Abstract
Animals must ensure that they can execute behaviors important for physiological homeostasis under constantly changing environmental conditions. The neural mechanisms that regulate this behavioral robustness are not well understood. The nematode Caenorhabditis elegans thermoregulates primarily via modulation of navigation behavior. Upon encountering temperatures higher than its cultivation temperature (Tc), C. elegans exhibits negative thermotaxis toward colder temperatures using a biased random walk strategy. We find that C. elegans exhibits robust negative thermotaxis bias under conditions of varying Tc and temperature ranges. By cell ablation and cell-specific rescue experiments, we show that the ASI chemosensory neurons are newly identified components of the thermosensory circuit, and that different combinations of ASI and the previously identified AFD and AWC thermosensory neurons are necessary and sufficient under different conditions to execute a negative thermotaxis strategy. ASI responds to temperature stimuli within a defined operating range defined by Tc, and signaling from AFD regulates the bounds of this operating range, suggesting that neuromodulation among thermosensory neurons maintains coherence of behavioral output. Our observations demonstrate that a negative thermotaxis navigational strategy can be generated via different combinations of thermosensory neurons acting degenerately, and emphasize the importance of defining context when analyzing neuronal contributions to a behavior.
Introduction
Animals must maintain cellular and physiological homeostasis over a wide range of conditions. Although robustness can be achieved via dedicated networks that function only under specific conditions, this strategy does not generate reliable outputs when faced with unforeseen circumstances (Tononi et al., 1999). In contrast, degenerate systems, in which multiple distinct components contribute to the generation of the same output in a context-dependent manner, can permit adaptive flexibility and tolerate perturbation (Tononi et al., 1999; Edelman and Gally, 2001; Wagner, 2005; Whitacre, 2010). The role of degeneracy in generating robustness has been studied experimentally and theoretically (Hartman et al., 2001; Prinz et al., 2004; Wagner, 2005; Wagner and Wright, 2007), but much remains to be understood about the mechanisms that ensure behavioral robustness under varying conditions.
The nematode Caenorhabditis elegans exhibits a particularly robust and adaptable behavior on thermal gradients. C. elegans forms a memory of its cultivation temperature (Tc) and exhibits stereotyped behaviors at specific temperature ranges relative to Tc. At temperatures higher than Tc, worms move down the gradient toward colder temperatures in a behavior called negative thermotaxis (Hedgecock and Russell, 1975; Mori and Ohshima, 1995). Tc memory is plastic and can be reset upon cultivation at a different temperature (Hedgecock and Russell, 1975).
Modulation of locomotory behavior is the primary mode of thermoregulation in C. elegans (Ramot et al., 2008b). Given the critical role of temperature in regulating nearly all aspects of C. elegans physiology [e.g., Klass (1977), Golden and Riddle (1984), and van der Linden et al. (2010)], it is essential that worms maintain their body temperature within a physiologically optimal range. Theoretical simulations have shown that negative thermotaxis is remarkably robust and resistant to environmental perturbations or to genetic changes affecting the animals' locomotion (Ramot et al., 2008b). Indeed, systematic analyses of negative thermotaxis indicate that worms can exhibit this behavior over varying conditions such as a wide temperature range, different initial temperatures encountered, and differing gradients that they are expected to experience in the soil in different climates (Yamada and Ohshima, 2003; Anderson et al., 2007; Ramot et al., 2008b; Nakazato and Mochizuki, 2009; Jurado et al., 2010). Thus, this behavior has evolved to be both robust and highly flexible.
The sensory neurons required for negative thermotaxis have been partly elucidated. The bilateral AFD neurons are a major thermosensory neuron type in C. elegans (Mori and Ohshima, 1995), and exhibit temperature-induced changes in activity at temperatures above Tc (Kimura et al., 2004; Clark et al., 2006; Ramot et al., 2008a). The threshold of temperature responses in AFD is determined by Tc (Kimura et al., 2004; Biron et al., 2006; Clark et al., 2006; Ramot et al., 2008a), and provides a cellular correlate for Tc memory. In addition to AFD, the AWC sensory neurons also respond to temperature in a Tc-dependent manner and modulate negative thermotaxis (Biron et al., 2008; Kuhara et al., 2008). However, the roles of AFD and AWC in generating thermotactic navigation behaviors have been examined only under a limited set of conditions. Thus, how these neurons contribute to thermosensory behaviors under different conditions and whether additional neurons are also required are unknown.
Here we show that the strategy underlying negative thermotaxis behavior is generated via different configurations of thermosensory neurons under conditions of varying Tc and temperature range of the thermal gradient. We identify the ASI chemosensory neurons as a new component of the thermosensory circuit, and show that this neuron type responds to temperature changes within a Tc-dependent operating range. Interestingly, we find that AFD may set the operating range of ASI under specific conditions via peptidergic neuromodulation. Our observations describe a mechanism that may buffer the network from genetic or physical perturbation, and allow animals to mount a robust and coherent behavioral response over a wide range of environmental conditions.
Materials and Methods
Strains.
The wild-type strain was C. elegans Bristol, strain N2, raised on OP50 bacterial lawns. Mutant strains used were PR678 tax-4(p678); KR1787 unc-13(e51); CB169 unc-31(e169); and GN112 [pgIs1 (AFD-ablated); gift from Miriam Goodman, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA] (Glauser et al., 2011).
The AWC-ablated strain (PY7502) was generated via expression of recCaspases (Chelur and Chalfie, 2007) under ceh-36 (Kim et al., 2010) promoter sequences together with gfp driven under the srtx-1 promoter (Colosimo et al., 2004) to visualize loss of AWC. The ceh-36 regulatory sequences used drive expression exclusively in the AWC neurons (Kim et al., 2010). Loss of AWC neurons was confirmed by the absence of gfp expression driven under the srtx-1 promoter and failure of AWC-ablated animals to respond to an AWC-sensed odorant (Bargmann et al., 1993). The ASI-ablated strain (PY7505) was generated via expression of recCaspases under the gpa-4 (Jansen et al., 1999) and gcy-27 (Ortiz et al., 2006) promoters together with gfp expression under the gcy-27 promoter to confirm loss of ASI. Loss of ASI neurons was also confirmed by failure of the ASI neurons to uptake a lipophilic dye (Herman and Hedgecock, 1990), and constitutive entry of ASI-ablated animals into the alternate dauer developmental stage (Bargmann and Horvitz, 1991; Ailion and Thomas, 2000). A fraction of ASI-ablated animals bypassed the dauer stage and grew into adults, allowing examination of their thermosensory behavioral phenotypes. recCaspase-expressing constructs were injected at 50 ng/μl. Extrachromosomal arrays carrying the recCaspase constructs and cell-specific markers were stably integrated into the genome via UV irradiation-induced mutagenesis, and outcrossed at least twice before analyses. Strains lacking two neuron types were generated by crossing together strains lacking each individual cell type.
Strains expressing wild-type tax-4 cDNA in specific cell types were generated by injecting the expression construct at 20 ng/μl with the unc-122p::dsRed coinjection marker at 50 ng/μl. At least two independent transgenic lines were generated for each construct and exhibited similar behaviors. For strains expressing tax-4 in multiple cell types, constructs driving expression in individual cell types were coinjected. Constructs driving pkc-1(gf) expression were injected at 50 ng/μl together with a coinjection marker. Corresponding wild-type and AFD-ablated strains shown in Figure 3F carried the same extrachromosomal arrays expressing pkc-1(gf).
Molecular biology.
Wild-type tax-4 cDNA sequences (Satterlee et al., 2004) were expressed under the following promoters: ttx-1 (AFD) (Satterlee et al., 2001), ceh-36 (AWC) (Kim et al., 2010), srbc-65 (ASI) (Kim et al., 2009), osm-10 (ASH, ASI, PHA, PHB) (Hart et al., 1999), ops-1 (ASG) (Sarafi-Reinach and Sengupta, 2000). Promoters driving recCaspase expression were as follows: ceh-36 (AWC) (Chelur and Chalfie, 2007), gpa-4 and gcy-27 (ASI) (Jansen et al., 1999), str-3 and gpa-4 [ASI (EC1)] (Jansen et al., 1999; Peckol et al., 2001), gcy-27 and str-3 [ASI (EC2)] (Peckol et al., 2001), srtx-1 [AFD and AWC (EC1)] (Colosimo et al., 2004). The pkc-1(gf) cDNA was expressed under the following promoters: gcy-8 (AFD) (Yu et al., 1997), ceh-36 (AWC) (Kim et al., 2010), and srbc-65 (ASI) (Kim et al., 2009). GCaMP 2.2b was expressed under the gpa-4 promoter (courtesy of K. Kim, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA; and C. Bargmann, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY). Constructs were generated in C. elegans expression vectors (gift from A. Fire) or using the Gateway system (Invitrogen).
Negative thermotaxis behavioral assays.
Assays were performed essentially as previously described (Chi et al., 2007; Clark et al., 2007; Biron et al., 2008). Worms were cultivated overnight with food at the desired Tc. Before the assay, 15 young adult animals were briefly transferred onto a 6 cm NGM plate without food, and then transferred onto the center of a 10 cm agar plate and placed on an aluminum plate. A linear temperature gradient was generated and monitored on the aluminum plate by Peltier thermoelectric temperature controllers (Oven Industries). The temperature of the agar on the assay plate was measured using a two probe digital thermometer (Fluke Electronics) to confirm accuracy. Animals were allowed to move freely for 5 min before the start of the assay. Worm movement was then recorded using a CCD camera (Panasonic WV-CP484) for 30 min with images acquired at 1 Hz. The center of mass for each animal was tracked in each frame by thresholding. Centers of mass were considered particles and individual particles were tracked over time to reflect worm trajectories. Images were analyzed using LabView (National Instruments) and custom-written scripts in MATLAB (The MathWorks). Forward movement of duration >6 s (1 body length) were considered runs. Run trajectories were analyzed with respect to gradient direction. Turns were defined as changes in direction that were >13°/s. Runs that contributed to the negative thermotaxis bias were within 45–315° or 135–225°.
Calcium imaging.
Imaging of temperature-evoked Ca2+ dynamics in ASI were performed as previously described (Biron et al., 2008). Briefly, animals expressing GCaMP 2.2b in ASI were cultivated overnight at a given temperature. Before the start of the assay, individual animals were glued to an agarose pad using cyanoacrylate glue at room temperature. The agarose pad containing the worm was transferred to an indium tin oxide covered slide with 5 μl of water between the pad and slide to ensure good thermal contact. Current controlled via temperature-regulated feedback using LabView was passed through the slide to generate heat, and temperature was measured using a T-type thermocouple. The elapsed time from removal of the animal from the incubator to initiation of imaging was <3 min. Individual animals were imaged for 15 min with upward or downward ramp stimuli and 5 min at constant temperatures at a rate of 2 Hz. Images were captured using MetaMorph (Molecular Devices) and a Hamamatsu Orca digital camera. Data were analyzed using scripts in MATLAB. A fluorescence change >10% in ASI was considered a response.
Results
The TAX-4 cyclic nucleotide-gated channel is required for robust negative thermotaxis bias under multiple growth and assay conditions
Wild-type adult C. elegans hermaphrodites exhibit strong negative thermotaxis when grown at different temperatures and placed on thermal gradients at temperatures above Tc (Hedgecock and Russell, 1975; Mori and Ohshima, 1995). To establish a baseline for our assays, we examined negative thermotaxis behaviors of wild-type animals grown at either 15°C or 20°C and placed at temperatures 4–10°C above Tc (Tstart) in the center of a linear thermal gradient on a 10 cm agar plate (Fig. 1A). Since gradient steepness was maintained at 1.0°C/cm, the range of the gradient varied as a function of Tc and Tstart (Fig. 1A). We specifically excluded starting temperatures that were below Tc + 4°C since C. elegans tracks isotherms near Tc (Hedgecock and Russell, 1975; Mori and Ohshima, 1995), and exhibition of this alternate behavior affects negative thermotaxis.
The net displacement of animals on a spatial thermal gradient over time has been used as a measure of negative thermotaxis behavior (Hedgecock and Russell, 1975; Inada et al., 2006; Ramot et al., 2008b). However, since multiple behavioral parameters can contribute to these endpoint accumulations, we focused specifically on quantifying the underlying thermotactic navigational strategy (Ryu and Samuel, 2002; Zariwala et al., 2003; Clark et al., 2007). Detailed measurements of individual animal trajectories have shown that C. elegans executes negative thermotaxis using a biased random walk strategy such that forward movement (called runs) is lengthened when animals are moving toward favorable (colder) conditions, and shortened when they are moving toward warmer conditions (Ryu and Samuel, 2002; Zariwala et al., 2003). Runs are terminated by reversals and turns, which reorient the animal in a random direction.
This navigational strategy can be quantified by calculating the thermotaxis bias [similar to the previously described cryophilic bias (Chi et al., 2007; Clark et al., 2007)], which we define as the (total duration of runs in the warmer direction) − (total duration of runs in the colder direction)/total duration of runs. Thus, a negative thermotaxis bias would enable animals to perform negative thermotaxis. Under our conditions, we found that wild-type animals grown at either 15°C or 20°C exhibited a robust negative thermotaxis bias at Tstart > Tc + 5°C (Fig. 1B,C). This bias was weakened when Tstart > Tc + 9°C for animals grown at 15°C (Fig. 1B), although a similar decline was not observed for animals grown at 20°C (Fig. 1C), possibly due to activation of nociceptive pathways at higher temperatures (Wittenburg and Baumeister, 1999; Chatzigeorgiou et al., 2010; Glauser et al., 2011). These results are consistent with but not identical to previous observations (Ramot et al., 2008b), likely due to differences in assay parameters and analyses of endpoint measurements as compared to navigational decisions (see Materials and Methods).
The TAX-4 cyclic nucleotide-gated (CNG) channel is expressed in a subset of sensory neurons including the AFD and AWC thermosensory neurons (Komatsu et al., 1996), and is required for thermotactic behaviors under the subset of conditions that have been examined (Mori and Ohshima, 1995; Komatsu et al., 1996). tax-4 mutants are atactic and fail to exhibit temperature-evoked activity in the AFD thermosensory neurons (Mori and Ohshima, 1995; Kimura et al., 2004; Ramot et al., 2008a). We determined whether TAX-4 is essential for negative thermotaxis under different assay conditions. As shown in Figure 1, B and C, tax-4 mutants were atactic regardless of Tc or Tstart. Thus, wild-type C. elegans exhibits a robust negative thermotaxis bias under varying conditions, and this behavior is TAX-4 dependent.
The ASI chemosensory neurons act partly degenerately with the AFD and AWC thermosensory neurons to generate negative thermotaxis bias under specific conditions
We next determined whether known thermosensory neurons contribute in a similar manner to negative thermotaxis behavior under different conditions, or whether additional neurons are recruited under specific circumstances. To address this issue, we restored TAX-4 function to individual tax-4-expressing sensory neurons via expression of wild-type tax-4 sequences under cell-specific or -selective promoters, and quantified thermotaxis bias. Since it was impractical to examine all conditions shown in Figure 1, B and C, we restricted our analysis to conditions where Tstart = Tc + 6°C or 8°C for Tc = 15°C, and Tc + 6°C for Tc = 20°C. Wild-type animals exhibit a robust negative thermotaxis bias under all these conditions; we elected to not examine higher starting temperatures for animals grown at 20°C to restrict our analysis to non-nociceptive conditions.
We found that restoration of tax-4 expression to either the known AFD or AWC thermosensory neurons alone did not rescue the tax-4 thermosensory behavioral defects under any condition (Fig. 2A–C), although expression in AWC rescued the defects in AWC-mediated olfactory behaviors (Bargmann et al., 1993) (data not shown). However, expression in both AFD and AWC restored negative thermotaxis bias to levels similar to those exhibited by wild-type animals (Fig. 2A–C), indicating that AFD and AWC together are sufficient to confer negative thermotaxis bias under all examined conditions.
We next investigated whether additional tax-4-expressing neurons contribute in a degenerate manner with AFD or AWC to maintain behavioral robustness. We were particularly interested in the ASI chemosensory neurons, since gene expression in ASI has previously been shown to be regulated by temperature (Schackwitz et al., 1996; Ailion and Thomas, 2000) similar to the case in AFD (Satterlee et al., 2004), and ASI is presynaptic to interneurons implicated in the thermosensory circuit (White et al., 1986; Mori and Ohshima, 1995). Unexpectedly, we found that restoration of tax-4 expression in AFD and ASI was sufficient to restore negative thermotaxis bias when Tstart = Tc + 6°C, regardless of Tc (Fig. 2A,B). However, no rescue was observed upon expression in AFD and ASI when Tstart = Tc + 8°C (Fig. 2C). We did not observe behavioral rescue upon expression in AFD and the tax-4-expressing ASG chemosensory neurons (Fig. 2A–C), upon expression in ASI alone, or together in AWC and ASI under any examined condition (Fig. 2A–C). Together, these results indicate that AFD together with either AWC or ASI is sufficient to confer negative thermotaxis bias when Tstart = Tc + 6°C in a tax-4 mutant background, but that ASI may not contribute at starting temperatures that are distant from Tc.
It is important to note that while expression of wild-type tax-4 sequences in subsets of these sensory neurons restored the cumulative negative thermotaxis bias under specific conditions, not all aspects of the overall behavior were fully rescued. Thus, individual strains exhibited differences in the duration of runs toward the cold and warm directions as a function of time when compared to wild-type animals and to each other (Table 1). Thus, while TAX-4 function in subsets of AFD, AWC, or ASI neurons is sufficient to rescue the ability of tax-4 mutants to modulate their navigation in response to temperature changes, it is likely that additional tax-4-expressing neurons also contribute to the overall behavior, further highlighting the importance of dissecting underlying strategies when examining the execution of a behavior.
Since ASI is presynaptic to AWC (White et al., 1986), we next investigated the possibility that restoration of TAX-4 function in ASI activates AWC in the tax-4 mutant background to generate negative thermotaxis bias. We reasoned that if this is the case, then the behaviors of tax-4 mutants in which TAX-4 function is restored in AFD and ASI should resemble those of tax-4 mutant animals in which TAX-4 function is restored only in AFD and in which AWC is activated via cell-specific expression of a constitutively activated allele of the pkc-1 protein kinase C gene (pkc-1(gf)). pkc-1(gf) has been shown to activate neurons likely via promotion of synaptic transmission and neuropeptide release (Okochi et al., 2005; Sieburth et al., 2005, 2007; Tsunozaki et al., 2008; Macosko et al., 2009) (also see Fig. 3F). However, the behaviors of animals from these two strains were distinct (Fig. 2A–C), suggesting that ASI does not act solely via activation of AWC.
Different subsets of sensory neurons are necessary for negative thermotaxis bias under distinct conditions
The above experiments established sufficiency of subsets of sensory neurons to generate negative thermotaxis bias under different conditions in a tax-4 mutant background; we next investigated their necessity. We generated strains in which each of the AWC or ASI neurons has been genetically ablated via cell-specific expression of caspases in an otherwise wild-type background (see Materials and Methods) (Chelur and Chalfie, 2007). A strain in which the AFD neurons have been similarly ablated via expression of a caspase has been reported (Glauser et al., 2011). We confirmed that ablation of each of the AFD, AWC, and ASI neurons did not significantly affect overall speed at either Tc = 15°C or Tc = 20°C (legend to Fig. 3).
We first determined the requirement for each of the previously identified AWC and AFD thermosensory neurons in the generation of negative thermotaxis bias under conditions of varying Tc and Tstart. Surprisingly, we found that ablation of AFD or AWC resulted in significant defects in negative thermotaxis bias only under a limited set of examined conditions (Fig. 3A,B). Thus, loss of AFD alone abolished the negative bias at Tc = 15°C and Tstart = Tc + 6°C (Fig. 3A,C), but had little effect under other examined conditions (Fig. 3A–E). Ablation of AWC also significantly decreased negative thermotaxis bias at Tc = 15°C and Tstart = Tc + 6°C (Fig. 3A,C), as well as at higher temperatures relative to Tc (Fig. 3A,B), but had little effect under other conditions (Fig. 3A–E).
We next determined whether multiple thermosensory neurons are necessary to confer negative thermotaxis bias under specific conditions. We found that while ablation of both AFD and AWC resulted in a lack of any bias when Tc = 15°C regardless of Tstart (Fig. 3C,E), no effects were observed at Tc = 20°C (Fig. 3D). In contrast, ablation of both AFD and ASI abolished negative thermotaxis bias under all conditions (Fig. 3C–E). Loss of ASI alone resulted in enhanced negative thermotaxis bias under a subset of conditions (Fig. 3D,E). It is likely that additional compensatory mechanisms contribute to this behavior, since we found that loss of AWC and ASI restored negative thermotaxis bias at Tc = 15°C and Tstart = Tc + 6°C (Fig. 3C). The cell specificity of the ablations were verified by examining the behaviors of strains in which one or more of these neurons were ablated via caspase expression under independent promoters (“EC,” Fig. 3C,D). Together, these results indicate that different sensory neuron combinations are necessary to generate negative thermotaxis bias under different conditions.
The contributions of sensory neurons to negative thermotaxis bias can be altered by enhancing their output
The above data indicate that at Tc = 15°C and Tstart = Tc + 6°C, ablation of AFD strongly decreases negative thermotaxis bias (Fig. 3A,C). Since AWC and ASI act partly redundantly under other conditions together with AFD (Fig. 3D,E), we investigated whether activation of one or both of these neuron types would be sufficient to bypass the effects of AFD loss under these conditions.
Expression of pkc-1(gf) in either AFD or ASI in an otherwise wild-type background did not affect negative thermotaxis bias (Fig. 3F), although expression in AWC weakly enhanced the bias (Fig. 3F). However, pkc-1(gf) expression in either AWC or ASI in an AFD-ablated background was sufficient to fully bypass the behavioral defects and restore negative thermotaxis bias (Fig. 3F). Expression of pkc-1(gf) in both AWC and ASI did not further enhance the behavioral phenotype (Fig. 3F). These results indicate that activation of either AWC or ASI output can bypass loss of AFD function under the conditions examined, further supporting the hypothesis that these neurons can act degenerately to generate negative thermotaxis bias.
The ASI neurons respond to temperature within a Tc-defined operating range
Since a role for the ASI chemosensory neurons in regulating thermosensory behaviors has not been described previously, we next investigated whether ASI responds to temperature by expressing the genetically encoded Ca2+ sensor GCaMP 2.2b (Nakai et al., 2001) cell-specifically in ASI and monitoring intracellular Ca2+ dynamics. In response to a linear upward or downward temperature ramp with a slope of 0.01°C/s (Clark et al., 2006), we detected Ca2+ transients in the ASI cell bodies. These responses were stochastic and characterized by an average 3 s rise in Ca2+ followed by an average 20 s decay back to baseline with varying amplitudes (Fig. 4A). Responses were largely observed at temperatures greater than Tc at both Tc = 15°C and 20°C when temperatures were rising or falling, respectively (Fig. 4Bi,Cii), although responses were also observed at temperatures below Tc under the converse conditions (Fig. 4Bii,Ci). Response amplitudes did not vary significantly across the operating ranges (Fig. 4B,C). We also noted that there may be an upper bound to the response range at Tc = 15°C when temperatures were rising such that fewer responses were observed at higher temperatures (Fig. 4Bi). Response frequency was significantly decreased in tax-4 mutants under all conditions (Fig. 4B,C) indicating that as in AWC and AFD, ASI thermosensory responses are likely to be mediated via cGMP signaling.
Our behavioral experiments indicate that the contribution of different sensory neurons to negative thermotaxis bias varies with Tstart. To mimic the conditions encountered by animals when initiating negative thermotaxis and to further examine the operating range of ASI, we grew animals at 15°C or 20°C, and then shifted them to a different temperature immediately before imaging. Consistent with observations in response to an upward linear temperature ramp, we found that for animals grown at Tc = 15°C, Ca2+ responses were first observed when shifted to 17°C (Fig. 5A,B). Response frequency increased when shifted to 19°C, but then decreased when shifted to 21°C and higher (Fig. 5A,B). Similarly, for animals grown at 20°C, responses were first observed at 20°C with an increase in frequency upon shift to higher temperatures followed by a decrease (Fig. 5C,D). Thus, when grown at either 15°C or 20°C, highest response frequency was observed when animals were shifted to Tc + 3 to 5°C, with fewer responses at or below Tc as well as at temperatures far from Tc (Fig. 5B,D). These results indicate that ASI has a defined but plastic operating range whose lower and upper bounds may be defined by Tc.
The operating range of ASI may be partly set by AFD-mediated neuromodulation
Since ASI shares synaptic connections with neurons previously implicated in the thermosensory circuit (White et al., 1986), we next examined whether thermosensory responses of ASI are set via neuromodulation. To examine a role for synaptic transmission, we examined ASI responses in animals carrying the e51 lesion in the unc-13 synaptic protein gene; this lesion specifically affects synaptic vesicle exocytosis (Speese et al., 2007). We also examined animals mutant for the unc-31 CAPS protein gene, which regulates dense core vesicle exocytosis and thus neuropeptidergic signaling, and may also affect synaptic transmission (Ann et al., 1997; Berwin et al., 1998; Renden et al., 2001; Grishanin et al., 2004; Fujita et al., 2007; Sieburth et al., 2007; Speese et al., 2007). The average frequency and amplitude of ASI Ca2+ responses were largely unaffected in either mutant background (Fig. 5E), implying that ASI may be directly thermosensory, although regulation of ASI responses via electrical synapses cannot be ruled out (Macosko et al., 2009; Chatzigeorgiou and Schafer, 2011). Interestingly however, we found that the operating range of ASI was shifted in unc-31 but not in unc-13 mutants in a Tc-dependent manner (Fig. 5C,D). Thus, in unc-31 mutants grown at 20°C, a significant number of Ca2+ events were observed when animals were shifted to 17°C, with a correlated downward shift in the upper bound of the operating range (Fig. 5C,D). This shift was evident at Tc = 20°C, but not at Tc = 15°C (compare Fig. 5B,D), implying a Tc dependence of this signaling.
We next determined whether signaling from AFD plays a role in setting the operating range of ASI. To do so, we examined temperature-induced Ca2+ responses in ASI in strains lacking AFD. We found that a significant number of events were also observed at 17°C with a downward shift in the upper bound, similar to observations in unc-31 mutants (Fig. 5C,D). The effects of AFD ablations were again evident at Tc = 20°C but not at 15°C (Fig. 5B,D). Consistent with the lack of direct synaptic connections from AFD to ASI (White et al., 1986), these results suggest that AFD may signal via neuropeptides to set the operating range of ASI under specific conditions.
Discussion
Our results suggest that degeneracy among thermosensory neurons contributes to the maintenance of behavioral robustness. We identify the ASI chemosensory neurons as new thermosensory components of this circuit, and describe a role for neuromodulation in regulating thermosensory neuron properties. Our findings emphasize the importance of defining conditions when dissecting neuronal contributions to a behavior, and demonstrate that similar behavioral strategies can be driven by different neuron combinations.
Multiple sensory neuron configurations can generate the same behavioral strategy under different conditions
Different combinations of AFD, AWC, and ASI are necessary and sufficient to generate negative thermotaxis bias depending on the environmental context (Fig. 6). It is important to note that the AFD, AWC, and ASI neurons are not structurally or functionally redundant in that each neuron type has independent functions and contributes to generation of the bias and the overall behavior in a unique manner (Bargmann and Horvitz, 1991; Bargmann et al., 1993; Mori and Ohshima, 1995; Ailion and Thomas, 2000; Wagner, 2005; Chung et al., 2006; Biron et al., 2008; Kuhara et al., 2008; Whitacre and Bender, 2010) (Fig. 6A). Instead, it is the ability of each of these neuron types to be incorporated into the thermosensory circuit under specific conditions that permits the generation of negative thermotaxis bias over a wide range of environmental parameters. Why specific neuron types are required under specific conditions is not yet clear, although it is intriguing to note that the extent of degeneracy appears to correlate with absolute temperature (Fig. 6B). Thus, while loss of any one of two single thermosensory neurons affects the bias at lower temperatures, this behavior is affected only upon loss of one pairwise combination of three thermosensory neurons at the highest temperatures examined (Fig. 6B). We speculate that increased degeneracy among thermosensory neurons driving negative thermotaxis at higher temperatures enables increased robustness allowing animals to escape potentially harmful conditions.
Work in multiple systems suggests that degeneracy may be a common property of neuronal networks. For instance, simulation has shown that the same output can be generated by compensatory changes in synaptic strengths and neuronal properties in a simple model circuit (Prinz et al., 2004; Tang et al., 2010), and reconfiguration of contributions of individual neurons under different conditions results in similar motor patterns from the crab gastric mill central pattern generator circuit (Saideman et al., 2007). In C. elegans, a distributed circuit contributes to hyperoxia avoidance under different conditions (Chang et al., 2006), and different groups of gustatory neurons are recruited to mediate attraction to salt depending on oxygen concentrations (Pocock and Hobert, 2010). Recent work also indicates that robustness of mating behavior is conferred via a distributed network of ray sensory neurons in the C. elegans male (Koo et al., 2011). This flexibility in circuit configuration may greatly enhance the adaptability of the system to constantly changing environmental demands.
Our results also raise the intriguing possibility that many, and perhaps all, C. elegans sensory neurons are polymodal. To date, at least five sensory neuron types including ASI, AFD and AWC in C. elegans have been shown to respond to multiple sensory stimuli (Bargmann et al., 1993; Hart et al., 1995; Maricq et al., 1995; Biron et al., 2008; Kuhara et al., 2008; Chatzigeorgiou et al., 2010; Bretscher et al., 2011). This multifunctionality may greatly expand the sensory repertoire of an organism with a small number of sensory neurons, and further increases the opportunities for functional degeneracy.
Each thermosensory neuron type responds to temperature in a unique manner
An intriguing feature of the C. elegans thermosensory neurons is that each neuron type exhibits cell type-specific responses to temperature changes. Imaging experiments have shown that Ca2+ dynamics in AFD to temperature stimuli are deterministic and highly reproducible and are restricted to temperatures above Tc (Kimura et al., 2004; Clark et al., 2006). Although AFD exhibits a restricted operating range for stimuli relevant for isothermal tracking behavior around Tc (Wasserman et al., 2011), the responses of these neurons to temperature changes are similar over the temperature range relevant for negative thermotaxis (Kimura et al., 2004; Clark et al., 2006) (Fig. 6A). In contrast, the AWC neurons exhibit stochastic temperature-evoked Ca2+ changes that are stimulus-correlated with different temporal kinetics at temperatures above and below Tc (Biron et al., 2008). Moreover, AWC appears to be more active at temperatures further from Tc (Biron et al., 2008) (Fig. 6A). Temperature-induced activity in ASI is again distinct; these neurons also exhibit temperature-induced stochastic Ca2+ transients in a defined operating range with decreased responses close to or far from Tc (Fig. 6A). Regardless of these unique patterns of responses, these neurons are able to partly compensate for loss of other thermosensory neurons under defined conditions (Fig. 6B). Moreover, manipulation of neuronal output via expression of pkc-1(gf) can also bypass the requirement for specific neuron types. Thus, the behavioral output must be an emergent property of the circuit following integration of information from thermosensory neurons. A major next step will be to determine how the circuit compensates for the loss of individual neuron types by comparing activity in shared and unique downstream interneuron and motor neuron subsets in the absence and presence of specific thermosensory neurons.
Neuromodulation may coordinate the operating ranges of multiple thermosensory neurons
Despite exhibiting different temperature response patterns, both AFD and ASI are responsive to temperature changes largely above Tc (Kimura et al., 2004; Clark et al., 2006; this work). Coherence of the Tc-dependent response thresholds between the two neuron types may be regulated via neuropeptidergic signaling from AFD at Tc = 20°C. This signaling may serve to ensure that both neurons are functional in the same temperature range relative to Tc. Coherence among network components via neuromodulation has been observed in the circadian network in mammals where nearly all cell types can generate rhythms in a cell-autonomous manner, but whose phases are synchronized in part by hormonal signaling from the “master” oscillator in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (Reppert and Weaver, 2002; Dibner et al., 2010). However, despite the obvious importance of AFD, the thermosensory circuit can still retain functionality in the absence of AFD under certain circumstances, highlighting its distributed nature. It remains possible that signaling within the thermosensory circuit is more complex and may include feedback signaling from shared downstream interneurons to modulate thermosensory neuron properties (Tomioka et al., 2006; Ignell et al., 2009; Chalasani et al., 2010).
Our observations reveal remarkable complexity in the small and hard-wired neuronal network of C. elegans. Given the conservation of neuronal functions and circuit properties across species, this work suggests that degenerate components that generate the same behavioral strategy under different conditions may be a common feature of most, if not all nervous systems, and may contribute to the maintenance of critical behaviors under dynamic environmental conditions.
Footnotes
This work was supported in part by the NIH (R01 GM081639 to P.S., Core Grant P30 NS45713 to the Brandeis Biology Department, T32 EB009419 to M.B.) and the National Science Foundation (Integrative Graduate Education and Research Traineeship Program DGE-0549390 to M.B.). We are grateful to Harry Bell for technical assistance, Cori Bargmann, Miriam Goodman, and Kyuhyung Kim for reagents, and Cori Bargmann, David Biron, Eve Marder, Miriam Goodman, Sara Wasserman, and the Sengupta laboratory for critical comments and advice.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Piali Sengupta at the above address. sengupta{at}brandeis.edu