F(10,420) = 0.50, p = 0.89]. In fact, NL1 KO mice showed a slight increase in average swim speed compared with WT (Fig. 3C) [three-way mixed ANOVA (n = 23 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 4.59, p < 0.038; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 2.21, p = 0.14; main effect of day (within-subjects factor), F(10,420) = 5.76, p < 0.000001; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,42) = 0.0058, p = 0.94; genotype × day interaction, F(10,420) = 0.52, p = 0.88; sex × day interaction, F(10,420) = 0.50, p = 0.89; genotype × sex × day interaction, F(10,420) = 0.60, p = 0.82], likely explaining why their latency to reach the platform learning curve appeared normal. During these training trials, NL1 KO mice did not spend more time near the wall of the maze (thigmotaxis) compared with WT (Fig. 3D). On a spatial memory test 24 h after the end of Morris water maze training (probe trial), WT mice spent significantly more time in the target quadrant than all other quadrants (Fig. 3E) [planned comparisons (contrast analysis), target vs opposite, F(1,42) = 27.21, p < 0.000006; target vs adjacent left, F(1,42) = 21.15, p < 0.00004; target vs adjacent right, F(1,42) = 5.99, p < 0.02], whereas NL1 KO mice showed no significant preference for the target quadrant compared with any other quadrant [planned comparisons (contrast analysis), target vs opposite, F(1,42) = 2.10, p = 0.15; target vs adjacent left, F(1,42) = 2.06, p = 0.16; target vs adjacent right, F(1,42) = 1.05, p = 0.31]. The NL1 KO mice performed at near chance levels (Fig. 3E), indicating a spatial memory deficit. Furthermore, NL1 KO mice spent significantly less time in the target quadrant and more time in the opposite quadrant than WT (Fig. 3E) [planned comparisons (contrast analysis), target quadrant, F(1,42) = 4.67, p < 0.037; opposite quadrant, F(1,42) = 8.67, p < 0.006; adjacent left quadrant, F(1,42) = 1.89, p = 0.17; adjacent right quadrant, F(1,42) = 0.69, p = 0.41]. It is important to note that NL1 KO mice learned the visible platform task as well as controls (supplemental Fig. 2I, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), indicating that basic neurological function (swimming, vision, etc.) was intact.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

NL1 KO mice exhibit impaired spatial memory in the Morris water maze. A–D, Training trials for the Morris water maze task. For each day of training, data were averaged across the four daily trials, and, in all three-way mixed ANOVAs, “day” was treated as the repeated measure. NL1 KO mice displayed an abnormal learning curve as measured by distance to reach the submerged platform compared with WT (A) [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p < 0.0089; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.61; day (within-subjects factor), p < 0.000001; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.92; genotype × day interaction, p = 0.65; sex × day interaction, p = 0.50; genotype × sex × day interaction, p = 0.89]. The latency to reach the platform (B) was normal in the NL1 KO mice [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p = 0.47; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.65; day (within-subjects factor), p < 0.000001; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.74; genotype × day interaction, p = 0.93; sex × day interaction, p = 0.11; genotype × sex × day interaction, p = 0.50]. NL1 KO mice exhibited faster average swim speeds (C) than controls [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,42) = 4.59, p < 0.038; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.14; day (within-subjects factor), p < 0.000001; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.94; genotype × day interaction, p = 0.88; sex × day interaction, p = 0.89; genotype × sex × day interaction, p = 0.82]. Percentage time spent near the wall of the maze (D, percentage thigmotaxis) was not different across groups [three-way mixed ANOVA, genotype (between-subjects factor), p = 0.95; sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.82; day (within-subjects factor), p < 0.000001; genotype × sex interaction, p = 0.99; genotype × day interaction, p = 0.60; sex × day interaction, p = 0.78; genotype × sex × day interaction, p = 0.96]. There were no differences between groups in the visible platform task (supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). n = 23 littermate pairs. Legend in A also applies to B–D. E, Percentage of time that mice spent swimming in each quadrant of the pool during the probe trial (i.e., spatial memory test) on day 12 of the Morris water maze [three-way mixed ANOVA, sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.22; genotype (between-subjects factor), p = 0.84; quadrant (within-subjects factor), p < 0.000001; sex × genotype interaction, p = 0.84; sex × quadrant interaction, p = 0.53; genotype × quadrant interaction, p < 0.02; sex × genotype × quadrant interaction, p = 0.96]. NL1 KO mice spent less time in the target quadrant than controls (*p < 0.037 for NL1 KO target vs WT target, planned comparisons) and, unlike control mice (#p < 0.05 compared with all other quadrants, planned comparisons) showed no preference for the target quadrant, indicating a deficit in spatial memory. n = 23 littermate pairs. Data represent means ± SEM.

NL1 KO mice exhibit deficits in hippocampal LTP

Because NL1 KO mice exhibit a decrease in hippocampus-dependent spatial memory and a decrease in the NMDA/AMPA ratio in area CA1 of the hippocampus (Chubykin et al., 2007), we predicted that NL1 KO mice would exhibit a decrease in LTP in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Indeed, theta burst stimulation (five bursts of four pulses at 100 Hz with an interburst interval of 0.2 s) resulted in a significantly reduced magnitude of LTP in area CA1 of the hippocampus in slices from NL1 KO mice compared with WT littermate controls (Fig. 4A,B) [LTP 50–60 min after TBS induction (fEPSP expressed as the fraction of control): NL1 KO (n = 6), 1.49 ± 0.09; WT (n = 6), 1.88 ± 0.13, mean ± SEM; t test, p < 0.031]. This decrease in LTP magnitude was not accompanied by any alteration in basal synaptic transmission because input–output curves (Fig. 4C,D) and paired-pulse facilitation (Fig. 4E,F) were normal. Based on the previously observed deficits in NMDAR transmission in NL1 KO mice (Chubykin et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2008b), it is reasonable to assume that the LTP phenotype is most likely caused by a deficit in LTP induction.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Decreased theta burst-induced LTP in area CA1 of NL1 KO mice. A, Representative traces showing fEPSPs from a WT and a KO animal before (1) and after (2) LTP induction by TBS. B, Time course of fEPSPs before and after LTP induction by TBS in WT (n = 6) and KO (n = 6) animals. The time points of the representative traces in A are noted on the graph (see 1 and 2). C, AMPA-mediated synaptic strength is unchanged in NL1 KO mice. Representative traces from a WT and a KO animal show input–output measurements with increasing stimulus intensities. Responses were measured in area CA1 of mouse hippocampus. D, Average fEPSP slope plotted against fiber-volley amplitude. Input–output measurements were performed in wild-type (n = 6) and KO (n = 5) animals. E, Overlaid representative traces from a WT and a KO animal showing paired-pulse facilitation of excitatory synaptic responses at interstimulus intervals ranging from 30 to 600 ms. F, Average paired-pulse facilitation of excitatory synaptic responses did not show any difference between WT (n = 12) and KO (n = 8) animals. Data plotted as means ± SEM.

NL1 KO mice exhibit a decrease in the NMDA/AMPA ratio in the hippocampus (Chubykin et al., 2007), which could be attributable to either a change in postsynaptic receptor function or a change in the number of NMDA- or AMPA-containing (i.e., silent or nonsilent) synapses. Therefore, we examined the effect of NL1 loss on synaptic density in vivo. We found no significant alterations in total synapse density (supplemental Fig. 3, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), excitatory synapse density (Figs. 5, 6), or inhibitory synapse density (Figs. 5, 6) in the hippocampus. Furthermore, no changes were observed in the size of immunopositive puncta with any targeted antigen (Figs. 5, 6) (supplemental Fig. 3, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Also, as mentioned above, whole-brain immunoblots detected no significant changes in the expression levels of multiple NMDAR subunits in NL1 KO mice (Table 1). The findings that NL1 KO mice exhibit no change in the number of excitatory immunopositive puncta and no change in the expression levels of NMDAR subunits suggest that the decreased hippocampal NMDA/AMPA ratio observed in NL1 KO mice (Chubykin et al., 2007) may be attributable to altered excitatory postsynaptic receptor function rather than changes in synapse or NMDAR number. However, the current results cannot rule out more subtle effects of NL1 deletion on excitatory or inhibitory synapse number, synaptic NMDAR numbers, or specific subtypes of inhibitory or excitatory synapses.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Number and size of glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses are normal in NL1-deficient CA1 region of hippocampus. A, Representative confocal images of wild-type and NL1 KO CA1 region of hippocampus double immunostained for VGLUT1 and VGAT. Number (B) and size (C) of both VGLUT1- and VGAT-positive puncta are normal in NL1 KO neurons. y-axis depicts normalized number (B) and size (C) with wild-type control. n = 3 mice per genotype.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Number and size of glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses are normal in NL1-deficient CA3 region of hippocampus. A, Representative confocal images of wild-type and NL1 KO CA3 region of hippocampus double immunostained for VGLUT1 and VGAT. B, C, Number (B) and size (C) of both VGLUT1- and VGAT-positive puncta are normal in NL1 KO neurons. y-axis depicts normalized number (B) and size (C) with wild-type control. n = 3 mice per genotype.

NL1 KO mice exhibit increased repetitive grooming behavior

Because NL and neurexin 1 mutations in humans have been linked to autism spectrum disorders (Jamain et al., 2003; Chih et al., 2004; Comoletti et al., 2004; Laumonnier et al., 2004; Feng et al., 2006; Szatmari et al., 2007), we characterized grooming behavior in NL1 KO mice, a behavior that might reflect the repetitive, stereotyped behavior core symptom domain of autism (Moy et al., 2006; Crawley, 2007). NL1 KO mice spent more than double the amount of time spontaneously grooming compared with WT mice [NL1 KO (n = 22), 56.15 ± 11.32 s; WT (n = 22), 25.15 ± 7.57 s, mean ± SEM; two-way ANOVA, main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,40) = 5.87, p < 0.020; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,40) = 2.0, p = 0.17; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,40) = 0.63, p = 0.43]. In a marble burying task, which has been described as a task relevant to anxiety and to obsessive-compulsive/repetitive behavior (Broekkamp et al., 1986; Njung'e and Handley, 1991; Borsini et al., 2002; Deacon, 2006; Thomas et al., 2009), there was no difference between NL1 KO and WT mice [mean ± SEM number of marbles buried, NL1 KO mice, 5.5 ± 1.37; WT mice, 8.86 ± 1.54; two-way ANOVA (n = 22 pairs), main effect of genotype (between-subjects factor), F(1,40) = 2.51, p = 0.12; main effect of sex (between-subjects factor), F(1,40) = 0.01, p = 0.93; genotype × sex interaction, F(1,40) = 0.03, p = 0.86].

NL1 KO mice exhibit a reduced NMDA/AMPA ratio in the dorsal striatum

Although a decrease in the NMDA/AMPA ratio has been observed in the hippocampus of NL1 KO mice (Chubykin et al., 2007), it is unlikely that this is responsible for the increased grooming behavior observed in NL1 KO mice (see above) because the hippocampus is not known to be involved with mammalian grooming behavior. Because the dorsal striatum has been repeatedly implicated in rodent grooming behavior (Cromwell and Berridge, 1996; Aldridge et al., 2004; Welch et al., 2007), we hypothesized that NL1 KO mice might exhibit similar alterations in synaptic transmission in the dorsal striatum.

Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings of striatal medium spiny neurons were performed to determine the NMDA/AMPA ratio in corticostriatal synapses. Baseline values for access resistance (14.2 ± 0.7 MΩ, wild type; 13.6 ± 0.8 MΩ, NL1 KO), cell membrane resistance (340 ± 54 MΩ, wild type; 312 ± 58 MΩ, NL1 KO), and cell capacitance (167.2 ± 11 pF, wild type; 169.3 ± 10 pF, NL1 KO) did not differ between the groups. The NMDA/AMPA ratio was assessed by two measurements: the peak of the evoked EPSCs (eEPSCs) at −80 mV, to detect the AMPAR currents; and the current amplitude, 50 ms after spike onset and at +40 mV, to detect the NMDAR currents. AMPAR eEPSC amplitude was 471 ± 61 pA for wild type and 470 ± 62 pA for NL1 KO (t test, p = 0.995). Consistent with our hypothesis, the NMDA/AMPA ratio in the striatum of NL1 KO mice was significantly reduced by ∼30% (Fig. 7A) [NMDA/AMPA ratio, NL1 KO (n = 22), 0.77 ± 0.07; WT (n = 23), 1.00 ± 0.08, mean ± SEM; t test, p < 0.01].

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

NL1 KO mice exhibit a decreased NMDA/AMPA ratio in the dorsal striatum accompanied by increased repetitive behavior that can be rescued pharmacologically. A, NMDAR- and AMPAR-mediated responses are indicated in wild-type traces. Current amplitude was normalized to that of the AMPAR peak. Amplitude of NMDAR currents at +40 mV was measured in a 2 ms window set at 50 ms from spike onset (dashed line on traces). B, The time spent grooming was observed 30 min after systemic administration of the NMDA partial coagonist DCS (20 mg/kg). Administration of DCS rescued the increased grooming phenotype in NL1 KO mice [*p < 0.02 compared with all other groups, post hoc Tukey's honestly significant difference test; three-way ANOVA, sex (between-subjects factor), p = 0.45; genotype (between-subjects factor), p < 0.004; treatment (between-subjects factor), p < 0.02; sex × genotype interaction, p = 0.48; sex × treatment, p = 0.54; genotype × treatment interaction, p = 0.09; sex × genotype × treatment interaction, p = 0.36]. Vehicle (Veh), n = 20 littermate pairs; DCS, n = 19 littermate pairs. Data represent means ± SEM.

Systemic d-cycloserine rescues the increased repetitive behavior in NL1 KO mice

We next examined whether a drug that is known to enhance NMDA receptor function and NMDA receptor-dependent behaviors in vivo could acutely reverse the increased grooming behavior in NL1 KO mice. Given that NL1 KO mice exhibited a decrease in the NMDA/AMPA ratio in the dorsal striatum, we hypothesized that altering NMDA receptor function pharmacologically would rescue the abnormal grooming behavior in NL1 KO mice. To test this, we systemically administered either the NMDA receptor coagonist DCS or vehicle 30 min before measuring grooming behavior. Consistent with our previous findings (see above), NL1 KO mice treated with vehicle displayed increased grooming compared with WT mice treated with vehicle (Fig. 7B) (post hoc Tukey's test, KO plus vehicle vs WT plus vehicle, p < 0.005). However, 20 mg/kg DCS given 30 min before testing rescued the increased grooming in NL1 KO mice (Fig. 7B) (post hoc Tukey's test, KO plus DCS vs WT plus DCS, p = 0.78).

Discussion

NL1 and hippocampus-dependent learning and memory

Genetic deletion of the excitatory synapse cell-adhesion molecule NL1 in mice leads to decreased long-term synaptic plasticity in area CA1 of the hippocampus and disruption of hippocampus-dependent spatial memory. In addition to a decreased rate of learning, NL1 KO mice were unable to use a spatial strategy to locate a submerged platform in the Morris water maze. These deficits were not associated with altered thigmotaxis and are not explained by differences in swim speed, coordination, locomotor activity, or vision. The most parsimonious explanation for impaired hippocampus-dependent learning and memory in NL1 KOs is the decreased LTP we observed in NL1 KO mice, a finding perhaps best linked to the decreased NMDA/AMPA current ratio observed previously in area CA1 of the hippocampus of NL1 KO mice (Chubykin et al., 2007). Our findings are consistent with a recent report suggesting that NL1 is required for NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic currents and normal expression of LTP in the amygdala (Kim et al., 2008b). A finding of mildly reduced learning, but not memory, and decreased LTP induction has also been observed in NL1 transgenic mice that overexpress NL1, although the mechanisms underlying these findings remain unclear (Dahlhaus et al., 2010). These cognitive abnormalities are consistent with the idea that NL1 or neurexin 1 may play important roles at the synapse relevant to comorbid mental retardation in autism spectrum disorders.

NL1 and social behavior

Interestingly, NL1 KO mice exhibit little to no deficits in social behavior. In only one of several tasks did NL1 KOs interact less with a caged adult target compared with wild-type littermate controls. This is observed despite normal locomotor activity, habituation, and olfactory ability for food and social odors. Furthermore, NL1 KO mice do not engage in nesting behavior as readily as their WT littermate counterparts. On many other social tasks, however, NL1 KO mice show normal interaction and approach to social targets as well as normal social recognition. Given the finding of reduced social interaction in only one social approach task and in nesting behavior, the bulk of the data do not favor a strong abnormality in social behavior in the NL1 KO mice.

NL1 and enhanced repetitive, grooming behavior

NL1 KO mice exhibit a clear, significant increase in repetitive, grooming behavior, and this phenotype is robust and reproducible. Although the clinical significance of NL1 deletion is not entirely clear, NL1 binds to presynaptic neurexins, which have been implicated in human autism (Feng et al., 2006; Szatmari et al., 2007). We find that NL1 deletion does lead to a small but significant reduction in neurexin levels in the brain, and therefore, it is possible that the enhanced repetitive behavior in NL1 KO mice could be representative of one of the symptoms of autism, namely increased repetitive, stereotyped behaviors. Indeed, neurexin 1 deficient mice also exhibit a similar increase in repetitive grooming behavior (Etherton et al., 2009). Of course, grooming behavior is not consistent across species, and increased repetitive grooming behavior has been suggested to exhibit significant face validity not only for autism spectrum disorders but also for obsessive/compulsive disorder (OCD) and trichotillomania (Welch et al., 2007; Bienvenu et al., 2009; Züchner et al., 2009).

Consistent with a link between the enhanced repetitive behavior and our findings of reduced NMDA/AMPA ratio in both the hippocampus (Chubykin et al., 2007) and the dorsal striatum (Fig. 7), we successfully rescued the increased grooming in NL1 KO mice with systemic d-cycloserine at a dose reported previously in the literature to augment NMDA receptor-dependent forms of learning and memory in the brain (Flood et al., 1992; Zlomuzica et al., 2007). Vehicle-treated NL1 KO mice showed significantly enhanced grooming, whereas DCS-treated NL1 KO mice revealed completely normal levels of grooming. No alteration of grooming behavior was observed in WT littermates with DCS treatment. These data support the hypothesis that reduced NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic transmission in NL1 KO mice (Fig. 7) mediates the enhanced grooming in these mice, suggesting a potential treatment for at least one cause of this behavioral abnormality of potential relevance to autism, OCD, or trichotillomania. Interestingly, a small pilot study of d-cycloserine treatment in autism has been published previously and indicates that DCS may be of potential benefit in patients with autism, although the social domain was more prominently affected in this inconclusive, underpowered pilot study (Posey et al., 2004).

One alternative possibility for increased grooming in our mice is an NMDAR-related altered sensation or nociception. However, three lines of evidence argue against this. First, we have detected a decrease (not an increase) in NMDAR currents in both the hippocampus and the striatum of NL1 KO mice, suggesting a low likelihood for an NMDAR-driven increase in itching or other sensation that might increase grooming in NL1 KO mice (Ferreira and Lorenzetti, 1994; Tan-No et al., 2000). Second, the grooming bouts were primarily syntactic (i.e., followed a cephalocaudal direction). It is difficult to imagine why focal sensory abnormalities would lead to a syntactic grooming pattern rather than focal scratching behavior. Finally, sensory thresholds in these mutants, as determined with the footshock and hotplate tests, were not consistently altered in a single direction, further decreasing the likelihood of altered sensory function in the increased grooming behavior of NL1 KO mice.

Role of NL1 in synaptic transmission and NMDA receptor function

The precise mechanisms through which NL1 modulates NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic transmission remain to be determined. NL1 is selectively localized to excitatory synapses (Song et al., 1999) and interacts with NMDA receptors through its interaction with postsynaptic density protein-95 (PSD-95) via a PDZ (PSD-95/Discs large/zona ocludens-1) type I domain on its C terminus (Irie et al., 1997). Several PSD-95-associated proteins are thought to be clustered before assembly of postsynaptic spines (Prange et al., 2004; Gerrow et al., 2006). NL1 and NMDARs are enriched in those clusters (Irie et al., 1997), whereas AMPARs incorporate into synapses after spine formation (Nam and Chen, 2005). It is possible that the association between NL1 and other PSD-95-associated proteins, including the NMDAR, may preclude the efficient incorporation of some of these proteins into the synapses of NL1 KO mice. NMDA receptors themselves are highly dynamic, even within the plasma membrane (Newpher and Ehlers, 2008), and phosphorylation of the NMDAR is known to alter its trafficking (Chung et al., 2004; Lin et al., 2006). Additional study is needed to determine whether this type of NMDAR modulation is altered in NL1 KO mice.

Relevance to autism, mental retardation, and other neuropsychiatric disorders

Although they exhibit increased repetitive behaviors and cognitive deficits, it is premature to suggest that NL1 KO mice represent an accurate rodent model of human autism or mental retardation. Although chromosomal rearrangements in regions that harbor the NL1 gene (Konstantareas and Homatidis, 1999; Zoghbi, 2003; Yan et al., 2004) and copy number variations of the NL1 gene (Glessner et al., 2009) have been implicated in cases of autism in humans, the evidence for a direct link between NL1 and human disease remains sparse (Talebizadeh et al., 2004; Vincent et al., 2004; Gauthier et al., 2005; Ylisaukko-oja et al., 2005). Additional study of the NL1 KO mice, however, will provide insights into the neural basis of increased repetitive behaviors of potential relevance to autism spectrum disorders, OCD, and trichotillomania. With the recent implication of neurexin 1 and shank3 in human cases of autism spectrum disorders and the known binding of neurexins and shank3 to NL1, one might expect the NL1 KO phenotype to foreshadow a subset of the neurexin 1 or shank3 KO phenotype.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by grants from Autism Speaks (C.M.P.), the Simons Foundation (T.C.S.), National Institute of Mental Health Grants MH065975-05 (C.M.P.) and R37 MH52804-08 (T.C.S.), and gifts from BRAINS for Autism/Debra Caudy and Clay Heighten (Founders), the Crystal Charity Ball, and The Hartwell Foundation (C.M.P.). C.M.P. is a Hartwell Scholar.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Craig M. Powell, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Department of Neurology, 5323 Harry Hines Boulevard, Dallas, TX 75390-8813. craig.powell{at}utsouthwestern.edu.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Aldridge JW,
    2. Berridge KC,
    3. Rosen AR
    (2004) Basal ganglia neural mechanisms of natural movement sequences. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 82:732–739.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. American Psychiatric Association
    (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC).
  3. ↵
    1. Berton O,
    2. McClung CA,
    3. Dileone RJ,
    4. Krishnan V,
    5. Renthal W,
    6. Russo SJ,
    7. Graham D,
    8. Tsankova NM,
    9. Bolanos CA,
    10. Rios M,
    11. Monteggia LM,
    12. Self DW,
    13. Nestler EJ
    (2006) Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress. Science 311:864–868.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Bienvenu OJ,
    2. Wang Y,
    3. Shugart YY,
    4. Welch JM,
    5. Grados MA,
    6. Fyer AJ,
    7. Rauch SL,
    8. McCracken JT,
    9. Rasmussen SA,
    10. Murphy DL,
    11. Cullen B,
    12. Valle D,
    13. Hoehn-Saric R,
    14. Greenberg BD,
    15. Pinto A,
    16. Knowles JA,
    17. Piacentini J,
    18. Pauls DL,
    19. Liang KY,
    20. Willour VL,
    21. et al.
    (2009) Sapap3 and pathological grooming in humans: Results from the OCD collaborative genetics study. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 150B:710–720.
    OpenUrl
  5. ↵
    1. Borsini F,
    2. Podhorna J,
    3. Marazziti D
    (2002) Do animal models of anxiety predict anxiolytic-like effects of antidepressants? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 163:121–141.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Boucard AA,
    2. Chubykin AA,
    3. Comoletti D,
    4. Taylor P,
    5. Südhof TC
    (2005) A splice code for trans-synaptic cell adhesion mediated by binding of neuroligin 1 to alpha- and beta-neurexins. Neuron 48:229–236.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Broekkamp CL,
    2. Rijk HW,
    3. Joly-Gelouin D,
    4. Lloyd KL
    (1986) Major tranquillizers can be distinguished from minor tranquillizers on the basis of effects on marble burying and swim-induced grooming in mice. Eur J Pharmacol 126:223–229.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Budreck EC,
    2. Scheiffele P
    (2007) Neuroligin-3 is a neuronal adhesion protein at GABAergic and glutamatergic synapses. Eur J Neurosci 26:1738–1748.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Chih B,
    2. Afridi SK,
    3. Clark L,
    4. Scheiffele P
    (2004) Disorder-associated mutations lead to functional inactivation of neuroligins. Hum Mol Genet 13:1471–1477.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Chih B,
    2. Engelman H,
    3. Scheiffele P
    (2005) Control of excitatory and inhibitory synapse formation by neuroligins. Science 307:1324–1328.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Chubykin AA,
    2. Liu X,
    3. Comoletti D,
    4. Tsigelny I,
    5. Taylor P,
    6. Südhof TC
    (2005) Dissection of synapse induction by neuroligins: effect of a neuroligin mutation associated with autism. J Biol Chem 280:22365–22374.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Chubykin AA,
    2. Atasoy D,
    3. Etherton MR,
    4. Brose N,
    5. Kavalali ET,
    6. Gibson JR,
    7. Südhof TC
    (2007) Activity-dependent validation of excitatory versus inhibitory synapses by neuroligin-1 versus neuroligin-2. Neuron 54:919–931.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Chung HJ,
    2. Huang YH,
    3. Lau LF,
    4. Huganir RL
    (2004) Regulation of the NMDA receptor complex and trafficking by activity-dependent phosphorylation of the NR2B subunit PDZ ligand. J Neurosci 24:10248–10259.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Comoletti D,
    2. De Jaco A,
    3. Jennings LL,
    4. Flynn RE,
    5. Gaietta G,
    6. Tsigelny I,
    7. Ellisman MH,
    8. Taylor P
    (2004) The Arg451Cys-neuroligin-3 mutation associated with autism reveals a defect in protein processing. J Neurosci 24:4889–4893.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Crawley JN
    (2007) Mouse behavioral assays relevant to the symptoms of autism. Brain Pathol 17:448–459.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Cromwell HC,
    2. Berridge KC
    (1996) Implementation of action sequences by a neostriatal site: a lesion mapping study of grooming syntax. J Neurosci 16:3444–3458.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Dahlhaus R,
    2. Hines RM,
    3. Eadie BD,
    4. Kannangara TS,
    5. Hines DJ,
    6. Brown CE,
    7. Christie BR,
    8. El-Husseini A
    (2010) Overexpression of the cell adhesion protein neuroligin-1 induces learning deficits and impairs synaptic plasticity by altering the ratio of excitation to inhibition in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 20:305–322.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Deacon RM
    (2006) Digging and marble burying in mice: simple methods for in vivo identification of biological impacts. Nat Protoc 1:122–124.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Dean C,
    2. Scholl FG,
    3. Choih J,
    4. DeMaria S,
    5. Berger J,
    6. Isacoff E,
    7. Scheiffele P
    (2003) Neurexin mediates the assembly of presynaptic terminals. Nat Neurosci 6:708–716.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Ding J,
    2. Peterson JD,
    3. Surmeier DJ
    (2008) Corticostriatal and thalamostriatal synapses have distinctive properties. J Neurosci 28:6483–6492.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Durand CM,
    2. Betancur C,
    3. Boeckers TM,
    4. Bockmann J,
    5. Chaste P,
    6. Fauchereau F,
    7. Nygren G,
    8. Rastam M,
    9. Gillberg IC,
    10. Anckarsäter H,
    11. Sponheim E,
    12. Goubran-Botros H,
    13. Delorme R,
    14. Chabane N,
    15. Mouren-Simeoni MC,
    16. de Mas P,
    17. Bieth E,
    18. Rogé B,
    19. Héron D,
    20. Burglen L,
    21. Gillberg C,
    22. Leboyer M,
    23. Bourgeron T
    (2007) Mutations in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are associated with autism spectrum disorders. Nat Genet 39:25–27.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Etherton MR,
    2. Blaiss CA,
    3. Powell CM,
    4. Südhof TC
    (2009) Mouse neurexin-1alpha deletion causes correlated electrophysiological and behavioral changes consistent with cognitive impairments. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:17998–18003.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Feng J,
    2. Schroer R,
    3. Yan J,
    4. Song W,
    5. Yang C,
    6. Bockholt A,
    7. Cook EH Jr.,
    8. Skinner C,
    9. Schwartz CE,
    10. Sommer SS
    (2006) High frequency of neurexin 1beta signal peptide structural variants in patients with autism. Neurosci Lett 409:10–13.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Ferreira SH,
    2. Lorenzetti BB
    (1994) Glutamate spinal retrograde sensitization of primary sensory neurons associated with nociception. Neuropharmacology 33:1479–1485.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Flood JF,
    2. Morley JE,
    3. Lanthorn TH
    (1992) Effect on memory processing by d-cycloserine, an agonist of the NMDA/glycine receptor. Eur J Pharmacol 221:249–254.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Gauthier J,
    2. Bonnel A,
    3. St-Onge J,
    4. Karemera L,
    5. Laurent S,
    6. Mottron L,
    7. Fombonne E,
    8. Joober R,
    9. Rouleau GA
    (2005) NLGN3/NLGN4 gene mutations are not responsible for autism in the Quebec population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 132B:74–75.
    OpenUrl
  27. ↵
    1. Gerrow K,
    2. Romorini S,
    3. Nabi SM,
    4. Colicos MA,
    5. Sala C,
    6. El-Husseini A
    (2006) A preformed complex of postsynaptic proteins is involved in excitatory synapse development. Neuron 49:547–562.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Glessner JT,
    2. Wang K,
    3. Cai G,
    4. Korvatska O,
    5. Kim CE,
    6. Wood S,
    7. Zhang H,
    8. Estes A,
    9. Brune CW,
    10. Bradfield JP,
    11. Imielinski M,
    12. Frackelton EC,
    13. Reichert J,
    14. Crawford EL,
    15. Munson J,
    16. Sleiman PM,
    17. Chiavacci R,
    18. Annaiah K,
    19. Thomas K,
    20. Hou C,
    21. Glaberson W,
    22. et al.
    (2009) Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature 459:569–573.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Graf ER,
    2. Zhang X,
    3. Jin SX,
    4. Linhoff MW,
    5. Craig AM
    (2004) Neurexins induce differentiation of GABA and glutamate postsynaptic specializations via neuroligins. Cell 119:1013–1026.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Ichtchenko K,
    2. Hata Y,
    3. Nguyen T,
    4. Ullrich B,
    5. Missler M,
    6. Moomaw C,
    7. Südhof TC
    (1995) Neuroligin 1: a splice site-specific ligand for beta-neurexins. Cell 81:435–443.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Ichtchenko K,
    2. Nguyen T,
    3. Südhof TC
    (1996) Structures, alternative splicing, and neurexin binding of multiple neuroligins. J Biol Chem 271:2676–2682.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Irie M,
    2. Hata Y,
    3. Takeuchi M,
    4. Ichtchenko K,
    5. Toyoda A,
    6. Hirao K,
    7. Takai Y,
    8. Rosahl TW,
    9. Südhof TC
    (1997) Binding of neuroligins to PSD-95. Science 277:1511–1515.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Jamain S,
    2. Quach H,
    3. Betancur C,
    4. Råstam M,
    5. Colineaux C,
    6. Gillberg IC,
    7. Soderstrom H,
    8. Giros B,
    9. Leboyer M,
    10. Gillberg C,
    11. Bourgeron T
    (2003) Mutations of the X-linked genes encoding neuroligins NLGN3 and NLGN4 are associated with autism. Nat Genet 34:27–29.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Kim HG,
    2. Kishikawa S,
    3. Higgins AW,
    4. Seong IS,
    5. Donovan DJ,
    6. Shen Y,
    7. Lally E,
    8. Weiss LA,
    9. Najm J,
    10. Kutsche K,
    11. Descartes M,
    12. Holt L,
    13. Braddock S,
    14. Troxell R,
    15. Kaplan L,
    16. Volkmar F,
    17. Klin A,
    18. Tsatsanis K,
    19. Harris DJ,
    20. Noens I,
    21. et al.
    (2008a) Disruption of neurexin 1 associated with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Hum Genet 82:199–207.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Kim J,
    2. Jung SY,
    3. Lee YK,
    4. Park S,
    5. Choi JS,
    6. Lee CJ,
    7. Kim HS,
    8. Choi YB,
    9. Scheiffele P,
    10. Bailey CH,
    11. Kandel ER,
    12. Kim JH
    (2008b) Neuroligin-1 is required for normal expression of LTP and associative fear memory in the amygdala of adult animals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:9087–9092.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Konstantareas MM,
    2. Homatidis S
    (1999) Chromosomal abnormalities in a series of children with autistic disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 29:275–285.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Krishnan V,
    2. Han MH,
    3. Graham DL,
    4. Berton O,
    5. Renthal W,
    6. Russo SJ,
    7. Laplant Q,
    8. Graham A,
    9. Lutter M,
    10. Lagace DC,
    11. Ghose S,
    12. Reister R,
    13. Tannous P,
    14. Green TA,
    15. Neve RL,
    16. Chakravarty S,
    17. Kumar A,
    18. Eisch AJ,
    19. Self DW,
    20. Lee FS,
    21. et al.
    (2007) Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell 131:391–404.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Kwon CH,
    2. Luikart BW,
    3. Powell CM,
    4. Zhou J,
    5. Matheny SA,
    6. Zhang W,
    7. Li Y,
    8. Baker SJ,
    9. Parada LF
    (2006) Pten regulates neuronal arborization and social interaction in mice. Neuron 50:377–388.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Laumonnier F,
    2. Bonnet-Brilhault F,
    3. Gomot M,
    4. Blanc R,
    5. David A,
    6. Moizard MP,
    7. Raynaud M,
    8. Ronce N,
    9. Lemonnier E,
    10. Calvas P,
    11. Laudier B,
    12. Chelly J,
    13. Fryns JP,
    14. Ropers HH,
    15. Hamel BC,
    16. Andres C,
    17. Barthélémy C,
    18. Moraine C,
    19. Briault S
    (2004) X-linked mental retardation and autism are associated with a mutation in the NLGN4 gene, a member of the neuroligin family. Am J Hum Genet 74:552–557.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Levinson JN,
    2. Chéry N,
    3. Huang K,
    4. Wong TP,
    5. Gerrow K,
    6. Kang R,
    7. Prange O,
    8. Wang YT,
    9. El-Husseini A
    (2005) Neuroligins mediate excitatory and inhibitory synapse formation: involvement of PSD-95 and neurexin-1beta in neuroligin-induced synaptic specificity. J Biol Chem 280:17312–17319.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    1. Lijam N,
    2. Paylor R,
    3. McDonald MP,
    4. Crawley JN,
    5. Deng CX,
    6. Herrup K,
    7. Stevens KE,
    8. Maccaferri G,
    9. McBain CJ,
    10. Sussman DJ,
    11. Wynshaw-Boris A
    (1997) Social interaction and sensorimotor gating abnormalities in mice lacking Dvl1. Cell 90:895–905.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Lin Y,
    2. Jover-Mengual T,
    3. Wong J,
    4. Bennett MV,
    5. Zukin RS
    (2006) PSD-95 and PKC converge in regulating NMDA receptor trafficking and gating. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:19902–19907.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Lutter M,
    2. Sakata I,
    3. Osborne-Lawrence S,
    4. Rovinsky SA,
    5. Anderson JG,
    6. Jung S,
    7. Birnbaum S,
    8. Yanagisawa M,
    9. Elmquist JK,
    10. Nestler EJ,
    11. Zigman JM
    (2008) The orexigenic hormone ghrelin defends against depressive symptoms of chronic stress. Nat Neurosci 11:752–753.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Moretti P,
    2. Bouwknecht JA,
    3. Teague R,
    4. Paylor R,
    5. Zoghbi HY
    (2005) Abnormalities of social interactions and home-cage behavior in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. Hum Mol Genet 14:205–220.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Moy SS,
    2. Nadler JJ,
    3. Perez A,
    4. Barbaro RP,
    5. Johns JM,
    6. Magnuson TR,
    7. Piven J,
    8. Crawley JN
    (2004) Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes Brain Behav 3:287–302.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Moy SS,
    2. Nadler JJ,
    3. Magnuson TR,
    4. Crawley JN
    (2006) Mouse models of autism spectrum disorders: the challenge for behavioral genetics. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet 142C:40–51.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Myme CI,
    2. Sugino K,
    3. Turrigiano GG,
    4. Nelson SB
    (2003) The NMDA-to-AMPA ratio at synapses onto layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons is conserved across prefrontal and visual cortices. J Neurophysiol 90:771–779.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Nadler JJ,
    2. Moy SS,
    3. Dold G,
    4. Trang D,
    5. Simmons N,
    6. Perez A,
    7. Young NB,
    8. Barbaro RP,
    9. Piven J,
    10. Magnuson TR,
    11. Crawley JN
    (2004) Automated apparatus for quantitation of social approach behaviors in mice. Genes Brain Behav 3:303–314.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Nam CI,
    2. Chen L
    (2005) Postsynaptic assembly induced by neurexin-neuroligin interaction and neurotransmitter. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:6137–6142.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Newpher TM,
    2. Ehlers MD
    (2008) Glutamate receptor dynamics in dendritic microdomains. Neuron 58:472–497.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Njung'e K,
    2. Handley SL
    (1991) Evaluation of marble-burying behavior as a model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 38:63–67.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Posey DJ,
    2. Kem DL,
    3. Swiezy NB,
    4. Sweeten TL,
    5. Wiegand RE,
    6. McDougle CJ
    (2004) A pilot study of d-cycloserine in subjects with autistic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 161:2115–2117.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Powell CM,
    2. Schoch S,
    3. Monteggia L,
    4. Barrot M,
    5. Matos MF,
    6. Feldmann N,
    7. Südhof TC,
    8. Nestler EJ
    (2004) The presynaptic active zone protein RIM1alpha is critical for normal learning and memory. Neuron 42:143–153.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Prange O,
    2. Wong TP,
    3. Gerrow K,
    4. Wang YT,
    5. El-Husseini A
    (2004) A balance between excitatory and inhibitory synapses is controlled by PSD-95 and neuroligin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:13915–13920.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Scheiffele P,
    2. Fan J,
    3. Choih J,
    4. Fetter R,
    5. Serafini T
    (2000) Neuroligin expressed in nonneuronal cells triggers presynaptic development in contacting axons. Cell 101:657–669.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Song JY,
    2. Ichtchenko K,
    3. Südhof TC,
    4. Brose N
    (1999) Neuroligin 1 is a postsynaptic cell-adhesion molecule of excitatory synapses. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:1100–1105.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Südhof TC
    (2008) Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease. Nature 455:903–911.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Szatmari P,
    2. Paterson AD,
    3. Zwaigenbaum L,
    4. Roberts W,
    5. Brian J,
    6. Liu XQ,
    7. Vincent JB,
    8. Skaug JL,
    9. Thompson AP,
    10. Senman L,
    11. Feuk L,
    12. Qian C,
    13. Bryson SE,
    14. Jones MB,
    15. Marshall CR,
    16. Scherer SW,
    17. Vieland VJ,
    18. Bartlett C,
    19. Mangin LV,
    20. Goedken R
    (2007) Mapping autism risk loci using genetic linkage and chromosomal rearrangements. Nat Genet 39:319–328.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Tabuchi K,
    2. Blundell J,
    3. Etherton MR,
    4. Hammer RE,
    5. Liu X,
    6. Powell CM,
    7. Südhof TC
    (2007) A neuroligin-3 mutation implicated in autism increases inhibitory synaptic transmission in mice. Science 318:71–76.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Talebizadeh Z,
    2. Bittel DC,
    3. Veatch OJ,
    4. Butler MG,
    5. Takahashi TN,
    6. Miles JH
    (2004) Do known mutations in neuroligin genes (NLGN3 and NLGN4) cause autism? J Autism Dev Disord 34:735–736.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Tan-No K,
    2. Taira A,
    3. Wako K,
    4. Niijima F,
    5. Nakagawasai O,
    6. Tadano T,
    7. Sakurada C,
    8. Sakurada T,
    9. Kisara K
    (2000) Intrathecally administered spermine produces the scratching, biting and licking behaviour in mice. Pain 86:55–61.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Thomas A,
    2. Burant A,
    3. Bui N,
    4. Graham D,
    5. Yuva-Paylor LA,
    6. Paylor R
    (2009) Marble burying reflects a repetitive and perseverative behavior more than novelty-induced anxiety. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 204:361–373.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  63. ↵
    1. Tsankova NM,
    2. Berton O,
    3. Renthal W,
    4. Kumar A,
    5. Neve RL,
    6. Nestler EJ
    (2006) Sustained hippocampal chromatin regulation in a mouse model of depression and antidepressant action. Nat Neurosci 9:519–525.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Varoqueaux F,
    2. Jamain S,
    3. Brose N
    (2004) Neuroligin 2 is exclusively localized to inhibitory synapses. Eur J Cell Biol 83:449–456.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Varoqueaux F,
    2. Aramuni G,
    3. Rawson RL,
    4. Mohrmann R,
    5. Missler M,
    6. Gottmann K,
    7. Zhang W,
    8. Südhof TC,
    9. Brose N
    (2006) Neuroligins determine synapse maturation and function. Neuron 51:741–754.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Vincent JB,
    2. Kolozsvari D,
    3. Roberts WS,
    4. Bolton PF,
    5. Gurling HM,
    6. Scherer SW
    (2004) Mutation screening of X-chromosomal neuroligin genes: no mutations in 196 autism probands. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 129B:82–84.
    OpenUrl
  67. ↵
    1. Volk LJ,
    2. Pfeiffer BE,
    3. Gibson JR,
    4. Huber KM
    (2007) Multiple Gq-coupled receptors converge on a common protein synthesis-dependent long-term depression that is affected in fragile X syndrome mental retardation. J Neurosci 27:11624–11634.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    1. Welch JM,
    2. Lu J,
    3. Rodriguiz RM,
    4. Trotta NC,
    5. Peca J,
    6. Ding JD,
    7. Feliciano C,
    8. Chen M,
    9. Adams JP,
    10. Luo J,
    11. Dudek SM,
    12. Weinberg RJ,
    13. Calakos N,
    14. Wetsel WC,
    15. Feng G
    (2007) Cortico-striatal synaptic defects and OCD-like behaviours in Sapap3-mutant mice. Nature 448:894–900.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Yan J,
    2. Oliveira G,
    3. Coutinho A,
    4. Yang C,
    5. Feng J,
    6. Katz C,
    7. Sram J,
    8. Bockholt A,
    9. Jones IR,
    10. Craddock N,
    11. Cook EH Jr.,
    12. Vicente A,
    13. Sommer SS
    (2005) Analysis of the neuroligin 3 and 4 genes in autism and other neuropsychiatric patients. Mol Psychiatry 10:329–332.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Yan J,
    2. Feng J,
    3. Schroer R,
    4. Li W,
    5. Skinner C,
    6. Schwartz CE,
    7. Cook EH Jr.,
    8. Sommer SS
    (2008a) Analysis of the neuroligin 4Y gene in patients with autism. Psychiatr Genet 18:204–207.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Yan J,
    2. Noltner K,
    3. Feng J,
    4. Li W,
    5. Schroer R,
    6. Skinner C,
    7. Zeng W,
    8. Schwartz CE,
    9. Sommer SS
    (2008b) Neurexin 1alpha structural variants associated with autism. Neurosci Lett 438:368–370.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Yan QJ,
    2. Asafo-Adjei PK,
    3. Arnold HM,
    4. Brown RE,
    5. Bauchwitz RP
    (2004) A phenotypic and molecular characterization of the fmr1-tm1Cgr fragile X mouse. Genes Brain Behav 3:337–359.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Ylisaukko-oja T,
    2. Rehnström K,
    3. Auranen M,
    4. Vanhala R,
    5. Alen R,
    6. Kempas E,
    7. Ellonen P,
    8. Turunen JA,
    9. Makkonen I,
    10. Riikonen R,
    11. Nieminen-von Wendt T,
    12. von Wendt L,
    13. Peltonen L,
    14. Järvelä I
    (2005) Analysis of four neuroligin genes as candidates for autism. Eur J Hum Genet 13:1285–1292.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Zlomuzica A,
    2. De Souza Silva MA,
    3. Huston JP,
    4. Dere E
    (2007) NMDA receptor modulation by D-cycloserine promotes episodic-like memory in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 193:503–509.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Zoghbi HY
    (2003) Postnatal neurodevelopmental disorders: meeting at the synapse? Science 302:826–830.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  76. ↵
    1. Züchner S,
    2. Wendland JR,
    3. Ashley-Koch AE,
    4. Collins AL,
    5. Tran-Viet KN,
    6. Quinn K,
    7. Timpano KC,
    8. Cuccaro ML,
    9. Pericak-Vance MA,
    10. Steffens DC,
    11. Krishnan KR,
    12. Feng G,
    13. Murphy DL
    (2009) Multiple rare SAPAP3 missense variants in trichotillomania and OCD. Mol Psychiatry 14:6–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 30 (6)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 30, Issue 6
10 Feb 2010
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Neuroligin-1 Deletion Results in Impaired Spatial Memory and Increased Repetitive Behavior
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Neuroligin-1 Deletion Results in Impaired Spatial Memory and Increased Repetitive Behavior
Jacqueline Blundell, Cory A. Blaiss, Mark R. Etherton, Felipe Espinosa, Katsuhiko Tabuchi, Christopher Walz, Marc F. Bolliger, Thomas C. Südhof, Craig M. Powell
Journal of Neuroscience 10 February 2010, 30 (6) 2115-2129; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4517-09.2010

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Neuroligin-1 Deletion Results in Impaired Spatial Memory and Increased Repetitive Behavior
Jacqueline Blundell, Cory A. Blaiss, Mark R. Etherton, Felipe Espinosa, Katsuhiko Tabuchi, Christopher Walz, Marc F. Bolliger, Thomas C. Südhof, Craig M. Powell
Journal of Neuroscience 10 February 2010, 30 (6) 2115-2129; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4517-09.2010
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
  • Generation of Intensity Selectivity by Differential Synaptic Tuning: Fast-Saturating Excitation But Slow-Saturating Inhibition
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.