Abstract
The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is exquisitely sensitive to its neurochemical environment. Minor fluctuations in cortical dopamine (DA) can profoundly alter working memory, a PFC-dependent cognitive function that supports an array of essential human behaviors. Dopamine's action in the PFC follows an inverted U-shaped curve, where an optimal DA level results in maximal function and insufficient or excessive DA impairs PFC function. In animals, 17β-estradiol (the major estrogen in most mammals, referred to henceforth as estradiol) has been shown to enhance DA activity, yet no human study has adequately addressed whether estradiol's impact on cognition occurs by way of modulating specific neurochemical systems. Here we examined the effects of endogenous fluctuations in estradiol on working memory in healthy young women as a function of baseline PFC DA [indexed by catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met genotype and, at a finer scale, COMT enzyme activity]. The results demonstrate that estradiol status impacts working memory function and, crucially, the direction of the effect depends on indices of baseline DA. Moreover, consistent with a DA cortical efficiency hypothesis, functional MRI revealed that inferred optimal DA was associated with reduced PFC activity sustained across task blocks and selectively enhanced PFC activity on trials with the greatest demand for cognitive control. The magnitude of PFC activity during high control trials was predictive of an individual's performance. These findings show that although estrogen, considered in isolation, may have unpredictable effects on cognitive performance, its influence is clarified when considered within a larger neuromodulatory framework. Given the clinical prevalence of dopaminergic drugs, understanding the relationship between estrogen and DA is essential for advancing women's health.
Introduction
Working memory (WM) is a fundamental cognitive ability that supports an array of complex abilities, from problem solving to fluid intelligence (Barrett et al., 2004). Dopamine (DA) signaling in prefrontal cortex (PFC) is critical for WM function (Brozoski et al., 1979; Sawaguchi and Goldman-Rakic, 1991; Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1995). Dopamine's relationship to performance on frontally mediated cognitive tasks is not linear; DA function follows an inverted U-shaped curve, where optimal DA results in maximal function and insufficient or excessive levels lead to PFC dysfunction (Cai and Arnsten, 1997; Zahrt et al., 1997; Granon et al., 2000; Egan et al., 2001; Gibbs and D'Esposito, 2005a; Vijayraghavan et al., 2007; Williams-Gray et al., 2007). Thus, taking basal DA levels into account is essential for predicting how DA augmentation (e.g., via a drug) will affect cognitive performance (Egan et al., 2001).
Individual differences in baseline PFC DA stem, in part, from genetic alterations that impact the DA system. The catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene codes for an enzyme that metabolizes released DA. COMT enzyme is crucial for regulating frontal DA transmission, accounting for >60% of total DA turnover in PFC [compared with 15% in the striatum (Männistö and Kaakkola, 1999)]. A recent COMT gene polymorphism (Val158Met) alters the efficiency of the eponymous enzyme; compared with the ancestral val/val allelic variant, met/met individuals have decreased COMT activity, enhanced PFC-dependent cognitive function, and greater cortical efficiency (Egan et al., 2001; Tunbridge et al., 2006).
In animals, strong evidence indicates that estradiol enhances DA activity on rapid and protracted timescales (Thompson and Moss, 1994; Becker, 2000). Estradiol enhances DA synthesis, release, and turnover and modifies basal firing rates of DA neurons via membrane estrogen receptors (Xiao and Becker, 1994; Pasqualini et al., 1995; Becker, 1990, 2000). Some evidence links estrogen and WM function; for example, improvements in WM have been observed in postmenopausal women on estrogen replacement compared with nonusers, but the data are inconsistent (Duff and Hampson, 2000; Sherwin, 2005). Some evidence suggests that WM span fluctuates throughout the estrogen cycle (Rosenberg and Park, 2002), but here too the data are inconsistent (Gasbarri et al., 2008). The complicated relationship between estrogen and WM may stem from unaccounted variability in baseline DA across individuals. Accordingly, a study in rats showed that the administration of a DA drug had different effects on WM performance depending on the rats' estrous stage at the time of testing (Shansky et al., 2004).
Evidence for an estradiol–DA link exists in animals, yet no human study has examined whether DA mediates estradiol's effects on PFC function. Given that estradiol levels may be higher in the PFC than any other cortical area (Bixo et al., 1995), it is likely that estradiol's effects on WM function are mediated, in part, through modulation of PFC DA activity. Thus, we predicted that performance on tasks that are sensitive to PFC DA signaling would vary throughout the menstrual cycle. Importantly, we predicted that estradiol's behavioral effects would not be consistent unless individual variation in baseline DA was accounted for. Here we examine estradiol's effects on WM behavior and neural activity as a function of COMT genotype and enzyme activity.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
Seventy-nine healthy female participants (mean age, 21.7 ± 2.4 years) were recruited via advertisements on the UC Berkeley campus and prescreened for COMT Val158Met polymorphism. Exclusionary criteria included any history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, an episode of loss of consciousness, use of psychotropic drugs, a history of substance abuse, MRI contraindications, abnormal or infrequent menstrual cycle, and/or use of a hormonal birth control. Blood samples were collected by a licensed phlebotomist and sent for DNA extraction and genotyping. The menstrual cycles of homozygous participants were tracked for ≥4 months (across at least four menstruation periods) to select subjects with the most regular cycles, who were then invited for inclusion in the central study. Twenty-four women were enrolled: 13 val/val, eight met/met, and three val/met (the three heterozygotes were enrolled before their genotype was known; their data are included in analyses assessing the effects of estradiol regardless of genotype). Subjects underwent both behavioral and fMRI testing on two occasions (two val/val and two val/met subjects did not return for the second session), resulting in the total acquisition of 44 datasets. Of the 24 subjects, 21 had no history of smoking and three reported having previously smoked (>4 months before study enrollment), albeit infrequently (less than four cigarettes a week). The study was approved by the UC Berkeley Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects. All volunteers gave written informed consent and were paid for their participation. Ethnicity was matched between genotypic groups to minimize population stratification effects. Of met/met participants, 63% self-identified as Asian American and 37% identified as Caucasian American. The numbers are nearly identical for val/val participants, with 62% Asian American and 38% Caucasian American. (The data reflect the UC Berkeley student population from which participants were recruited.) Indices of parental socioeconomic status were not obtained; educational status did not differ between participants.
Experimental design.
Following inclusion in the main study, subjects were seen on three occasions. On the first visit, exclusionary criteria were reviewed, consent for the main study was obtained, and the participants completed an introductory neuropsychological exam. The second and third visits were time-locked to each subject's menstrual cycle. One test session occurred on or near day 1 or 2 of the subject's cycle (during menses, when estradiol levels are low) and the other session occurred on day 11 or 12 (late follicular phase when estradiol levels peak); the order was counter-balanced across subjects. This timing assumes a 28 d cycle. The period between the start of menses and ovulation (follicular phase) can vary in duration, contributing to a longer or shorter overall cycle duration. The period from ovulation to the start of the next cycle (luteal phase) is less variable, with an average duration of 14 d. Subjects' menstrual cycles were tracked (including start and duration of menses) for ≥4 months before testing through 1 month posttesting to confirm cycles were predictable and regular. Blood samples were acquired on both test days to measure estradiol and COMT enzyme levels. Samples were collected following, at minimum, 4 h of no food or drink consumption (excluding water).
Neuropsychological measures.
The initial neuropsychological exam included the following: (1) the North American Adult Reading Test (Nelson, 1982), (2) the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961), (3) a paper-and-pen version of the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935), (4) the controlled oral-word association test (average number of words generated in 60 s for the letters F, A, and S and categories animals and fruits) (Benton, 1968), (5) the Montreal Cognitive Assessment test (to assess mild cognitive impairment) (Nasreddine et al., 2005), and (6) the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (Patton et al., 1995). Listening span (Salthouse and Babcock, 1991) and automated reading span (Daneman and Carpenter, 1980; Unsworth et al., 2005) were collected in a subsample of 19 subjects on a separate day. Genotypic groups did not differ significantly with respect to age, education, or any background neuropsychological measure.
Test day measures.
During the low and high estradiol test sessions, subjects completed an N-back verbal working memory task during fMRI scanning. To assess mood, subjects completed a positive and negative affect scale (Watson et al., 1988) and visual analog scales for the items anxious, happy, sad, nauseous, drowsy, jittery, fatigued, and dizzy, pre- and post-fMRI scan. Motor speed was measured with a box completion task and vigilance was measured with a numeral cancellation task (Lewis and Kupke, 1977).
Cognitive paradigm performed during fMRI scanning.
The N-back verbal working memory task varied by load (0-, 2-, 3-back) and trial type (nonlure, target, lure) (Fig. 1A). Subjects were presented with a series of single consonants that appeared on the screen every 2 s, one after the other. Using two buttons, subjects responded to every letter to indicate whether it matched or did not match the letter seen n previously. A target was defined as a letter that matched the letter seen n-previously; for example, in a 3-back condition, the second R in the sequence R-T-K-R-D is the target. Lure trials matched a recently seen but nontarget letter. For example, in a 3-back condition, the second D in the sequence D-T-K-R-D is a lure; it matches the letter seen four letters earlier but not three, as the condition specifies. All other nontarget trials were categorized as nonlures. In contrast to targets and nonlures, lures create high interference as they give a spurious sense of familiarity, and place the greatest demand on cognitive control mechanisms (Gray et al., 2003). Previous data demonstrate that lure trials are uniquely sensitive to differences in PFC-dependent processes within and between groups (Perlstein et al., 2001, 2004) and individual differences in fluid intelligence (Gray et al., 2003). Lure interference effects are also more pronounced in older adults, although target performance is unimpaired (Schmiedek et al., 2009).
On 0-back blocks, subjects indicated whether or not a target letter (X) appeared. Zero-back blocks consisted of targets and nontargets; lure trials were not relevant in this condition. The task was set up as 16 blocks of trials (eight 0-back blocks, four 2-back blocks, and four 3-back blocks) ordered in a Latin squares sequence. Subjects were instructed on the condition before each block of trials. A block consisted of 20 trials plus five randomly placed null events to introduce temporal jitter (necessary for event-related fMRI analyses). A trial consisted of a stimulus presented for 1 s followed by a 1 s delay. A null event was a 2 s blank period (the same duration as a regular trial). Trial types included targets (20%), lures (15%), and nonlures (65%). Each block of trials (50 s) was preceded by instructions (6 s) and fixation (10 s) and followed by a brief period of rest (20 s).
Genetic analysis.
DNA extraction and analysis was conducted according to standard methods on samples obtained from informed, consenting subjects. The Ernest Gallo Clinic and Research Center Genomics Core (Emeryville, CA) performed genotyping of the COMT Val158/Met (rs4680) polymorphism with PCR using TaqMan technology (Applied Biosystems). The COMT genotype frequencies of prescreened subjects were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (χ2 = 2.01, df = 1, not significant). The genotype distribution was as follows: val/val, 26 subjects; val/met, 33 subjects; met/met, 20 subjects).
COMT enzyme analysis.
One 4–5 ml blood sample was collected in a vacutainer EDTA tube (BD Diagnostic Systems) on each test day by a licensed phlebotomist to measure COMT enzyme levels. Genotyping the Val158Met polymorphism provides a static determination of genotype. COMT function is determined by the extent to which the gene gets translated into an active protein—the COMT enzyme. Enzymatic activity is strongly related to COMT genotype but can vary even within the static designations of val/val, val/met, and met/met. Assaying blood COMT enzyme levels provides an additional, more direct measure of COMT activity than assaying genotype alone and reflects brain COMT levels (Chen et al., 2004). COMT enzyme activity was determined in whole peripheral blood collected on each scan day. The COMT enzyme activity assay is based on the organic solvent extraction method that separates the radioactive product; the methylated catechol; and the free radioactive coenzyme, 3H-S-adenosyl-methionine, as described previously (Apud et al., 2007). Determination of whole-blood COMT activity confirmed that met/met samples (N = 14) had greatly reduced COMT activity (23,408 ± 2465 cpm mg protein, mean ± SEM) compared with val/val samples (N = 23) (51,474 ± 3688 cpm mg protein; t(35) = 5.480; p < 0.0001). Variance in the amount of active COMT protein is evident within genotypic designations and is likely due to epigenetic or epistatic interactions.
Estradiol assay/menstrual cycle phase monitoring.
One 4–5 ml blood sample was collected in a vacutainer SST tube (BD Diagnostic Systems) on each test day by a licensed phlebotomist to measure serum estradiol levels. Samples were stored on ice until centrifugation, after which serum was collected and stored at −20°C until assayed. 17β-Estradiol concentrations were determined using a commercially available enzyme immunoassay (ELISA; Calbiotech). The assay sensitivity was 10 pg/ml and the intra-assay coefficient of variation was 2.26%. All samples were run in a single assay and all instructions provided by the manufacturer were followed without modification. Menses/early follicular phase testing was first confirmed from cycle records: all low estradiol test sessions occurred within 3 d of the start of menses (duration of menses across subjects ranged from 3 to 7 d). Determination of serum estradiol levels confirmed that subjects tested during the early follicular phase had reduced levels of circulating estradiol (137.5 ± 58.9 pg/ml) compared with the late-follicular phase (251 ± 178 pg/ml). Thirty-seven samples were assayed. Samples from seven test sessions (four low estradiol sessions and three high estradiol sessions) were not assayed due to subject constraints (difficulty acquiring an adequate blood sample) or the sample being unusable (problems extracting serum from whole blood). A record of those subjects' menstrual cycles confirmed that the four low sessions for which blood samples were not assayed occurred while the subject was in menses.
fMRI data acquisition and analysis.
MRI data were acquired with a Siemens 3T Trio Magnetom scanner. Functional data were obtained using a T2* weighted echo-planar imaging sequence sensitive to blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast (repetition time, 2000 ms; echo time, 28 ms; field of view, 230 mm; flip angle, 90°; voxel size, 2.0 × 1.8 × 3.0). Each functional volume consisted of 29 3 mm oblique axial slices separated by a 0.45 mm interslice gap. A high-resolution T1 (MPRAGE) anatomical scan was also acquired. Following acquisition, MRI data were converted to Nifti format; processing included correction for slice-timing and motion artifacts, spatial normalization, and spatial smoothing with a three-dimensional Gaussian kernel (8 mm full-width half-maximum). For spatial normalization, the individual subject's MPRAGE was coregistered to the mean functional image and normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) structural template. Normalization parameters were then written to the functional images. Time series were high-pass filtered (240 s cutoff). Statistical parametric maps of BOLD activation were calculated in SPM5 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) using the general linear model approach (Worsley and Friston, 1995).
The main analysis focused on BOLD activity in load-sensitive regions within PFC. Statistical maps representing areas with linear increases of activity across memory load (3- > 2- > 0-back) were generated at the random effects level (family-wise error p value < 0.00001). The most significant load-sensitive PFC region (that which exceeded a threshold of T = 8) was within middle frontal gyrus (MFG). Thus, this region was the focus of our current analyses, consistent with previous COMT N-back findings (Egan et al., 2001; Mattay et al., 2003), which localize COMT genotype effects to dorsolateral PFC. To capture the true linear relationship between load conditions, the conditions were weighted in the contrast as follows: 0-back (−5/3), 2-back (1/3), 3-back (4/3). Bilateral PFC regions of interest (ROIs) were defined as 10 mm spheres around peak loci. The statistical model was then reapplied to the average BOLD signal within the ROI using MarsBar (www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/marsbar.html). Mean beta weights from the ROIs defined from the load contrast were extracted and compared directly between genotype (val, met) and estradiol (low, high) groups and entered into an ANOVA to examine estradiol × genotype effects. Event-related analyses modeled only correct trials to avoid error-related confounds.
To obtain additional evidence for a relationship between DA and sustained PFC function, we entered COMT enzyme as a covariate of interest at the group level. Whole-brain maps revealed a specific, significant relationship between COMT enzymatic activity and left PFC activity, with no other association across the brain. To probe this finding further in an unbiased manner, we used an independently defined left and right PFC ROI from a meta-analysis of N-back activations observed across 24 fMRI studies (Owen et al., 2005) and reapplied the statistical model (parametric load contrast) to the average signal across the ROI [10 mm sphere around coordinates; left dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC): −44, 17, 25; right dlPFC: 40, 31, 34; MNI space) for each subject's session. Data from the left and right PFC ROI were extracted and the mean signal values were submitted to Pearson's product–moment correlation.
Results
Predictions
We predicted that val/val subjects (low baseline DA) would show improved WM performance when estradiol levels were high versus low, whereas met/met subjects (high DA) would show the opposite pattern, with the best performance occurring when estradiol levels were low (Fig. 1B). These predictions were based on pharmacological COMT studies showing that val/val individuals improve on PFC-dependent tasks after administration of a DA agonist, whereas met/met individuals are impaired by the same drug (Egan et al., 2001). Considering the estrogen–cognition relationship within a model of PFC DA function allows us to address, with more precision, how estradiol impacts DA-dependent neural processes. Demand for cognitive control during the WM task increases across trial type, from nonlures (lowest demand) to targets to lures (highest demand) (Gray et al., 2003) and across load (Braver et al., 1997). Given evidence that increased WM demands lead to increased synaptic DA release in PFC (Watanabe et al., 1997; Phillips et al., 2004; Aalto et al., 2005), a further prediction was that the most pronounced gene/hormone effects would occur during lure trials or during high-load blocks, when cognitive control demands are highest. At a neural level, we predicted that increased synaptic DA would be associated with greater neural efficiency (i.e., at an equal level of performance, a high DA state would increase the signal-to-noise ratio of cortical processing, decreasing the extent of PFC activity), in keeping with previous COMT and patient findings (Egan et al., 2001; Tunbridge et al., 2006).
Subjective effects
No differences were observed between genotypic groups (val, met) or estradiol status (low, high) on background demographic or neuropsychological measures (including age, education, Beck's Depression Inventory, Montreal Cognitive Assessment, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, letter fluency, and North American Adult Reading Test), or measures of mood (visual analog scales for the items anxious, happy, sad, nauseous, drowsy, jittery, fatigued, and dizzy), affect (positive and negative affect scale), motor speed, and vigilance administered before and after MRI scanning on each test day.
Behavioral results
Parametric effects of N-back WM load (Fig. 2A) and trial type (Fig. 2B) were strongly reflected in performance. No group differences were observed for overall task performance (i.e., block-level performance). The overall mean accuracies (response times) across groups were as follows: val/val, 75.9% (546.3 ms); met/met, 75.6% (560.8 ms); low estradiol, 76.1% (561.9); high estradiol, 75.4% (547.4 ms). At the trial level, group differences in performance were not significant when examining estradiol status (low, high) and genotype (val, met) alone. Performance differences emerged when examining the impact of estradiol and genotype together, as predicted. On the high-interference/high-control lure trials during the 2-back condition, val/val women performed poorly at the start of their cycle when estradiol was low and improved when estradiol levels were high. met/met women showed the opposite pattern, with strong performance under low estradiol conditions and impaired performance under high estradiol conditions (Fig. 2C). Comparing the low DA (val, low estradiol) and optimal DA (met, low estradiol) groups shows a significant difference in 2-back lure accuracy. In the 3-back condition, lure performance was poor across all subjects (mean accuracy: 2-back lure, 84%; 3-back lure, 67%) and the effects were not significant as a function of genotype. Of note, the 3-back lure data appear to fit the inverted U-shaped theoretical model (Fig. 1B) if the predicted task-induced shift in DA (Watanabe et al., 1997; Phillips et al., 2004; Aalto et al., 2005) is weighted more strongly (i.e., for the condition that places the greatest demand on cognitive control, 3-back lures, val and met groups are shifted farther along the DA axis).
We also examined lure accuracy within low and high estradiol groups as a function of COMT enzyme level (which provides a measure of the biologically active protein above and beyond static genotype) (Fig. 2D). For high estradiol subjects, COMT enzyme activity was positively correlated with lure accuracy; for low estradiol subjects, enzyme levels were negatively correlated with lure accuracy (Fig. 2E). This behavioral finding is consistent with an inverted U-shaped function at the individual subject level. That is, just before ovulation, when estradiol levels are elevated, the best performers were women with high COMT activity (indicative of low baseline PFC DA). Conversely, toward the beginning of the cycle, when estradiol levels are reduced, the best performers were women with low COMT activity (high baseline PFC DA), in keeping with DA's inverted U-shaped action (Egan et al., 2001; Mattay et al., 2003).
Functional MRI results
To investigate the neural effects underlying hormone–genotype interactions, we examined BOLD activity in load-sensitive PFC regions, which reflects areas involved in WM maintenance processes. Despite the observation that behavioral performance across loads did not differ between groups, by examining PFC activity sustained across task blocks, we found that bilateral MFG activity markedly declined as inferred PFC DA increased. Higher DA (high estradiol, met genotype, or both) was associated with lower sustained PFC activation, whereas lower DA (low estradiol, val genotype, or both) was associated with greater sustained PFC activation (Fig. 3B). Parameter estimates, or mean beta values, in right MFG (Fig. 3A) differed as a function of genotype (val, met) (p = 0.038) and estradiol status (low, high) (p = 0.011). Similar results were observed in left MFG.
Furthermore, average signal magnitude in left MFG correlated with COMT activity (r = 0.513, p < 0.001; right MFG did not reach significance) (Fig. 3C), suggesting that, as the amount of DA available in PFC declines (that is, as COMT activity increases), PFC activity is enhanced.
We examined activity during high-interference/high-control lure trials and lower-interference target trials within the WM load-sensitive PFC ROIs. Target-related activity showed the same pattern of decreased bilateral MFG activity with increased PFC DA as observed at the sustained-activity block level (Fig. 4A). Lure trials showed a different pattern (Fig. 4B), where the magnitude of MFG activation mirrored the inverted U-shaped performance curve (i.e., there was a selective increase in activity for higher, more optimal DA groups: val/val plus low estradiol compared with met/met plus low estradiol; p = 0.028). Moreover, the greater a subject's MFG response to lure trials, the better their lure performance (2-back lures: r = 0.532, p < 0.005; total lures: r = 0.392, p = 0.009) (Fig. 4C).
To further probe the connection between PFC DA, MFG activity, and lure performance at the single subject level, we compared COMT enzyme with lure activity within these PFC ROIs. In right MFG, activity during lure trials was negatively correlated with COMT enzyme (r = 0.425, p = 0.006; the association did not meet significance within the left MFG ROI) (Fig. 4D). In the right MFG, the relationship was significant for both 2-back (r = 0.334, p = 0.035) and 3-back (r = 0.4, p = 0.01) lure trials.
In summary, the fMRI findings suggest that cortical DA is associated with a broadly efficient pattern of sustained activity (that which occurs across WM blocks), and a selective, event-related enhancement of activity during episodes of high interference (e.g., lures), when the demand for cognitive control is greatest. Furthermore, the extent to which an individual enhances PFC activation during the demanding lure trials is predictive of their performance.
Discussion
In humans, there is a strong effort to understand the effects of estrogen on cognition, but the data have been inconsistent. This study suggests that taking baseline DA into account is crucial for detecting the direction of estrogen's effect on WM. Specifically, the results establish that estradiol can be beneficial or detrimental to WM and, critically, the direction of the effect depends on COMT genotype and, on a finer scale, COMT enzymatic activity (proxies of baseline PFC DA). At the behavioral level, the effects of estradiol and COMT genotype emerged on trials that required a high demand of cognitive control. Neurally, hormonal and genotypic differences were apparent even in the absence of significant behavioral differences. Suboptimal DA subjects showed greater task-related PFC activity across blocks but reduced PFC activity on the high interference lure trials. Optimal DA subjects showed the opposite pattern. Conceptually similar results with the same cognitive task were observed in a study of the neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence (gF) (Gray et al., 2003), in which low gF subjects (like our suboptimal DA subjects) showed enhanced sustained PFC activity during a WM task but paradoxically reduced activity during high-control interference trials. High gF individuals showed the opposite pattern (like our optimal DA subjects).
Our findings are in keeping with work from Egan et al. (2001), who demonstrated that Met allele load predicts the efficiency of PFC physiology. Although all genotypic groups in their sample (val/val, val/met, met/met) showed similar levels of performance on a 2-back WM task, task-related PFC BOLD activity was greatest for val/val subjects, reduced for val/met, and lowest for met/met subjects. In a related pharmacogenomic study, Mattay et al. (2003) found that amphetamine administration improved WM performance and enhanced cortical efficiency for val/val subjects. met/met subjects, however, became cortically inefficient on drug, presumably because near-optimal basal DA levels were heightened beyond the optimal range. Similarly, Mehta et al. (2000) showed that the indirect catecholamine agonist methylphenidate (Ritalin) improves WM performance and reduces task-related regional cerebral blood flow in the PFC. These beneficial drug effects were most pronounced for subjects with lower baseline WM capacities (a likely behavioral index of DA synthesis capacity) (Cools et al., 2008). Similar results were found using the D2 receptor agonist bromocriptine (Kimberg et al., 1997; Gibbs and D'Esposito, 2005b; Cools et al., 2007). Together, these findings contribute to a body of evidence demonstrating that optimal DA levels are associated with greater cortical efficiency (i.e., at a given level of performance, DA optimizes the signal-to-noise ratio of cortical processing, decreasing the extent of task-related PFC activity as measured by BOLD) (Durstewitz and Seamans, 2002; Winterer and Weinberger, 2004; Vijayraghavan et al., 2007). The relationship between DA, WM, and cortical efficiency has been demonstrated in humans using pharmacological manipulation of DA; we observe a strikingly similar relationship when considering the natural hormonal fluctuations that occur over the coarse of a woman's menstrual cycle. Further, we found that estradiol's DA agonist-like effect on behavioral and neural processes depends on COMT Val158Met genotype and, at an individual level, COMT enzymatic activity, which suggests a dependence on baseline PFC DA.
The importance of ovarian hormones to the neurophysiology underlying prefrontal function was established by Berman and colleagues (1997). The authors examined the influence of gonadal hormones on regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) by suppressing endogenous ovarian activity and reintroducing exogenous estradiol and progesterone. Task-evoked prefrontal rCBF patterns were dramatically reduced under hormone-suppression conditions and normalized after hormone replacement. This paradigm provides excellent control for the manipulation of gonadal hormones. Menstrual cycle studies depend on the endogenous fluctuation of sex hormones, and the timing of hormone fluctuation varies between women and between cycles. In the present study, women were tracked for ≥4 months to confirm the consistency of their cycle and to infer hormone status at the time of testing (supplemented by direct estradiol measurement from serum samples), but this paradigm is limited in its ability to tease apart the concerted action of multiple sex hormones.
Norepinephrine (NE) also plays a key role in WM mechanisms (Li and Mei, 1994; Li et al., 1999; Ramos et al., 2005) and our results do not rule out the possibility that NE signaling, and modulation thereof, is part of estradiol's underlying mechanism for modulating PFC function. However, in this study, the effects of estradiol were highly dependent on COMT. COMT inactivates catecholamines, including DA and NE, via o-methylation (Männistö and Kaakkola, 1999). Evidence suggests that COMT's role in metabolizing NE in PFC is minor compared with its role in DA metabolism. Administration of tolcapone, a brain-penetrant COMT inhibitor, enhances extracellular DA in rat medial PFC with little to no change in NE (measured following stimulation of the catecholamine system) (Tunbridge et al., 2004). Furthermore, Huotari et al. (2002) observed that, following l-DOPA administration, COMT knock-out mice show increased accumulation of cortical DA compared with wild-type animals; cortical NE levels did not differ significantly across groups. Together, these data suggest that COMT predominantly impacts PFC DA, with minimal influence over NE signaling.
GABA, the principle inhibitory neurotransmitter, has also been shown to fluctuate over the menstrual cycle (Epperson et al., 2002; for review, see Cosgrove et al., 2007) and mounting evidence demonstrates that estradiol modulates glutamatergic activity (McCarthy et al., 2002). Thus, estradiol's influence on cognition cannot be fully understood without taking other neurochemical systems into account. Moreover, estradiol does not exert its effect in isolation; other gonadal hormones such as progesterone likely modulate PFC function (Berman et al., 1997). In vitro and in vivo experimental paradigms in animals and carefully designed studies in humans are critical for clarifying the complex relationship between endogenous fluctuations in neuroactive gonadal hormones, neurochemical signaling, and cognition (Aubele and Kritzer, 2011).
The results of our study are in keeping with work in rodents and nonhuman primates showing that estradiol enhances DA activity, but a definitive study showing an estradiol–DA link in humans is lacking. Further studies are needed to directly assess estradiol's effect on DA neurotransmission in humans. The only method currently available to assess DA release in vivo in humans combines pharmacological stimulation of the DA system with positron emission tomography, in conjunction with a DA-receptor radioligand such as [11C]-raclopride (Laruelle, 2000; Cropley et al., 2006).
Although no study has compared estradiol levels (Bixo et al., 1995) or the pattern of estrogen receptor (ER) expression across specific subregions of PFC in humans, quantitative morphometric studies in nonhuman primates demonstrate estradiol's influence on synaptic organization within dorsolateral PFC (Kritzer and Kohama, 1998), a region that expresses ERα (Perlman et al., 2005; Montague et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2010). Specifically, when ovariectomized monkeys were treated with estradiol (using a cyclical regimen that mirrors the preovulatory spike in estradiol), striking increases in dendritic spine density were observed in dlPFC layer III pyramidal neurons (Hao et al., 2006). These physiological changes were accompanied by improved performance on a delayed-response working-memory task (Rapp et al., 2003). Importantly, the estradiol-dependent effects we observed in humans using fMRI BOLD are also in dlPFC. Thus, the relationship between estradiol, DA, and working memory is underscored by the overlapping neural proximity with which these changes are observed.
Another important clue into estradiol's site of action comes from a series of immunocytochemistry experiments showing ERβ expression in midbrain dopaminergic neurons that project to PFC and constitute the mesocortical DA pathway (Creutz and Kritzer, 2002). Until recently it was thought that ∼30% of cells within the rat mesocortical pathway were dopaminergic (TH-immunoreactive) (Swanson, 1982). However, the data came solely from males and a recent study discovered that females have a significantly greater proportion of dopaminergic cells, ∼50%, within this pathway (Kritzer and Creutz, 2008). Sex differences in mesocortical organization may carry functional consequences for cognitive processes that rely on PFC DA signaling.
Collectively, these data carry direct ramifications for women's health. The response to DA medications (e.g., Ritalin for attention deficit disorder and l-DOPA for Parkinson's disease) may differ between men and women, and within women in different endocrine states (Lukas et al., 1996; Justice and de Wit, 1999, 2000; Evans et al., 2002). A substantial body of evidence from the animal and human literature documents sex differences in neural and behavioral responses to dopaminergic drugs, including pharmacological treatments and drugs of abuse (Goldstein et al., 2002; Lynch et al., 2002; Carroll et al., 2004; Becker and Hu, 2008). The present findings suggest that although estrogen, considered in isolation, may have unpredictable effects on cognition, its influence is clarified when considered within a larger neuromodulatory framework. A man and woman's milieu differ; until we understand how, we cannot fully understand neural processes as they unfold in the healthy state, much less in the diseased state.
Footnotes
-
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (M.D.), a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (E.J.), and the Berkeley Consortium on Population Health and Human Development– Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health and Society Scholars Program (E.J.). We thank Daniel Weinberger, Bhaskar Kolachana, and Jingshan Chen for advice on and analysis of COMT enzyme data and Lance Kriegsfeld for help with the estradiol ELISA.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Emily Jacobs, Center for Brain Science, Harvard University, Northwest Science East Wing, Room 210-05, 52 Oxford Street, Cambridge, MA 02138. emily.jacobs{at}gmail.com