Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

The General and Outcome-Specific Forms of Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer Are Differentially Mediated by the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell

Laura H. Corbit and Bernard W. Balleine
Journal of Neuroscience 17 August 2011, 31 (33) 11786-11794; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2711-11.2011
Laura H. Corbit
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Bernard W. Balleine
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Schematic representation of excitotoxic lesions of the NAC core (A) and shell (B) for Experiment 1. Shaded areas represent the maximum (dark gray) and minimum (light gray) extent of the lesions for the animals included in the behavioral analyses. Coronal sections are taken from the following points in the anteroposterior plane beginning at top left: +1.7, +1.2, +1.0, and +0.7 mm anterior to bregma (Paxinos and Watson, 1998). C, Schematic representation of the placement of cannula tips for the NAC core (left) and shell (right) groups for Experiment 2.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    A, Pavlovian conditioning. Mean number of magazine entries during the CS presentations and during the pre-CS intervals (±1 SEM) across days of Pavlovian training for the three lesioned groups. Rats in all groups learned to enter the magazine selectively during the stimulus periods. B, Instrumental conditioning. Mean lever-press responses for the three groups across days of training (±1 SEM). For the first day, each response was reinforced. Thereafter, responding was reinforced on an RR-5 schedule of reinforcement for days 2–4, and on an RR-10 schedule of reinforcement for days 5–7. Rats in all groups acquired the instrumental responses and increased response rates across days.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Effects of core and shell lesions on the outcome-specific and general forms of Pavlovian–instrumental transfer. A, Mean number of lever presses during the Pavlovian–instrumental transfer tests (+SEM). The number of responses during the baseline period in which no stimuli were present is displayed in each case. “Same” refers to the stimulus paired in training with the same outcome as was earned by the available lever. “Different” refers to the stimulus paired with the outcome earned by the other response. “General” refers to the third stimulus paired with the reward not earned by either response. Sham rats (left) demonstrated both outcome-specific and general transfer effects. The performance of shell rats (middle) was increased by stimulus presentation, but they showed no evidence of an outcome-specific PIT effect. In contrast, core rats (right) showed evidence of outcome-specific PIT but a reduced general PIT effect. *Responding during the stimulus was significantly different from responding during the baseline intervals (p < 0.01). ⋀Responding during the “same” stimulus was greater than during the “different” stimulus (p < 0.01). B, Effects of shell and core lesions on outcome-specific PIT. Mean lever presses during each of the same and different stimuli with baseline response rates for each group subtracted. *Responding was greater during the same compared to the different stimulus; **significant difference compared to the sham group. C, Effects of shell and core lesions on general PIT. Mean lever presses during the general stimulus for each group with their respective baseline response rates subtracted. **Significant difference compared to the sham group.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    A, Pavlovian conditioning. Mean number of magazine entries during the CS presentations and during the pre-CS intervals (±1 SEM) across days of Pavlovian training for the core and shell groups. Both groups learned to enter the magazine selectively during the stimulus periods. B, Instrumental conditioning. Mean lever-press responses for the core and shell groups across days of training (±1 SEM). For the first day, each response was reinforced. Thereafter, responding was reinforced on an RR-5 schedule of reinforcement for days 2–4, and on an RR-10 schedule of reinforcement for days 5–7. Rats in both groups acquired the instrumental responses and increased response rates across days.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Effects of reversible inactivation of the core or shell on the outcome-specific and general forms of Pavlovian–instrumental transfer. A, Mean lever presses (+SEM) during each stimulus and the baseline intervals in the PIT tests following saline and muscimol infusions for rats in the shell group. Following saline, there was evidence of both specific and general PIT. Following inactivation, while there was still an excitatory influence of the stimuli, the outcome-specific PIT effect was lost. B, Mean lever presses (+SEM) in the PIT tests following saline and muscimol infusions for rats in the core group. Following saline, there was evidence of both specific and general PIT. Following inactivation, while the same stimulus still elevated performance relative to both baseline and the different stimulus, providing evidence of outcome-specific PIT, the general excitatory influence of the stimuli was reduced. **Significant increase from baseline (p < 0.0063); *p < 0.05; ⋀responding during the “same” stimulus was greater than during the “different” stimulus (p < 0.05). C, Effects of shell and core inactivation on outcome-specific PIT. Baseline responses were subtracted from lever presses during the same and different stimuli to further evaluate the effects of inactivation on any excitatory effects. *Greater responding during the same compared to the different stimulus; **reduction from saline treatment and a significant group difference. D, Effects of shell and core inactivation on general PIT. Mean lever presses during the general stimulus with the baseline response rate subtracted. **Reduction from saline treatment and a significant group difference.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Design of Experiments 1 and 2

    ConditioningInstrumental trainingTransfer test
    S1–O1S1Embedded Image
    S2–O2R1→O1; R2→O2S2R1 vs R2
    S3–O3S3
    • During initial conditioning rats received pairing of three auditory cues, S1, S2, and S3 (noise, tone, and clicker, counterbalanced), with three separate outcomes, O1, O2, and O3 (pellet, sucrose, or polycose, counterbalanced). They then received instrumental training on two separate lever press actions, R1 and R2, each delivering one of the outcomes used in initial conditioning, O1 or O2. Finally a test was conducted on the levers in extinction in the presence of each of the three stimuli. The tests of S1 and S2 provide an assessment of the effects of the outcome-specific predictions of the cues. The test of S3, predicting an outcome not earned on the levers, provides a test of the general excitatory effects of reward prediction.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 31 (33)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 31, Issue 33
17 Aug 2011
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The General and Outcome-Specific Forms of Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer Are Differentially Mediated by the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
The General and Outcome-Specific Forms of Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer Are Differentially Mediated by the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell
Laura H. Corbit, Bernard W. Balleine
Journal of Neuroscience 17 August 2011, 31 (33) 11786-11794; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2711-11.2011

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
The General and Outcome-Specific Forms of Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer Are Differentially Mediated by the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell
Laura H. Corbit, Bernard W. Balleine
Journal of Neuroscience 17 August 2011, 31 (33) 11786-11794; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2711-11.2011
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Episodic Reinstatement in the Medial Temporal Lobe
  • Musical Expertise Induces Audiovisual Integration of Abstract Congruency Rules
  • The Laminar Development of Direction Selectivity in Ferret Visual Cortex
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.