Abstract
Visual organs perceive environmental stimuli required for rapid initiation of behaviors and can also entrain the circadian clock. The larval eye of Drosophila is capable of both functions. Each eye contains only 12 photoreceptors (PRs), which can be subdivided into two subtypes. Four PRs express blue-sensitive rhodopsin5 (rh5) and eight express green-sensitive rhodopsin6 (rh6). We found that either PR-subtype is sufficient to entrain the molecular clock by light, while only the Rh5-PR subtype is essential for light avoidance. Acetylcholine released from PRs confers both functions. Both subtypes of larval PRs innervate the main circadian pacemaker neurons of the larva, the neuropeptide PDF (pigment-dispersing factor)-expressing lateral neurons (LNs), providing sensory input to control circadian rhythms. However, we show that PDF-expressing LNs are dispensable for light avoidance, and a distinct set of three clock neurons is required. Thus we have identified distinct sensory and central circuitry regulating light avoidance behavior and clock entrainment. Our findings provide insights into the coding of sensory information for distinct behavioral functions and the underlying molecular and neuronal circuitry.
Introduction
Animals detect and process a complex array of visual information from the environment. In both insects and vertebrates, visual information is perceived by photoreceptor (PR) neurons in the eye that express G-protein-coupled receptors, Rhodopsins, which transform light with specific wavelengths into neuronal information. This information is transmitted to second-order neurons for visual information processing before being transferred to higher brain centers. In both insects and vertebrates, the visual system processes information for rapid behaviors and entrains circadian pacemaker neurons.
The simple visual system of the Drosophila larva provides an excellent model to investigate these distinct roles. The larval eye (Bolwig organ) consists of four PRs expressing blue-sensitive rhodopsin5 (rh5) and eight PRs expressing green-sensitive rhodopsin6 (rh6) (Sprecher et al., 2007; Sprecher and Desplan, 2008). Thus, compared with the relative complexity of the visual system in the adult fly, only 12 neurons composed of two distinct cell types contribute to the perception of light. Axons from these 12 PRs innervate the larval optic neuropil (LON). The larval eye, along with the blue-sensitive photoreceptor cryptochrome (cry), regulates both entrainment of circadian rhythms and light avoidance (Mazzoni et al., 2005). Here we identify the neurotransmitter and PR-subtypes that mediate circadian entrainment and light avoidance. We further identify central brain neurons mediating rapid light avoidance.
Materials and Methods
Drosophila melanogaster strains and genetics.
Flies and larvae were kept in 12 h light-dark (LD) cycles at 25°C. For wild-type immunostainings we used yw122 or yw122;Sp/CyO;TM2/TM6b. In behavioral control experiments we used the genotype depicted in the corresponding figure and figure legend. We used the following mutants, Gal4 drivers, upstream activator sequence (UAS)-responder, and lacZ lines: rh52, rh61 (Yamaguchi et al., 2008); cryb (Mazzoni et al., 2005); rh5-Gal4, rh6-Gal4, GMR-Gal4, tim-Gal4, pdf-Gal4, Cha-Gal4, cry-Gal4, cry-Gal80, pdf-Gal80, UAS-hid, UAS-rpr, UAS-CD8::GFP (Bloomington Stock Center); UAS-ShiTs1, UAS-H2B::YFP [anti-GFP (green fluorescent protein) antibody/Biogenesis recognizes the yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) antigen] (Bellaïche et al., 2001); Cha-Gal80, rh5-lacZ, rh6-lacZ (Cook et al., 2003); hs-flp, UAS-FRT-CD2-STOP-FRT-CD8::GFP. UAS-Kir2.1 is UAS-mKir2.1(III), which has previously been described (Baines et al., 2001). For all experiments male and female larvae were used.
The UAS-ChaRNAi construct consists of a P-element backbone harboring an inverted repeat of the Cha/VAChT locus cloned downstream of the Gal4 UAS-promoter. The inverted repeat is a 568 bp fragment targeting the first exon that is common to the Cha (choline acetyltransferase) and VAChT (vesicular ACh transporter) transcripts (Kitamoto et al., 1998). Cha is necessary for acetylcholine (ACh) synthesis, while VAChT is essential for transporting ACh into synaptic vesicles. Use of this ACh-RNAi (RNA interference) construct should block production of both of these proteins in specific cholinergic neurons and thereby impair ACh function.
The tethered UAS-α-bungarotoxin (UAS-αBtx) 499 bp construct, was synthesized by GenScript with EcoRI and XhoI ends for insertion into pUAST (Brand and Perrimon, 1993). This construct contained the following: (1) Drosophila Kozak consensus sequence (CAAA), (2) secretion signal sequence and α-bungarotoxin coding sequence (Ibañez-Tallon et al., 2004), (3) three repeats of a glycine-asparaginine linker (GN)3, (4) two copies of the myc-epitope tag, (5) (GN)9, and (6) Lynx1 C terminus for addition of glycosylphosphatidylinositol anchor for tethering to the membrane (Ibañez-Tallon et al., 2004). All of the protein-coding regions were codon-optimized for Drosophila (GenScript software).
Analysis of light-dependent clock entrainment.
Light-entrained third instar larvae were exposed to a 2 h light pulse of 750 lux at Zeitgeber time 13 (ZT13), 1 h following the onset of lights-off. Light-entrained larvae without a light pulse were tested at ZT15 as controls. Larval brains were fixed for 15 min with 4% paraformaldehyde/PBS and subsequently stained as previously described (Blau and Young, 1999). TIM (circadian transcription factor Timeless) levels of stained brains were analyzed using confocal microscopy. One section at the largest diameter of 8–12 neurons was scanned for each genotype and condition. All experiments were repeated at least four times in independent experiments. Confocal settings were identical for all scans in each experiment. Staining intensity was defined by the mean pixel intensity within the cytoplasm minus the mean pixel intensity in the surrounding tissue (measured with the Leica confocal analysis software). The average ratio of pulsed/unpulsed was calculated for each genotype using Leica SP2 confocal processing software.
Immunohistochemistry and antibodies.
Dissection and analysis of the brain and larval head skeleton were performed as previously described (Sprecher et al., 2007). Flip-out experiments were performed as described by Wong et al. (2002). Heat-shock was given in L1 larvae and dissections were performed in L3 larvae. Brains were mounted in Vectashield H-1000 (Vector Labs). Primary antibodies used were as follows: rabbit anti-Rh6, 1:10,000 (Tahayato et al., 2003); mouse anti-Rh5, 1:20, anti-Rh3, 1:20, or anti-Rh4 1:20 (Chou et al., 1996); mouse anti-Rh1, 1:20 [Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank (DSHB)], mouse anti-Fasciclin II, 1:10 (Lin and Goodman, 1994); rat anti-Elav, 1:30 (DSHB); sheep anti-GFP, 1:1000 (Biogenesis); rabbit anti-PDF (pigment-dispersing factor), 1:50 (DSHB); mouse anti-Chp, 1:10 (DSHB); mouse anti-βGAL, 1:20 (DSHB); and rat anti-TIM, 1:1000 (kindly provided by M. Rosbash, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA). Secondary goat antibodies were used for confocal microscopy conjugated with Alexa-488, Alexa-555, and Alexa-647 (Invitrogen), all at 1:300–1:500 dilution.
Laser confocal microscopy and image processing.
Leica TCS SP2 and SP5 microscopes were used for all imaging. Spacing between optical sections ranged from 0.2 to 1.5 μm and were recorded in “line average mode” with a picture resolution of 512 × 512 or 1024 × 1024 pixels. Captured images from optical sections were processed using Leica confocal software. Complete series of optical sections were imported and processed using ImageJ as previously described (Sprecher et al., 2006).
Behavioral assay and statistical analysis.
We used the behavioral paradigm previously described by Mazzoni et al. (2005). Briefly, third instar larvae were grown in a 12 h dark-light cycle and tested for light avoidance between ZT2 and ZT4 at 750 lux. GraphPad Prism 5.0 was used for all statistical analysis. Each experimental set of data was statistically analyzed using an ANOVA with a Tukey's multiple-comparison post hoc test. Normal probability plots have been performed on random datasets of independent experiments supporting the normality assumption.
Eclosion experiments.
Larvae grown in LD cycles on standard food-medium were placed in eclosion monitors (Trikinetics) at the late wandering third instar during the light phase. Eclosion monitors were transferred to constant darkness (DD) 12 h later and eclosion was monitored ∼4 d later. Data represent the combined eclosions from 48 h of recordings collected in 4 independent experiments.
Results
Rh5-PRs are required for light avoidance
The larval eye is essential for light avoidance, as larvae with all PRs ablated are blind (Sawin-McCormack et al., 1995; Mazzoni et al., 2005). We used a standardized light avoidance assay in which larvae choose between 750 lux light and darkness. In this assay, >70% of wild-type larvae robustly avoid light (Mazzoni et al., 2005). We assayed larvae carrying single mutations in rh5 or rh6, as well as rh5;rh6 double mutants, to determine which PR subsets are required for light avoidance. PRs maintain their normal projections and innervations of the LON in the absence of functional Rhodopsins (data not shown). rh5;rh6 double mutants were also completely blind (Fig. 1A). Light avoidance was also completely lost in rh5 single mutants (p < 0.001). However, photophobicity was unaffected in rh6 mutant larvae (Fig. 1A). Behavioral defects were observed as soon as 1 min after the start of the experiment and remained throughout the course of the 10 min assay (data not shown). These results indicate that rh5 is required for light avoidance, while rh6 is dispensable.
Function of Rh5-PRs and Rh6-PRs for light avoidance behavior and light-dependent TIM degradation in LNs. A, rh5;rh6 double-mutant, rh6 mutant, and rh5 mutant larvae. yw (70.0%; SEM, 4.7) and rh6 mutant (66.6%; SEM, 3.2) larvae were photophobic and did not differ from each other. rh5;rh6 double mutants (47.9%; SEM, 2.7) and rh5 mutants (50.6%; SEM, 2.1) both strongly reduced light avoidance compared with yw and rh6 mutant larvae (n = 12; ANOVA, p < 0.001). B, Genetic ablation of Rh5- or Rh6-PRs impairs light avoidance. In parental control strains rh5-Gal4 (67.8%; SEM, 4.1), rh6-Gal4 (71.2%; SEM, 4.1), and UAS-hid,rpr (67.8%; SEM, 4.1), larval behavior was not significantly different, nor was the behavior of rh6-Gal4/UAS-hid,rpr (64.2%; SEM, 3.7) larvae. In comparison, rh5-Gal4>UAS-hid,rpr (47.4%; SEM 2.2) larvae were blind and significantly different from all other larvae (n = 10; ANOVA, p < 0.001). C, D, Confocal image of two LNs of a wild-type larva (C) and a rh5;rh6 double mutant (D) after a 2 h light pulse stained with anti-PDF (red) and anti-TIM (green). TIM is degraded in wild-type larvae but remains present in rh5;rh6 double mutant. E, Quantification of TIM levels in control, rh5;rh6 double-mutant, rh5 mutant, rh6 mutant, and cryb mutant larvae; y-axis represents the average ratio of [TIM pulsed]/[TIM nonpulsed]. rh5 mutants (0.28; SEM, 0.04), rh6 mutants (0.31; SEM, 0.10) and cryb mutants (0.29; SEM, 0.03) are not significantly different from control animals, while rh5;rh6 double mutants (0.74; SEM, 0.17) were significantly different from all animals (n = 5; ANOVA, p < 0.01). Error bars represent ±SEM. F, Quantification of TIM protein levels of larvae harboring either GMR-Gal4 or UAS-ChaRNAi transgenes alone, rh5;rh6 double-mutant (control) larvae, and GMR>ChaRNAi larvae using confocal analysis. Parental GMR-Gal4 (0.24; SEM, 0.03) and UAS-ChaRNAi (0.32; SEM, 0.09) lines are not significantly different from each other. TIM is not degraded in the rh5;rh6 double mutant (0.79; SEM, 0.16) and GMR>ChaRNAi (0.87; SEM, 0.06) following light pulse, and the two lines are significantly different from GMR-Gal4 and UAS-ChaRNAi lines (n = 4; ANOVA, p < 0.01). Error bars represent ±SEM.
To verify this finding, we genetically ablated each PR-subtype and assayed larvae for light avoidance. We coexpressed the proapoptotic genes head involution defective (hid) and reaper (rpr) in a subtype-specific manner, using either rh5-Gal4 or rh6-Gal4. Anti-Rh5, anti-Rh6 antibody staining confirmed that this manipulation fully and selectively ablated the targeted PRs (data not shown). Light avoidance was abolished in larvae with rh5 neurons ablated (rh5>hid,rpr) (Fig. 1B) (ANOVA, p < 0.001). However, rh6>hid,rpr larvae robustly avoided light (Fig. 1B). These experiments confirm that only the Rh5-PR subtype is required for light avoidance, while the Rh6-PR subtype is dispensable.
Either PR-subtype is sufficient for entrainment of the circadian clock
In mammals, photoreceptors mediate vision while a distinct population of melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells function to entrain circadian pacemaker cells via the retinohypothalamic tract (Hannibal et al., 2000; Hattar et al., 2002). PRs of the Drosophila larval eye innervate the lateral neurons (LNs) and are required to entrain the molecular clock (Mazzoni et al., 2005). The larval LNs survive metamorphosis and become the master pacemaker neurons in adult flies [small ventral LNs (LNvs)]. Furthermore, larval LNs probably maintain the circadian phase through metamorphosis (Sehgal et al., 1992; Kaneko et al., 1997). We therefore sought to identify the PR-subtype(s) responsible for entraining the central clock located in LNs.
The circadian transcription factor TIM is rapidly degraded upon light exposure, and TIM levels are a measure of the molecular response to light of the circadian clock. To investigate the role of larval PR-subtypes in clock entrainment, we examined TIM degradation after a 2 h light pulse beginning at ZT13, when TIM is normally localized to the cytoplasm. LNs also express cryptochrome (cry), which encodes an intrinsic blue light-sensitive photopigment that induces TIM degradation upon light exposure in the early morning (Stanewsky et al., 1998; Ceriani et al., 1999; Griffin et al., 1999; Klarsfeld et al., 2004). However, degradation of TIM in cryb mutants was not significantly different from that in control animals with our 2 h light pulse protocol at ZT13 (Fig. 1E). This result is consistent with the fact that rhythmic cry expression is at low levels in the early evening (ZT13) (Stanewsky et al., 1998). Furthermore, since Cry protein itself is degraded by light, even low-level Cry protein accumulation would be minimal since larvae were only in darkness from ZT12 to ZT13.
Genetic ablation of all larval PRs blocks entrainment of the clock, presumably by inhibiting light-dependent degradation of TIM in LNs (Mazzoni et al., 2005). However, these findings are difficult to interpret because ablation of PRs also causes developmental abnormities in LNs (Malpel et al., 2002). To investigate the functional role of larval PR-subtypes in LN entrainment, we assayed light-induced TIM degradation in LNs of larvae mutant for rh5, for rh6, and for rh5;rh6 double mutants since LNs appear to develop normally in these mutants. In the absence of a light pulse, both wild-type and rh5;rh6 double-mutant larvae had high cytoplasmic TIM levels in LNs at ZT15. These findings suggest that the molecular clock still oscillates in blind animals, likely because it is entrained by Cry in the early morning. A 2 h light pulse significantly reduced TIM levels in LNs of wild-type larvae. However, TIM levels remained high in rh5;rh6 double mutants, indicating that the mutations render the molecular clock insensitive to light at this time of the day. Thus, the visual system is essential for TIM degradation by light in the early evening (Fig. 1E). We also analyzed TIM levels in rh5 and rh6 single mutants exposed to light. TIM levels responded to light like wild types and were reduced compared with rh5;rh6 double mutants after the light pulse (ANOVA; p < 0.01). Therefore, while larval PRs are essential to confer TIM degradation in the early evening, either PR-subtype by itself is sufficient to entrain the clock.
We monitored eclosion in rh5;rh6 double mutants to determine whether the reduced TIM degradation in these mutants correlates with disrupted circadian entrainment. We placed control and mutant late wandering third instar larvae in constant darkness 2–3 d before eclosion to avoid interference by rhodopsins expressed in the developing adult eye just before eclosion. Interestingly, rh5;rh6 double mutants were similar to wild type, with the majority eclosing in the subjective day (data not shown). This is consistent with previous findings that ablation of the larval eye does not abolish circadian eclosion rhythms (Malpel et al., 2004). These findings suggest that cry alone is sufficient to support proper eclosion rhythms by entraining the clock in the early morning. Even though rh5;rh6 mutant larvae have decreased ability to degrade TIM in response to light, they are still capable of proper circadian behavior.
Larval PR-subtypes project to distinct areas of the larval optic neuropil
PRs of the larval eye extend their axons from the anterior part of the head skeleton into the LON (Fig. 2A,C,D), where Rh5- and Rh6-PRs could achieve their unique function by signaling to different target neurons, and/or by signaling to the same neurons using distinct neurotransmitters. To investigate the axonal projection pattern of Rh5- and Rh6-PRs, we used rh5-lacZ and rh6-GFP reporters (Cook et al., 2003). We found that Rh5- and Rh6-PR projections occupy distinct regions of the LON: Rh5-PRs project medioventrally in the LON where they are surrounded by Rh6-PR termini (Fig. 2B) (data not shown). While the majority of synaptic connections localizes to the LON, we also observed Rh5 neurites bypassing the LON projecting to the adjacent ipsilateral central brain neuropil, indicating heterogeneous function of Rh5-PRs, as previously reported (Mazzoni et al., 2005) (data not shown).
Rh5- and Rh6-PRs project to distinct domains and are cholinergic. A, Larval PR axons terminate in the LON located medial to the developing medulla neuropil. PR axons are labeled with anti-Chp (red), neuronal cell bodies of the brain and optic lobe neuropil are labeled with anti-Elav (green); anti-Fasciclin II (anti-FasII) (blue) labels PR axons and a subset of central brain axon fascicles. B, Termini of PR-subtypes occupy distinct domains in the LON. Rh5 termini are labeled with rh5-LacZ (anti-βGal; red in B, blue in C) and Rh6 termini with rh6-GFP (anti-GFP; green in B, C). Both Rh5 and Rh6 PRs are marked with anti-22C10 in blue (B). C, LN dendrites arborize in the LON and overlap with both PR-subtypes. LNs are marked by anti-PDF (red). D, Representation of the larval PR-subtypes and LNs within the LON. E, Expression of UAS-H2B::YFP under the control of Cha-Gal4 labels the nuclei of Rh5- and Rh6-PRs (green, anti-YFP; red, anti-Rh6; blue, anti-Rh5). F, F′, Double labeling of Rh6-axon termini (arrow) labeled with rh6-GFP, or Rh5-axon termini (arrow) labeled with rh5-GFP with anti-Cha (blue) and anti-GFP (green). Anti-Cha staining is detected in both PR-subtypes.
Both larval PR-subtypes are cholinergic
Although PRs in the adult fly signal through the neurotransmitter histamine, larval PRs have been reported to be cholinergic (Gorczyca and Hall, 1987; Yasuyama et al., 1995). We investigated the neurotransmitter identity of larval PR-subtypes. Using an anti-histamine antibody, we could not detect staining in the projections of larval PRs or any other area of the LON (data not shown). None of the neurotransmitter systems including serotonin, dopamine, octopamine, glutamate, and GABA were detected using neurotransmitter-specific antibodies or Gal4 lines (anti-serotonin, anti-dopamine, ddc-Gal4, Th1-Gal4, Tdc-Gal4, vGlut-Gal4, gad1-Gal4) (data not shown). Instead, both larval PR-subtypes expressed choline acetyltransferase (Cha), the key enzyme in the biosynthesis of ACh, suggesting that ACh is the only classic neurotransmitter released from PRs. We used two independent experiments to assess whether both PR-subtypes were cholinergic. First, we expressed a UAS-Histone2B-YFP reporter under the control of Cha-Gal4 to label the nuclei of cholinergic neurons. We marked PRs with anti-Rh6 and anti-Rh5 antibodies and cholinergic neurons with anti-GFP (YFP is recognized by anti-GFP antibodies). All nuclei of Rh5- and Rh6-PRs are stained with anti-GFP, indicating that both PR-subtypes are cholinergic (Fig. 2E). Second, using rh5-lacZ or rh6-lacZ in combination with anti-Cha antibodies confirmed that Cha localizes to the axons of projections to the LON in both PR-subtypes (Fig. 2F,F′). Thus, both larval PR-subtypes are cholinergic.
Acetylcholine is required in larval PRs for circadian clock resetting
Larval LNs express a functional nicotinic ACh receptor, suggesting that they receive ACh input (Wegener et al., 2004). Therefore, ACh released from PRs may act to initiate TIM degradation and entrain the larval pacemaker neurons. We selectively disrupted ACh synthesis in PRs with Cha-RNAi to address whether ACh is required for light-dependent TIM degradation in LNs. TIM levels were measured after a light pulse as described above. Disrupting ACh synthesis in all PRs (GMR>ChaRNAi) blocked light-induced TIM degradation at ZT15. Although TIM levels in control larvae harboring the GMR-Gal4 or UAS-ChaRNAi transgenes alone were not different from wild type, GMR>ChaRNAi larvae displayed high TIM protein levels (ANOVA, p < 0.01) similar to rh5;rh6 double mutants (Fig. 1F). Therefore ACh functions in larval PRs for light-induced TIM degradation in pacemaker neurons.
ACh release from Rh5-PRs drives light avoidance
We sought to determine the role of ACh release from PRs in light avoidance. Inhibition of ACh synthesis in all PRs (GMR>ChaRNAi) abolished light avoidance (Fig. 3A) (ANOVA, p < 0.01), suggesting that ACh released from larval PRs is required for light avoidance. We selectively disrupted ACh synthesis in Rh5- or Rh6-PRs to address the unique requirement of the Rh5-PR subtype in light avoidance. Inhibiting ACh function in Rh5-PRs (rh5>ChaRNAi), but not in Rh6-PRs (rh6>ChaRNAi), abrogated light avoidance (Fig. 3A; p < 0.01). Therefore Rh5-PRs mediate light avoidance through release of ACh.
ACh from RH5-PRs and PDF-negative tim-expressing neurons are required for light avoidance behavior. A, Knockdown of ACh function in Rh5-PRs impairs light avoidance. The parental GMR-Gal4 (71.8%; SEM, 3.9), UAS-ChaRNAi (68.9%; SEM, 1.9), rh6-Gal4 (71.3%; SEM, 4.1), and rh5-Gal4 (64.4%; SEM 2.8) larvae were not significantly different from each other or rh6>ChaRNAi (73.8%; SEM, 3.7) larvae, while GMR>ChaRNAi (51.6%; SEM, 4.3) and rh5>ChaRNAi (49.5%; SEM, 2.3) reduced light avoidance compared with single-transgene controls (n = 10; ANOVA, p < 0.01). B, Inclusion of the Cha-Gal80 transgene rescues GMR-Gal4, UAS-ShiTS1 larvae decreased light avoidance at 30°C (54.3%; SEM, 2.7; n = 11) compared with 25°C (71.8%; SEM, 2.4; n = 29). GMR-Gal4, UAS-ShiTS1; Cha-Gal80 larvae do not reduce light avoidance compared with GMR-Gal4, UAS-ShiTS1 controls at 25°C (83.9%; SEM 4.3; n = 8). C, Disrupting ACh input in all clock neurons (LNs, DN1s and DN2s) impairs light avoidance. Control larvae harboring tim-Gal4 (65.2%; SEM, 3.7; n = 8) or UAS-α-Btx transgenes (67.0%; SEM 3.5; n = 10) had greater light avoidance than tim>α-Btx larvae (48.4%; SEM 3.3; n = 9). Light avoidance in pdf-Gal4 (66.1%; SEM 2.4; n = 8) and pdf>α-Btx larvae (64.8%; SEM 2.9; n = 9) did not differ from that of other controls. (ANOVA; p < 0.01). D, E, Larvae were tested in LD (D) or DD (E). LN-ablated larvae did not differ from wild type at either ZT24 or CT24, while tim-Gal4>UAS-hid, UAS-rpr larvae reduced light avoidance compared with single-transgene controls and larvae with ablated LNs (pdf>hid,rpr) (ANOVA, p < 0.01; n = 10). F, F′, pdf-Gal4 labels LNs (F) and expression of UAS-hid,rpr by pdf-Gal4 (F′) ablates the LNs; labeled with pdf>CD8::GFP (green) and anti-PDF (red) (F; pdf>CD8::GFP; F′, pdf>CD8::GFP>hid,rpr). G, Larvae with silenced LNs (pdf>Kir2.1) avoided light at wild-type levels (73.3%; SEM, 2.8; n = 10). H, Larvae with silenced DN2 and fifth LN silenced (tim-Gal4,cry-Gal80;UAS-Kir2.1) display strongly reduced light avoidance (60.4%; SEM, 2.4; n = 12) compared with Gal4 or UAS-responder lines (n = 12).Error bars represent ±SEM.
To examine the role of PRs in light avoidance, we conditionally blocked synaptic transmission with UAS-ShibireTS1 (UAS-ShiTS1). Larvae in which UAS-ShiTS1 is under the control of GMR-Gal4 avoided light normally at 20°C, but they were blind at 30°C (ANOVA, p < 0.001). We genetically rescued the GMR-Gal4-induced blindness of UAS-ShiTS1 using Cha-Gal80 to inhibit Gal4 function selectively in cholinergic cells (Kitamoto, 2002). Larvae in which GMR-Gal4 was inhibited with Cha-Gal80 had wild-type light avoidance at nonpermissive temperature (Fig. 3B).
Together with TIM-degradation experiments, our findings reveal that larval PRs confer both light entrainment of the circadian clock and rapid-light avoidance behavior. Both Rh5- and Rh6-PRs are capable of inducing TIM degradation in pacemaker neurons, while only Rh5-PRs are required for light avoidance. Both behaviors also require ACh produced by Rh5- and/or Rh6-PRs.
Light avoidance requires acetylcholine reception in clock neurons
To test the roles of LNs and other clock neurons in light avoidance, we assayed whether light avoidance requires reception of ACh by tim-expressing neurons (Mazzoni et al., 2005; Gong, 2009). We adopted a previously used strategy to inhibit ACh reception, and engineered a membrane-tethered α-bungarotoxin (α-Btx) that was shown to block, in a cell-autonomous manner, the response of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in Xenopus oocytes and zebrafish embryos (Ibañez-Tallon et al., 2004) (see Materials and Methods). We generated a line of flies containing UAS-α-Btx, which was then crossed to a line containing the pan-neuronal elav-Gal4 driver to test its efficiency. Pan-neuronal expression of α-Btx resulted in larval lethality, presumably due to widespread blockade of cholinergic transmission (data not shown). Lethality has also been reported in a Chats mutant allele at nonpermissive temperature (Kitamoto et al., 1992). Larvae expressing α-Btx in all clock neurons (tim-Gal4>α-Btx) were blind (Fig. 3C) (ANOVA, p < 0.001). In contrast, larvae expressing UAS-α-Btx in only LNs (pdf>α-Btx) did not differ significantly from the parental pdf-Gal4 or UAS-α-Btx animals (Fig. 3C). Thus, reception of input from cholinergic neurons is required for light avoidance in tim-expressing neurons, but not in the PDF-expressing LNs.
PDF neurons are dispensable for light avoidance
Larval neurons expressing circadian clock genes comprise the four PDF-expressing LNs, a fifth PDF-negative LN, and two pairs of dorsal neurons called DN1s and DN2s (Kaneko and Hall, 2000). All of these neurons express tim. We had previously reported that the circadian LNs act downstream of the larval eye to mediate light avoidance (Mazzoni et al., 2005). However, using a different assay, another group reported that larvae with ablated LNs display normal photophobic behavior (Hassan et al., 2005). In addition, we described above that ACh reception by LNs is not required for light avoidance, whereas ACh signaling from Rh5-PRs is required.
To conclusively determine whether LNs are required for light avoidance, we used UAS-hid and rpr to ablate all circadian clock neurons (tim-Gal4), or only LNs (pdf-Gal4; Fig. 3F). Expression of hid and rpr with tim-Gal4 or pdf-Gal4 efficiently ablated either class of neurons (Fig. 3F′; data not shown). To functionally test the role of these neurons, larvae were entrained to LD cycles and assayed for light avoidance, either when larvae had been exposed to light for 12 h (at ZT12), or after being transferred to DD, and assayed at the end of the second day in DD [circadian time 12 (CT12)]. Consistent with our previous report, larvae in DD had higher light avoidance scores than larvae taken from LD (Mazzoni et al., 2005) (p < 0.001). This is presumably due to desensitization of the visual system by light (Mazzoni et al., 2005). However, in contrast to our previous report (Mazzoni et al., 2005), we found no effect of LN ablation on light avoidance: LN-ablated larvae (pdf>hid,rpr) displayed wild-type photophobic behavior at both ZT12 and CT12 (p > 0.85, p > 0.97) (Fig. 3D,E). Ablation of all clock neurons with tim>hid,rpr severely disrupted light avoidance compared with larvae harboring UAS-hid, UAS-rpr, or tim-Gal4 transgenes alone, and PDF-expressing LN-ablated larvae (pdf>hid,rpr; p < 0.001) (Fig. 3D,E). Light avoidance of tim>hid,rpr was not different in LD compared with DD conditions (Fig. 3D,E). Therefore tim-expressing neurons are required for light avoidance, while pdf-expressing LNs are dispensable.
Next, we addressed the subpopulation of tim-expressing neurons mediating light avoidance. We silenced distinct populations of tim neurons with the inward-rectifying K+ channel mKir2.1 to circumvent potential developmental problems by genetic ablation experiments and to preserve the integrity of the neuronal circuit. As expected, silencing all tim neurons with mKir2.1 disrupted light avoidance, while larvae expressing UAS-mKir2.1 in PDF-expressing LNs avoided light as well as wild type (Fig. 3G), confirming that these neurons are not required for light avoidance. To definitively confirm that light avoidance defects in larvae with disrupted tim neurons were not due to LN function, we prevented expression of mKir2.1 in LNs using pdf-Gal80 (tim-Gal4, pdf-Gal80, UAS-mKir2.1). These larvae had reduced light avoidance (Fig. 3G). No behavioral alterations were observed by the expression of Gal80 alone (data not shown). In larvae, cry is expressed in all tim-expressing neurons except the DN2 cluster and the fifth LN (Kitamoto, 2002). cry-Gal4, UAS-mKir2.1 larvae did not show reduced light avoidance but, instead, increased light avoidance, suggesting that DN1s normally reduce photophobicity (addressed further by Collins and Blau, unpublished observation). To address the possible role of DN2s and the fifth LN in light avoidance, we used the tim-Gal4,cry-Gal80 combination. Interestingly tim-Gal4,cry-Gal80, UAS-mKir2.1 larvae exhibited reduced light avoidance (Fig. 3H). Therefore, DN2 neurons and/or the fifth LN, but not PDF-expressing LNs and DN1 neurons, are required for normal light avoidance.
To gain deeper insight into the functional relationship between larval PRs and their target clock neurons, we used confocal microscopy to assess their gross anatomy. pdf-Gal4 drove expression in four LNs with dendritic arborizations in the LON. Comparably, tim-Gal4 drove expression in all clock neurons, including all 5 LNs whose projections innervate the LON (Fig. 4B). In tim-Gal4, pdf-Gal80 larvae, only a single LN cell body was observed, but PR termini still made apparent contacts with GFP-labeled LN projections, suggesting that larval PRs connect to the non-PDF-expressing fifth LN (Fig. 4C; data not shown). This innervation was also detected when GFP was expressed with tim-Gal4,cry-Gal80 that only labels the fifth LN and DN2 neurons (Fig. 4D). Thus, in all cases, dendritic arborizations of clock neurons into the LON were observed.
PDF-expressing LNs and fifth LN innervate the LON, while DN2s do not. A–D, Projection pattern of the neurons expressing UAS-CD8::GFP under the control of pdf-Gal4 (A; labeling four LNs), tim-Gal4 (B; labeling all LNs, DN2s, and DN1s), tim-Gal4, pdf-Gal80 (C; labeling the fifth LN, DN2s, and DN1s), tim-Gal4, cry-Gal80 (D; labeling the fifth LN and DN2s). Anti-GFP in green, projections of Rh6-PRs showing the LON region labeled with anti-Rh6 in red. E–I, Genetic flip-out experiments showing anatomy of individual neurons generated with hs-flp; tim-Gal4/UAS-FRT-CD2-STOP-FRT-CD8::GFP (anti-GFP in green, anti-CD2 in red). E, G, PDF-expressing LNs connect to the LON and display a typical axonal extension toward the dorsal protocerebrum (arrow). F, H, The fifth LN innervates the LON and shows a very distinct projection pattern with various ramifications toward the central brain (arrows). I, The DN2 neuron does not show innervation of the LON; it projects its axon toward the contralateral brain hemisphere (dashed line shows the border of the brain hemispheres; Oes delimits the esophageal opening). Brackets in A–D, G, and H show the LON region.
To further distinguish innervation of PDF-expressing LNs, the fifth LN and DN2s, we assessed their anatomical properties using flip-out experiments (Wong et al., 2002). As previously described, we found that the PDF-expressing LNs innervate the LON and project into a distinct domain of the dorsal protocerebrum (Helfrich-Förster, 1997; Helfrich-Förster et al., 2002; Malpel et al., 2002) (Fig. 4E,G). In agreement with previous anatomical analysis (Helfrich-Förster et al., 2007), single-cell analysis of the fifth LN revealed prominent innervation of the LON with a broader projection domain than PDF-expressing LNs (Fig. 4F,H). Consistent with previously published data (Kaneko and Hall, 2000; Shafer et al., 2006), we observed that the DN2s project to the contralateral side of the brain, but do not show dendritic arborizations in the LON (Fig. 4I). Together, these findings suggest that PR projections not only contact PDF-expressing LNs, but also connect with other second-order neurons within the LON (likely the fifth LNs) that might mediate light avoidance.
Discussion
Genetic silencing and ablation experiments revealed that larval Rh5-PRs in combination with the fifth LN and DN2s are essential for rapid photophobic behavior. rh5 mutants and genetic ablation of Rh5-PRs strongly reduced light avoidance. In addition, selectively silencing the fifth LN and DN2s by expressing UAS-mKir2.1 in tim-Gal4;cry-Gal80 larvae disrupts light avoidance. Identifying direct Rh5-PR target neurons within the LON will further help our understanding of the circuitry underlying light avoidance. These second-order neurons may either include or signal to the fifth LN and/or the DN2 clock neurons that we have implicated in light avoidance. The connectivity of fifth LN with PRs makes it a prime candidate for directly mediating light avoidance.
Our results raise the possibility of distinct roles for PDF-positive and PDF-negative LNs, where PDF-positive LNs drive circadian behavior, while PDF-negative LNs (fifth LN) would be critical for processing light cues for avoidance behavior. Adult flies have distinct types of LNvs, large (l-LNvs) and small (s-LNvs), both of which express PDF and appear to have divergent functions. The PDF-positive LNvs appear to be the primary pacemaker cells in DD and regulate the morning activity peak in LD, while the fifth PDF-negative s-LNvs and some PDF-negative dorsal LNs, an adult-specific LN population, help regulate the evening activity peak in LD (Rieger et al., 2006).
To date, the connectivity of DN1 and DN2 neurons with the fifth LN and larval PRs remains unclear. It has recently been shown that explorative head-swinging behavior in response to a light pulse in late third instar larvae requires serotonergic neurons (Rodriguez Moncalvo and Campos, 2009). Dendritic arbors of serotonergic neurons in the LON dramatically increase during larval life. Interestingly, dendritic growth of these serotonergic neurons requires Rh6-PRs. Furthermore, serotonergic projections are in close proximity to the DNs and fifth LN (Hamasaka and Nässel, 2006). Therefore, it seems likely that Rh6-PRs function in combination with these serotonergic neurons for light-dependent behaviors (Rodriguez Moncalvo and Campos, 2005). It will be of great interest to investigate whether serotonin neurons functionally connect Rh5-PRs to the DN2 neurons or form connections in the LON with the fifth LN. It is possible that functional connections between serotonergic neurons and the clock neurons are required for light avoidance. Closely examining the neuroanatomy of LON-innervating neurons may reveal the neural connectivity by which larval PRs or the fifth LN communicates with DN1/DN2 neurons to regulate light avoidance. In addition, future work will examine the synaptic targets of Rh5-PRs that bypass the LON.
Either Rh5-PRs or Rh6-PRs are sufficient for normal cycling of gene expression within the larval pacemaker neurons. Disrupting the function of both rh5 and rh6 results in larvae that are less sensitive to TIM degradation-induced light pulses, but are still able to show normal eclosion patterns. Normal eclosion in the absence of the larval eye is likely mediated by cry function in the LNs. Future experiments examining eclosion patterns in rh5;rh6;cry triple mutants will be informative in revealing functional redundancy within this system. Together, our results reveal how both redundant and distinct PR connectivity modulates two visual behaviors. Larval PRs confer both light-dependent entrainment of the circadian clock and rapid-light avoidance behavior. Interestingly, the function for rapid-light behaviors already differs at the level of the PR-subtypes in the eye. While the Rh5-subtype is essential for rapid-light avoidance, the Rh6-subtype is dispensable.
Clock entrainment and rapid-light avoidance require ACh release from PRs. We have developed genetically expressed α-bungarotoxin as a tool for probing ACh function in neural populations and find that expression of α-Btx in all circadian neurons disrupts light avoidance. Future work refining the sites of ACh function in light avoidance should identify specific populations of central brain neurons regulating light avoidance. We have not been able to reproduce the results showing loss of photophobic behavior after ablation of PDF neurons reported in Mazzoni et al. (2005). Instead, we find that the circadian PDF-expressing LNs are dispensable for light avoidance, consistent with findings of Hassan et al. (2005). Further analysis of the tim-expressing neurons regulating light avoidance revealed a possible role for DN2 neurons and/or that the fifth LN is required for this behavior. It is likely that Rh5-PR neurons have LON targets in addition to PDF-expressing LNs that signal light avoidance. Therefore, we have also identified divergence in the central neurons mediating visual and circadian behaviors.
Notes
Supplemental material for this article is available at http://www.unifr.ch/zoology/eng/home/research-groups/sprecher/sprecherpub. The supplemental material consists of five figures and the corresponding figure legends: (1) Projection pattern of PRs in rh5,rh6 double mutants, projections of Rh5-PRs to the deeper brain; (2) Time course analysis of photobehavior of yw, rh6, rh5, and rh5;rh6 double mutants; (3) Eclosion rhythms of wild-type, rh6, rh5, and rh5;rh6 double mutants; (4) Connection of larval PRs to the 5th LN; and (5) Working model for the distinct roles of larval PR-subtypes. This material has not been peer reviewed.
Footnotes
-
This work was funded by Grant EY013012 from the National Eye Institute–National Institutes of Health (NIH) to C.D., by Grant GM063911 from NIH to J.B., by the Swiss National Science Foundation, the Novartis Foundation, and the Janggen-Pöhn Stiftung (to S.G.S.), and by National Institute of General Medical Sciences National Research Service Award 5F32GM086207 to A.C.K., and was conducted in a facility constructed with the support of a Research Facilities Improvement Grant C06 RR-15518-01 from the National Center for Research Resources, NIH. We thank A. H. Brand, S. Britt, T. Cook, the Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, V. Hartenstein, F. Hirth, the Kyoto Stock Center, K. Matthews, M. Rosbash, the Vienna Drosophila RNAi Center for flies and antibodies, and Peter McKenney and June Ng for preliminary studies. We thank Scott Waddell (University of Massachusetts Medical School) for generously providing the ChaRNAi line. Special thanks to our colleagues at New York University, the Blau, Desplan, and Sprecher laboratories for fruitful discussions, to Mike Nitabach for advice on construction of the tethered α-Btx transgene, and to Frank Pichaud (Medical Research Council, London).
- Correspondence should be addressed to Simon G. Sprecher at the above address. simon.sprecher{at}unifr.ch