Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Brief Communications

Medial Temporal Theta/Alpha Power Enhancement Precedes Successful Memory Encoding: Evidence Based on Intracranial EEG

Juergen Fell, Eva Ludowig, Bernhard P. Staresina, Tobias Wagner, Thorsten Kranz, Christian E. Elger and Nikolai Axmacher
Journal of Neuroscience 6 April 2011, 31 (14) 5392-5397; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3668-10.2011
Juergen Fell
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Eva Ludowig
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Bernhard P. Staresina
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Tobias Wagner
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Thorsten Kranz
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christian E. Elger
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nikolai Axmacher
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Not only poststimulus, but also prestimulus neural activity has been shown to be predictive for later successful memory encoding. However, it is still not clear which medial temporal lobe processes precede effective memory formation. Here, our aim was to investigate whether such prestimulus markers for successful memory encoding can be specified based on intracranial recordings directly from the hippocampus and rhinal cortex. For this purpose, we analyzed subsequent memory effects during a continuous word recognition paradigm in 31 presurgical epilepsy patients. We found that rhinal and hippocampal theta and successive alpha power enhancement before word presentation predicted successful memory encoding. Previous studies suggest that stimulus-triggered hippocampal theta activity is particularly related to memory retrieval and activation of a mnemonic context, whereas the alpha rhythm reflects inhibitory top-down control of task processing and executive functioning. In line with these suggestions, we propose that the observed medial temporal theta and alpha power increases before stimulus presentation reflect activation of contextual information and inhibitory top-down control processes preparing for stimulus-triggered memory processing.

Introduction

It is well established that the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and, in particular, the hippocampus play a crucial role for declarative long-term memory (Eichenbaum, 2000; Squire et al., 2004). Accordingly, studies using various complementary neuroimaging techniques have revealed evidence for differential activations following subsequently remembered versus forgotten items (for review, see Wagner et al., 1999; Otten and Rugg, 2002; Axmacher et al., 2006; Eichenbaum et al., 2007). In addition to poststimulus effects, it has recently been demonstrated that neural prestimulus activity can also be predictive of successful memory formation. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, Park and Rugg (2010) found increased hippocampal and parahippocampal activations associated with successful word encoding within the cue-item interval. Similar hippocampal prestimulus effects were described for the learning of reward-predicting images (Adcock et al., 2006) and for the memorization of aversive pictures (Mackiewicz et al., 2006). In two EEG studies, Otten et al. (2006, 2010) demonstrated that a frontal negative modulation occurring up to 1 s before cued words predicts later recollection.

Recently, medial temporal theta (4–8 Hz) activity shortly before word presentation (starting −200 ms) has been shown to precede successful encoding as inferred from source-reconstructed magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings (Guderian et al., 2009). Theta oscillations represent a putative basis for hippocampus-dependent memory processing (Kahana et al., 2001; Buzsáki, 2005), and stimulus-triggered theta activity seems to be particularly related to memory retrieval (Klimesch et al., 2001; Guderian and Düzel, 2005; Osipova et al., 2006). Therefore, Guderian et al. (2009) interpreted the prestimulus enhancement of theta-band activity associated with successful encoding as indicating the activation of a mnemonic context, in which the subsequently presented item can be embedded. However, MEG-based localization of activity from deep brain structures is prone to considerably higher uncertainty compared with neocortical sources (Tarkiainen et al., 2003; Santiuste et al., 2008).

Therefore, our aim was to investigate whether such prestimulus markers for successful memory encoding can be specified based on intracranial recordings directly from the hippocampus and rhinal cortex, which provide both the temporal and spatial resolution needed to unambiguously resolve this question. We used a continuous recognition paradigm in which words are presented sequentially on a computer screen and patients have to specify whether the word is presented for the first or second time. In a previous analysis of poststimulus effects, we found that successful memory formation was associated with increased hippocampal gamma power, but decreased rhinal and hippocampal power in the beta and alpha range (Fell et al., 2008). In the present study, the following hypotheses were targeted. First, we wanted to clarify by direct EEG recordings from within the human MTL whether prestimulus theta band activity indeed predicts subsequent memory, as reported based on source reconstruction of MEG data (Guderian et al., 2009). Second, we asked whether prestimulus power changes in other frequency bands precede successful memory formation. Third, we wanted to assess whether prestimulus effects related to successful memory formation suggest an earlier initiation of memory-related activations similar to those occurring in the poststimulus interval, or whether they indicate specific preparatory processes.

Materials and Methods

Intracranial EEG was recorded from 31 patients (14 female) suffering from pharmacoresistant temporal lobe epilepsies during a continuous word recognition experiment. Depth electrodes comprising 10 platinum contacts were implanted stereotactically along the longitudinal axis of each MTL (Van Roost et al., 1998) during presurgical evaluation because the seizure onset zone could not be precisely determined with noninvasive investigations. Presurgical evaluation revealed unilateral pathologies for all patients included in the present study. To reduce potential contamination by epileptogenic processes, only EEG recordings from the MTL contralateral to the zone of seizure origin were analyzed (Puce et al., 1989; Grunwald et al., 1995).

Patients ranged in age from 16 to 61 years (mean 40 years) and in duration of their epilepsy from 4 to 57 years (mean 23 years). At the time of the recordings, all patients received anticonvulsive medication (plasma levels within the therapeutic range). Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans or postsurgical histological examinations demonstrated unilateral hippocampal sclerosis in 16 patients (left: 5; right: 11), unilateral extrahippocampal lesions without signs of hippocampal sclerosis in 9 patients (left: 3; right: 6), unilateral hippocampal sclerosis with additional extrahippocampal lesions on the same side in 3 patients (left: 2; right: 1), and no clear lesion in 3 patients. All but two patients underwent subsequent epilepsy surgery after implantation (17 selective amygdalo-hippocampectomies, 7 temporal two-thirds resections, 5 lesionectomies). Informed consent for the intracranial EEG recordings and the use of the data for research purposes was obtained by all patients. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Bonn.

For the continuous word-recognition paradigm, 300 frequent German nouns were selected (mean word frequency was 50 per 1 million words according to the CELEX lexical database, version 2.5); 150 stimuli were only presented once, whereas the other 150 words were shown with one repetition. This repetition occurred in 50% of the trials after a short lag of 3–6 words and in 50% after a long lag of 10–30 words. Thus, 450 words were presented consecutively with a duration of 300 ms per word. In temporal-lobe epilepsy patients memory abilities vary to a much larger degree than in healthy subjects (Helmstaedter, 2002). To ensure that the patients were neither overstrained nor bored by the task, the length of the interstimulus interval was adjusted to the subjects' abilities (assessed from the responses in a few pilot trials) and was either short (1600 ± 200 ms; n = 6), intermediate (2000 ± 200 ms; n = 16), or long (2700 ± 200 ms; n = 9). After each word, subjects had to indicate by pressing one of two buttons whether the word was new (left button) or whether it had already been presented before (right button).

Depth electroencephalograms were referenced to linked mastoids, bandpass-filtered [0.01 Hz (6 dB/octave) to 70 Hz (12 dB/octave)], and recorded with a sampling rate of 200 Hz. Electrode contact placement was ascertained by examining magnetic resonance images acquired in the sagittal, axial, and coronal planes and adjusted to the longitudinal axis of the hippocampus. Electrode contacts were localized based on the individual magnetic resonance images and comparison with standardized anatomical atlases (Duvernoy, 1988). In most cases, 2–3 electrodes were localized within the rhinal cortex and 4–6 electrodes within the hippocampus. The rhinal electrode was defined as the electrode located within the anterior parahippocampal gyrus (based on the MRI data) with the largest mean N400 amplitude (new words) between 200 and 600 ms (Grunwald et al., 1999). Because our methods cannot clearly separate perirhinal and entorhinal generators, we use the term rhinal cortex without intending to indicate an integrated rhinal processing stage. The hippocampal electrode was defined as the electrode located within the hippocampus (based on the MRI data) with the largest mean amplitude (old words) of a later positive component between 300 and 1500 ms (Fernández et al., 1999). EEG measures from right and left hemisphere were combined for statistical analyses, because lateralization of verbal memory in MTL epilepsy patients is variable due to functional shifts (Helmstaedter et al., 2006).

An automated artifact rejection was implemented using MATLAB 7.1 (MathWorks). For each segment, the SD of the data points as well as the SD of the gradients (the increase or decrease between two successive data points) were determined. A segment was rejected if any data point or gradient deviated >5 SDs from the mean. Thus, segments with abnormally high amplitudes as well as abrupt rises or falls were eliminated. On average, 17% of trials were removed based on these criteria. The data from four patients, which still exhibited artifacts (observed by visual inspection) after applying the automated rejection procedure, were in addition visually scanned for artifacts, and in total, 22 more trials were discarded. The average number of remembered trials was 79 (minimum: 26, maximum: 131), the average number of forgotten trials, 41 (minimum: 11, maximum: 109).

We analyzed the EEG responses to the first presentation of words shown with one repetition. Responses were classified as remembered or forgotten depending on whether the word was subsequently (i.e., at the second presentation) correctly identified or not. EEG responses were segmented from −1000 to 1400 ms with respect to stimulus onset and were filtered in the frequency range from 1 Hz to 49 Hz (1 Hz steps) by continuous wavelet transforms implementing Morlet wavelets with a bandwidth parameter f0/σf = 5, i.e., roughly speaking, wavelets of five cycles length (Lachaux et al., 1999). Edge effects were minimized by concatenating mirrored (i.e., time inverted) segments at the left and right edge of the original segments, by performing the wavelet transforms on the extended segments, and by afterward discarding the concatenated parts. The concatenation of mirrored segments of the trial to the period preceding the trial may have two effects. (1) It reduces the temporal catchment area of the wavelet-transform (left wing), because no information from the time period before the selected window enters the wavelet-transform. This is a deliberate effect, because it reduces the influence of early poststimulus activity from the preceding trial. (2) It may occasionally induce the impression of broad-band high-frequency activity at the beginning of the trial due to a noncontinuous signal gradient. However, such a phenomenon was not observed in our data. To check whether the observed memory-related effects are affected by the mirroring procedure, we corroborated our analyses by cosine-tapered fast Fourier transforms requiring no mirroring (see supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org). Power values were averaged for nonoverlapping successive time windows of 100 ms duration from −1000 to 1400 ms (24 windows in total). Power values were normalized by dividing them by the average power across the complete 2.4 s interval separately for each subject and each filter frequency.

Results

First, we investigated accuracy and reaction times as a function of subsequent memory for all new items. We found that 66.7 ± 21.3% of these items were remembered afterward. Performance did not differ between patients with left and right focal hemisphere (t30 = 0.518, p = 0.61) or between male and female patients (t30 = 0.875, p = 0.39). Classification of later-remembered items was slower than classification of later-forgotten items (886 ± 39 ms vs 834 ± 34 ms; t31 = 4.08; p = 0.0003). Next, we analyzed behavioral performance depending on whether items were correctly recognized as old (hits) or new (correct rejections), or whether they were incorrectly classified (misses and false alarms). We found that 76.2 ± 30.7% (mean ± SD) of old and 66.6 ± 21.3% of new items, respectively, were classified correctly. Reaction times did not differ between hits and misses (878 ± 29 ms vs 880 ± 39 ms; n.s.), while they were faster for correct rejections than for false alarms (857 ± 36 ms vs 972 ± 42 ms; p < 0.0001; t30 = 5.23).

Figure 1A shows the differences due to memory (later remembered minus forgotten) of the normalized rhinal and hippocampal power changes. In the prestimulus domain, memory-related increases in the theta, alpha, and beta range between 1000 and 200 ms before stimulus onset can be observed. Specifically, the prestimulus effects seem to start in the theta range and then evolve across the alpha and beta range. For a first exploratory evaluation of prestimulus subsequent memory effects, we performed paired t tests (remembered vs forgotten) for each time/frequency element in the prestimulus domain (Fig. 1B). This analysis shows rhinal effects in the theta, alpha, lower beta, and upper beta range, as well as hippocampal effects in the theta and alpha range.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Difference plots for subsequently remembered versus forgotten words. A, Normalized power. B, p values resulting from paired t tests. C, p values resulting from a cluster-based analysis.

This evaluation was corroborated by a stringent nonparametric randomization test, implementing a cluster-based correction for multiple comparisons (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007) [alpha = 0.005, 10,000 randomizations] (Fig. 1C). Basically, this test compares cluster-based effects between two conditions (remembered, forgotten) with effects arising from data clusters, when trials have been randomly shuffled (trial-shuffled surrogates). Note that this procedure does not require multiple electrodes, but can be performed for single electrodes, deriving the clusters across the time and frequency dimensions. Next, six power measures were extracted based on these analyses (Table 1). All six measures yielded statistically significant differences due to memory on at least a 1% level. Rhinal theta and alpha power enhancement even survived rigorous Bonferroni correction (i.e., dividing the p value by the individual number of time/frequency elements and multiplying with 500, the total number of comparisons).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Subsequent memory effects for six prestimulus EEG measures as revealed by paired t test and linear discriminant analyses

We furthermore performed parametric discriminant analyses using pooled covariance matrices (SAS procedure DISCRIM; see also Table 1). The lowest error rate (32.26%) for predicting subsequent memory was reached for rhinal alpha power. A stepwise discriminant analysis with a significance level of p = 0.1 for entering and staying in the model revealed rhinal alpha power and hippocampal theta power as best predictors for later subsequent memory. Prediction based on both measures yielded an error rate of 27.42%. Finally, the time courses of rhinal alpha and hippocampal theta power show enhancements for subsequently remembered versus forgotten words during the prestimulus interval, followed by the inverse pattern in the early poststimulus interval (Fig. 2). The memory-related poststimulus differences for rhinal theta activity (200–400 ms; remembered vs forgotten: 0.97 ± 0.03 vs 0.97 ± 0.04, p = 0.98), rhinal alpha activity (100–200 ms; 1.07 ± 0.04 vs 1.21 ± 0.11, p = 0.24), and hippocampal alpha activity (100–200 ms; 1.05 ± 0.04 vs 1.10 ± 0.11, p = 0.61) are statistically not significant (paired t tests). There is a trend for a reduction of poststimulus hippocampal theta activity (200–400 ms; 0.96 ± 0.03 vs 1.04 ± 0.05, p = 0.107). However, two-way ANOVAs with prepost [prestimulus intervals: rhinal cortex (RH) −800, −400 ms, hippocampus (HI) −1000, −600 ms; poststimulus intervals: RH 100, 200 ms, HI 200, 400 ms] and subsequent memory as repeated measures revealed a trend for a rhinal alpha prepost* memory interaction (F(1,30) = 3.559, p = 0.069), as well as a significant hippocampal theta prepost*memory interaction (F(1,30) = 6.437, p = 0.017). Moreover, prestimulus and poststimulus differences due to memory were interindividually negatively correlated (rhinal alpha: r = −0.436, p < 0.01; hippocampal theta: r = −0.452, p < 0.01; supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material, shows the corresponding scatterplots).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Time courses for normalized rhinal alpha power and hippocampal theta power.

Finally, we investigated whether the memory-related differences for the rhinal and hippocampal power measures were interindividually correlated within the prestimulus and poststimulus time range. For the prestimulus range, we found statistically significant correlations between rhinal theta and alpha power (r = 0.418; p < 0.01), rhinal alpha and beta power (r = 0.725; p < 10−5), and hippocampal theta and alpha power (r = 0.574; p < 0.001). For the poststimulus time range, we again detected statistically significant correlations between rhinal theta and alpha power (r = 0.638, p < 0.0001) and hippocampal theta and alpha power (r = 0.662, p < 0.0001).

Discussion

Based on intracranial EEG recordings we detected an enhancement of prestimulus theta- and alpha-band activity in the rhinal cortex and hippocampus, which predicted successful memory formation. Within the rhinal cortex, but not within hippocampus, the prestimulus effects moreover extended into the beta range up to 34 Hz. Through additional statistical analyses (see supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org), we validated that these findings do not depend on the different interstimulus intervals and are not caused by serial behavioral effects. We furthermore evaluated whether prestimulus theta and alpha power effects are correlated with poststimulus phase-locking effects (Fell et al., 2008), as well as whether prestimulus slow shifts predicted subsequent memory (Otten et al., 2006). These analyses revealed no statistically significant results (see supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org).

Recent microelectrode recordings in epilepsy patients—which allow the simultaneous investigation of oscillations in local field potentials and action potentials—explored the functional relevance of neural oscillations for memory processes. Jacobs et al. (2007) were the first to report that action potentials within the human medial temporal lobe are locked to specific phases of oscillations in different frequency bands, suggesting that these oscillations serve as an internal clock for the timing of action potentials. Mechanistically, this coupling may be related to the fact that, in particular, high-frequency oscillations are controlled by inhibitory interneurons which leave only brief windows for excitation of pyramidal cells (Cardin et al., 2009). Furthermore, Rutishauser et al. (2010) recently found that the magnitude of hippocampal spike-field coherence predicted memory performance in a recognition memory task. Together, these findings indicate that oscillations within the medial temporal lobe are critical for successful information encoding.

In our data, the memory-related increase of prestimulus theta power is in accordance to the well known relevance of hippocampal theta activity for memory processes (Kahana et al., 2001) and is consistent with the findings of a recent MEG study (Guderian et al., 2009). In that study, memory-related theta differences became significant very shortly before stimulus presentation (starting at ∼−200 ms). In our experiment, we observed a memory-related theta increase starting as early as 1000 ms before word presentation, accompanied by a successive alpha power enhancement. Importantly, the precise time point of word presentation was unknown to participants in our study (jittered intertrial interval), but was precisely defined (500 ms after presentation of a fixation cross) in the study by Guderian et al. (2009), which may contribute to the different time courses of theta power. Moreover, peristimulus theta time courses reported by Guderian et al. (2009) were calculated based on a wavelet with a center frequency of 7 Hz, which may have included some of the activity we define as alpha.

Similar to the MEG results by Guderian et al. (2009), we observed an inverse pattern of memory-related theta activity shortly after stimulus presentation compared with activity before the stimulus and a negative correlation of prestimulus and poststimulus memory-related effects (see also supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). There is a trend for a memory-related reduction of poststimulus hippocampal theta activity in the time range between 200 ms and 400 ms after stimulus presentation. In a previous study (Fell et al., 2008), we interpreted the tendency (although statistically not significant) of a memory-related broad-band power decrease in the early poststimulus time range as reflecting the shutdown of ongoing neural activity to prepare for incoming stimulus information. A memory-related poststimulus reduction of hippocampal theta activity has also been reported by Sederberg et al. (2007). Our data thus indicate that the prestimulus effects are not related to an earlier initiation of memory-related activations, but that they reflect specific preparatory processes. Because stimulus-triggered hippocampal theta activity has been found to be particularly related to memory retrieval (Klimesch et al., 2001; Guderian and Düzel, 2005; Osipova et al., 2006), Guderian et al. (2009) speculated that the prestimulus theta increase my reflect the activation of a mnemonic context, in which the subsequently presented item can be embedded. This contextual information, for instance, could consist of the word category, the expected position of the word on the computer screen, its luminance, or its timing. Applying a method suitable to detect directional coupling (Rosenblum and Pikovsky, 2001; Axmacher et al., 2008; Wagner et al., 2010), we indeed found evidence for an increased hippocampal*rhinal interaction for subsequently remembered compared with forgotten items in the prestimulus time range, which may be interpreted as a sign of memory retrieval (see supplemental material, available at www.jneurosci.org). Our data furthermore indicate that the prestimulus theta increase may initiate the subsequent alpha enhancement, because both effects are interindividually correlated.

But how can the memory-related prestimulus alpha increase, itself, be interpreted? Several recent studies indicate that the alpha rhythm is not just related to cortical idling or to inwardly versus outwardly directed attention (Ray and Cole, 1985). Instead, alpha activity seems to reflect inhibitory top-down control, which may prepare for optimal subsequent task processing and executive functioning (Cooper et al., 2003; Klimesch et al., 2007; Min and Herrmann, 2007). For instance, alpha power was found to be increased in “dorsal-stream” areas devoted to processing of spatial relations during working memory maintenance of a face identity but not during maintenance of a face direction, suggesting an active role of alpha in the inhibition of task-irrelevant areas (Jokisch and Jensen, 2007). Furthermore, a rhinal and hippocampal alpha rhythm (between 10 and 12 Hz) has been detected in rats, which was prominent in a familiar environment, but disappeared in a novel environment (Nerad and Bilkey, 2005). This result is consistent with the idea of an alpha-related inhibitory control of memory formation, because there is no need for fresh encoding of the familiar environment. Based on these findings, we suggest that the observed memory-related prestimulus rhinal and hippocampal alpha increase may reflect an inhibitory top-down mechanism, which prepares for stimulus-triggered memory processing.

Footnotes

  • This research was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Transregional Collaborative Research Centre SFB/TR 3, project A9), as well as by the Volkswagen Foundation (Grant I/79878). We thank Christian Bien for support with the clinical management of the patients, Horst Urbach for providing the magnetic resonance images, and Timm Rosburg for data acquisition.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Juergen Fell, Department of Epileptology, University of Bonn, Sigmund-Freud-Strasse 25, D-53105 Bonn, Germany. juergen.fell{at}ukb.uni-bonn.de

References

  1. ↵
    1. Adcock RA,
    2. Thangavel A,
    3. Whitfield-Gabrieli S,
    4. Knutson B,
    5. Gabrieli JD
    (2006) Reward-motivated learning: mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron 50:507–517.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Axmacher N,
    2. Mormann F,
    3. Fernández G,
    4. Elger CE,
    5. Fell J
    (2006) Memory formation by neuronal synchronization. Brain Res Rev 52:170–182.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Axmacher N,
    2. Schmitz DP,
    3. Wagner T,
    4. Elger CE,
    5. Fell J
    (2008) Interactions between medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and inferior temporal regions during visual working memory: a combined intracranial EEG and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 28:7304–7312.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Buzsáki G
    (2005) Theta rhythm of navigation: link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus 15:827–840.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Cardin JA,
    2. Carlén M,
    3. Meletis K,
    4. Knoblich U,
    5. Zhang F,
    6. Deisseroth K,
    7. Tsai LH,
    8. Moore CI
    (2009) Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459:663–667.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Cooper NR,
    2. Croft RJ,
    3. Dominey SJ,
    4. Burgess AP,
    5. Gruzelier JH
    (2003) Paradox lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations during externally vs internally directed attention and the implications for idling and inhibition hypotheses. Int J Psychophysiol 47:65–74.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Duvernoy HM
    (1988) An atlas of applied anatomy, The human hippocampus (JF Bergmann Verlag, München), pp 25–43.
  8. ↵
    1. Eichenbaum H
    (2000) A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:41–50.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Eichenbaum H,
    2. Yonelinas AP,
    3. Ranganath C
    (2007) The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:123–152.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Fell J,
    2. Ludowig E,
    3. Rosburg T,
    4. Axmacher N,
    5. Elger CE
    (2008) Phase-locking within human mediotemporal lobe predicts memory formation. Neuroimage 43:410–419.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Fernández G,
    2. Effern A,
    3. Grunwald T,
    4. Pezer N,
    5. Lehnertz K,
    6. Dümpelmann M,
    7. Van Roost D,
    8. Elger CE
    (1999) Real-time tracking of memory formation in the human rhinal cortex and hippocampus. Science 285:1582–1585.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Grunwald T,
    2. Elger CE,
    3. Lehnertz K,
    4. Van Roost D,
    5. Heinze HJ
    (1995) Alterations of intrahippocampal cognitive potentials in temporal lobe epilepsy. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 95:53–62.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Grunwald T,
    2. Beck H,
    3. Lehnertz K,
    4. Blümcke I,
    5. Pezer N,
    6. Kurthen M,
    7. Fernández G,
    8. Van Roost D,
    9. Heinze HJ,
    10. Kutas M,
    11. Elger CE
    (1999) Evidence relating human verbal memory to hippocampal N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:12085–12089.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Guderian S,
    2. Düzel E
    (2005) Induced theta oscillations mediate large-scale synchrony with mediotemporal areas during recollection in humans. Hippocampus 15:901–912.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Guderian S,
    2. Schott BH,
    3. Richardson-Klavehn A,
    4. Düzel E
    (2009) Medial temporal theta state before an event predicts episodic encoding success in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:5365–5370.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Helmstaedter C
    (2002) Effects of chronic epilepsy on declarative memory systems. Prog Brain Res 135:439–453.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Helmstaedter C,
    2. Fritz NE,
    3. González Pérez PA,
    4. Elger CE,
    5. Weber B
    (2006) Shift-back of right to left hemisphere language dominance after control of epileptic seizures: evidence for epilepsy driven functional cerebral organization. Epilepsy Res 70:257–262.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Jacobs J,
    2. Kahana MJ,
    3. Ekstrom AD,
    4. Fried I
    (2007) Brain oscillations control timing of single-neuron activity in humans. J Neurosci 27:3839–3844.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Jokisch D,
    2. Jensen O
    (2007) Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal and ventral stream. J Neurosci 27:3244–3251.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Kahana MJ,
    2. Seelig D,
    3. Madsen JR
    (2001) Theta returns. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:739–744.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Klimesch W,
    2. Doppelmayr M,
    3. Yonelinas A,
    4. Kroll NE,
    5. Lazzara M,
    6. Röhm D,
    7. Gruber W
    (2001) Theta synchronization during episodic retrieval: neural correlates of conscious awareness. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 12:33–38.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Klimesch W,
    2. Sauseng P,
    3. Hanslmayr S
    (2007) EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 53:63–88.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Lachaux JP,
    2. Rodriguez E,
    3. Martinerie J,
    4. Varela FJ
    (1999) Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp 8:194–208.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Mackiewicz KL,
    2. Sarinopoulos I,
    3. Cleven KL,
    4. Nitschke JB
    (2006) The effect of anticipation and the specificity of sex differences for amygdala and hippocampus function in emotional memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:14200–14205.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Maris E,
    2. Oostenveld R
    (2007) Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J Neurosci Methods 164:177–190.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Min BK,
    2. Herrmann CS
    (2007) Prestimulus EEG alpha activity reflects prestimulus top-down processing. Neurosci Lett 422:131–135.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Nerad L,
    2. Bilkey DK
    (2005) Ten- to 12-Hz EEG oscillations in the rat hippocampus and rhinal cortex that is modulated by environmental familiarity. J Neurophysiol 93:1246–1254.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Osipova D,
    2. Takashima A,
    3. Oostenveld R,
    4. Fernández G,
    5. Maris E,
    6. Jensen O
    (2006) Theta and gamma oscillations predict encoding and retrieval of declarative memory. J Neurosci 26:7523–7531.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Otten LJ,
    2. Rugg MD
    (2002) The birth of a memory. Trends Neurosci 25:279–281.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Otten LJ,
    2. Quayle AH,
    3. Akram S,
    4. Ditewig TA,
    5. Rugg MD
    (2006) Brain activity before an event predicts later recollection. Nat Neurosci 9:489–491.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Otten LJ,
    2. Quayle AH,
    3. Puvaneswaran B
    (2010) Prestimulus subsequent memory effects for auditory and visual events. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1212–1223.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Park H,
    2. Rugg MD
    (2010) Prestimulus hippocampal activity predicts later recollection. Hippocampus 20:24–28.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Puce A,
    2. Kalnins RM,
    3. Berkovic SF,
    4. Donnan GA,
    5. Bladin PF
    (1989) Limbic P3 potentials, seizure localization, and surgical pathology in temporal lobe epilepsy. Ann Neurol 26:377–385.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Ray WJ,
    2. Cole HW
    (1985) EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processing. Science 228:750–752.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Rosenblum MG,
    2. Pikovsky AS
    (2001) Detecting direction of coupling in interacting oscillators. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 64:045202.
  36. ↵
    1. Rutishauser U,
    2. Ross IB,
    3. Mamelak AN,
    4. Schuman EM
    (2010) Human memory strength is predicted by theta-frequency phase-locking of single neurons. Nature 464:903–907.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Santiuste M,
    2. Nowak R,
    3. Russi A,
    4. Tarancon T,
    5. Oliver B,
    6. Ayats E,
    7. Scheler G,
    8. Graetz G
    (2008) Simultaneous magnetoencephalography and intracranial EEG registration: technical and clinical aspects. J Clin Neurophysiol 25:331–339.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Sederberg PB,
    2. Schulze-Bonhage A,
    3. Madsen JR,
    4. Bromfield EB,
    5. McCarthy DC,
    6. Brandt A,
    7. Tully MS,
    8. Kahana MJ
    (2007) Hippocampal and neocortical gamma oscillations predict memory formation in humans. Cereb Cortex 17:1190–1196.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    1. Squire LR,
    2. Stark CE,
    3. Clark RE
    (2004) The mediotemporal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:279–306.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Tarkiainen A,
    2. Liljeström M,
    3. Seppä M,
    4. Salmelin R
    (2003) The 3D topography of MEG source localization accuracy: effects of conductor model and noise. Clin Neurophysiol 114:1977–1992.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Van Roost D,
    2. Solymosi L,
    3. Schramm J,
    4. van Oosterwyck B,
    5. Elger CE
    (1998) Depth electrode implantation in the length axis of the hippocampus for the presurgical evaluation of mediotemporal lobe epilepsy: A computed tomography-based stereotactic insertion technique and its accuracy. Neurosurgery 43:819–826.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Wagner AD,
    2. Koutstaal W,
    3. Schacter DL
    (1999) When encoding yields remembering: insights from event-related neuroimaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 354:1307–1324.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Wagner T,
    2. Fell J,
    3. Lehnertz K
    (2010) The detection of transient directional couplings based on phase synchronization. New J Phys 12:053031.
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 31 (14)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 31, Issue 14
6 Apr 2011
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Medial Temporal Theta/Alpha Power Enhancement Precedes Successful Memory Encoding: Evidence Based on Intracranial EEG
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Medial Temporal Theta/Alpha Power Enhancement Precedes Successful Memory Encoding: Evidence Based on Intracranial EEG
Juergen Fell, Eva Ludowig, Bernhard P. Staresina, Tobias Wagner, Thorsten Kranz, Christian E. Elger, Nikolai Axmacher
Journal of Neuroscience 6 April 2011, 31 (14) 5392-5397; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3668-10.2011

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Medial Temporal Theta/Alpha Power Enhancement Precedes Successful Memory Encoding: Evidence Based on Intracranial EEG
Juergen Fell, Eva Ludowig, Bernhard P. Staresina, Tobias Wagner, Thorsten Kranz, Christian E. Elger, Nikolai Axmacher
Journal of Neuroscience 6 April 2011, 31 (14) 5392-5397; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3668-10.2011
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Heteromodal Cortical Areas Encode Sensory-Motor Features of Word Meaning
  • Pharmacologically Counteracting a Phenotypic Difference in Cerebellar GABAA Receptor Response to Alcohol Prevents Excessive Alcohol Consumption in a High Alcohol-Consuming Rodent Genotype
  • Neuromuscular NMDA Receptors Modulate Developmental Synapse Elimination
Show more Brief Communications
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.