Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

The Importance of Ventral Premotor Cortex for Body Ownership Processing

Robin Bekrater-Bodmann, Jens Foell and Sandra Kamping
Journal of Neuroscience 29 June 2011, 31 (26) 9443-9444; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2302-11.2011
Robin Bekrater-Bodmann
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jens Foell
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sandra Kamping
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

The perception of one's own hand is an eminently coherent impression: proprioceptive, tactile, and visual inputs usually correspond perfectly. Current theoretical frameworks postulate that this multimodal integration is required for a feeling of ownership of the body, and is ultimately accompanied by a sense of self. This might seem trivial; but actually, it is necessary for the discrimination between one's own body and its surroundings, as well as the distinction between self and other. However, how the brain manages the feeling of body integrity with all its parts remains an open question.

The major mystery of body perception can be expressed by this simple question: Why do I perceive my hand as belonging to me? The ventral premotor cortex (PMv) seems to play a key role in this integrative process. Early studies showed that multimodal receptive fields in the PMv of primates appear to combine visual and somatosensory input to locate one's own limb in peripersonal space (Graziano et al., 2000). Botvinick and Cohen (1998) complemented these findings by showing that a visible rubber hand is experienced as belonging to the subject's body when both the rubber hand and the subject's own hand are stimulated congruently in terms of location and temporal consistency. The authors assumed an involvement of the PMv in this so-called rubber hand illusion (RHI), which was confirmed by Ehrsson et al. (2004) using functional magnetic resonance imaging, indicating that activity in PMv is positively correlated with the experience of body ownership. However, it remained unclear whether multisensory integration within the PMv is necessary to generate a feeling of body ownership, or whether the neuronal processes in the RHI paradigm are unique and different from usual body perception processes.

This question was recently examined by Zeller and colleagues (2011). The authors assessed the experience of illusory ownership of an artificial limb in patients suffering from circumscribed unilateral ischemic strokes, which damaged the basal ganglia and periventricular white matter, affecting the putamen, precentral and postcentral cortices, and the thalamus. To evaluate the importance of lesion side, the RHI was induced both for the ipsilesional and contralesional side of the body, and was rated by using an RHI perception questionnaire. In doing so, the authors assessed the prevalence of RHI failure (RHIF), which was defined as a lack of the sensation of perceived ownership of the artificial limb. Additionally, most of the participants were asked how strongly they experienced the contralesional hand as belonging to themselves, assessing the presence of asomatognosia following stroke. These subjective responses were related to the degree of lesion by performing voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) to identify affected voxels that significantly correlated with the perception scores. These voxels were used as seed areas for a subsequent investigation using probabilistic diffusion tractography (PDT) in an averaged healthy brain to identify subcortical white matter tracts passing through these regions.

The authors observed contralesional RHIF in 25.7% and additional ipsilesional RHIF in 10.0% of the stroke patients. However, RHIF was not necessarily associated with asomatognosia, which was found in 28.1% of the patients. The inability to perceive illusory body ownership during RHI induction was associated with clearly defined white matter lesions, whereas control responses did not correlate with brain damage, indicating a specific relation between the experience of illusory body ownership and connecting fibers. PDT for the seed regions obtained in the VLSM showed that both RHIF and asomatognosia were associated with connections between frontal and parietal cortices, frontal operculum, thalamus, basal ganglia, and the cerebellum. The only prominent difference was an additional strong connection to PMv for the RHIF analysis, which was lacking in the asomatognosia analysis. The authors interpreted these results as evidence for causal neuronal involvement of the PMv in illusory body ownership experience.

Zeller and colleagues' (2011) results have implications for the study of body perception in general. Their findings indicate that the inability to perceive illusory ownership of an artificial limb (i.e., RHIF) and the loss of ability to recognize that a body part belongs to one's own body (i.e., asomatognosia) might involve two different neuronal processes in at least partially different brain areas. The consequence of this finding, namely that body ownership itself is not necessarily mediated by the PMv, is an important merit of the study, complementing convincingly the theoretical concept of the function of PMv.

Gentile et al. (2011) showed that the PMv reacts in an additive manner to incoming sensory input; i.e., the PMv is more active during visuotactile stimulation of one's own hand than during visual perception or tactile stimulation alone, indicating an integrative, stimulation-linking role of the PMv, which was also shown for the posterior parietal cortex (PCp) (Nakashita et al., 2008). Activity in both structures is positively correlated with vividness of RHI perceptions (Ehrsson et al., 2004), complementing findings which showed that both the PCp and the PMv can bridge the gap between multimodal sensory mismatches (Bremmer et al., 2001). The results reported in the present study suggest that stroke patients affected by lesions in the white tissue connecting the PMv with other brain regions were less able to integrate proprioceptive, visual, and tactile stimuli into a corresponding event, which might be the basis for the experience of RHI sensations, but not necessarily for common body ownership.

We would, however, like to point out that there is reason to suspect that the prevalence of RHIF due to stroke given in the study by Zeller and colleagues (2011) may be based on a faulty assumption: previous studies (e.g., Ehrsson et al., 2004) found that ∼25% of healthy participants are RHI non-responders; i.e., they are inherently unable to perceive the illusory perceptions induced by the RHI paradigm, even in the absence of structural failure. The RHIF rate of stroke patients reported in the present study is similar to this baseline. Further, the RHIF prevalence found in an additional healthy control sample might be underestimated (7.5% unilateral and 2.5% bilateral RHIF), suggesting that behavioral approaches to quantify illusory body ownership (Botvinick and Cohen, 1998) are necessary to evaluate the present results.

One additional peculiarity of the study by Zeller and colleagues (2011) is the lack of consistent cortical lesions associated with asomatognosia, especially within the parietal cortex (Feinberg et al., 2010), suggesting that the present method to assess this complex perceptional feature by questionnaire might not be sufficient, due to the abstract nature of this phenomenon. As in the assessment of RHIF, behavior-based approaches (Feinberg et al., 2010) might be more appropriate to obtain a valid diagnosis of asomatognosia.

Nevertheless, in light of the results by Zeller and colleagues (2011), the PMv has to be regarded as a gate that must be passed through to link multimodal sensory input and to incorporate external objects into the body representation, but not to create ownership itself. What could be the natural use of this function, when no artificial hands have to be incorporated into the body? Presumably, it is essential for the ability of primates to use tools. PMv, PCp, and other cortical areas are activated during tool use (Macaluso and Maravita, 2010), indicating that a widely distributed network involving frontal and parietal areas is geared to interact with the environment as immediately as possible. The PMv is a candidate region to act as a mediator between the internal and the external representation of the environment by several functions, which can be summarized as follows: (1) coding for the peripersonal space as well as its extensions, (2) coding for postural features of body parts within the peripersonal space, (3) ability to integrate multimodal sensory input, and (4) close connections to other body perception areas (such as the PCp and the insular cortex). As supported by the data from Zeller and colleagues (2011), the sensations during the RHI paradigm might be an involuntary consequence of these functions. This simple experimental setup may therefore be considered an important window into the mechanisms that allow us to pick up an object and to use it as an extension of ourselves.

In conclusion, Zeller and colleagues (2011) have taken an important and necessary step in the study of which paths the brain uses to generate the experience of body ownership. Even taking into account our critical points, they illuminate the importance of PMv for a special kind of body ownership processing and therefore filled a critical gap in the research of body perception.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short, critical reviews of recent papers in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

  • This work was supported by the PHANTOMMIND advanced grant of the European Research Council (FP7/2007–2013)/ERC Grant Agreement No. 230249.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Robin Bekrater-Bodmann, Department of Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, Central Institute of Mental Health, Square J 5, 68159 Mannheim, Germany. r.bekrater-bodmann{at}zi-mannheim.de

References

  1. ↵
    1. Botvinick M,
    2. Cohen J
    (1998) Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature 391:756.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Bremmer F,
    2. Schlack A,
    3. Shah NJ,
    4. Zafiris O,
    5. Kubischik M,
    6. Hoffmann K,
    7. Zilles K,
    8. Fink GR
    (2001) Polymodal motion processing in posterior parietal and premotor cortex: a human fMRI study strongly implies equivalencies between humans and monkeys. Neuron 29:287–296.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Ehrsson HH,
    2. Spence C,
    3. Passingham RE
    (2004) That's my hand! Activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Science 305:875–877.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Feinberg TE,
    2. Venneri A,
    3. Simone AM,
    4. Fan Y,
    5. Northoff G
    (2010) The neuroanatomy of asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 81:276–281.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Gentile G,
    2. Petkova VI,
    3. Ehrsson HH
    (2011) Integration of visual and tactile signals from the hand in the human brain: an FMRI study. J Neurophysiol 105:910–922.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Graziano MS,
    2. Cooke DF,
    3. Taylor CS
    (2000) Coding the location of the arm by sight. Science 290:1782–1786.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Macaluso E,
    2. Maravita A
    (2010) The representation of space near the body through touch and vision. Neuropsychologia 48:782–795.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Nakashita S,
    2. Saito DN,
    3. Kochiyama T,
    4. Honda M,
    5. Tanabe HC,
    6. Sadato N
    (2008) Tactile-visual integration in the posterior parietal cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain Res Bull 75:513–525.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Zeller D,
    2. Gross C,
    3. Bartsch A,
    4. Johansen-Berg H,
    5. Classen J
    (2011) Ventral premotor cortex may be required for dynamic changes in the feeling of limb ownership: a lesion study. J Neurosci 31:4852–4857.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 31 (26)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 31, Issue 26
29 Jun 2011
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The Importance of Ventral Premotor Cortex for Body Ownership Processing
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
The Importance of Ventral Premotor Cortex for Body Ownership Processing
Robin Bekrater-Bodmann, Jens Foell, Sandra Kamping
Journal of Neuroscience 29 June 2011, 31 (26) 9443-9444; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2302-11.2011

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
The Importance of Ventral Premotor Cortex for Body Ownership Processing
Robin Bekrater-Bodmann, Jens Foell, Sandra Kamping
Journal of Neuroscience 29 June 2011, 31 (26) 9443-9444; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2302-11.2011
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

  • Author Response to Bekrater-Bodmann et al. Journal Club
    Prof. Joseph Classen and Daniel Zeller
    Published on: 26 July 2017
  • Published on: (26 July 2017)
    Page navigation anchor for Author Response to Bekrater-Bodmann et al. Journal Club
    Author Response to Bekrater-Bodmann et al. Journal Club
    • Prof. Joseph Classen, Author, University of Leipzig
    • Other Contributors:
      • Daniel Zeller

    A response to this article may be found here.

    Competing Interests: None declared.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Early Neural Development of Social Interaction Perception in the Superior Temporal Sulcus
  • Universal Coding for Uncertainty?
  • Beyond Motor Control: Diffusion MRI Reveals Associations between the Cerebello-VTA Pathway and Socio-affective Behaviors in Humans
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.