Abstract
Although it is well established that multiple frontal, parietal, and occipital regions in humans are involved in anticipatory deployment of visual spatial attention, less is known about the electrophysiological signals in each region across multiple subsecond periods of attentional deployment. We used MEG measures of cortical stimulus-locked, signal-averaged (event-related field) activity during a task in which a symbolic cue directed covert attention to the relevant location on each trial. Direction-specific attention effects occurred in different cortical regions for each of multiple time periods during the delay between the cue and imperative stimulus. A sequence of activation from V1/V2 to extrastriate, parietal, and frontal regions occurred within 110 ms after cue, possibly related to extraction of cue meaning. Direction-specific activations ∼300 ms after cue in frontal eye field (FEF), lateral intraparietal area (LIP), and cuneus support early covert targeting of the cued location. This was followed by coactivation of a frontal–parietal system [superior frontal gyrus (SFG), middle frontal gyrus (MFG), LIP, anterior intraparietal sulcus (IPSa)] that may coordinate the transition from targeting the cued location to sustained deployment of attention to both space and feature in the last period. The last period involved direction-specific activity in parietal regions and both dorsal and ventral sensory regions [LIP, IPSa, ventral IPS, lateral occipital region, and fusiform gyrus], which was accompanied by activation that was not direction specific in right hemisphere frontal regions (FEF, SFG, MFG). Behavioral performance corresponded with the magnitude of attention-related activity in different brain regions at each time period during deployment. The results add to the emerging electrophysiological characterization of different cortical networks that operate during anticipatory deployment of visual spatial attention.
Introduction
The ability to covertly shift and hold anticipatory spatial attention is an important aspect of cognition (Posner 1980). Functional MRI studies have shown that multiple frontal, parietal, and visual sensory regions are active during anticipatory deployment of visual spatial attention (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Serences and Yantis, 2006). Scalp ERP studies indicate that multiple direction-specific attentional stages occur within fractions of a second following a cue to covertly deploy attention to a spatial location in anticipation of a stimulus, and they support the possibility of different configurations of active cortical regions in each deployment stage, although the brain regions involved in each ERP stage are not well established (Hopf and Mangun 2000; Nobre et al., 2000a; Praamstra et al., 2005; Slagter et al., 2005; van der Lubbe et al., 2006; Dale et al., 2008).
While there is general support for top-down frontal and parietal control during visual attention (Buschman and Miller, 2007; Saalmann et al., 2007; Bressler et al., 2008; Ruff et al., 2008; Capotosto et al., 2009; Gregoriou et al., 2009), evidence for the notion that different frontal and parietal control regions are involved during the different temporal stages of anticipatory attentional deployment is just beginning to emerge (Green and McDonald, 2008; Lauritzen et al., 2009; Pantazis et al., 2009). Recent EEG/MEG imaging studies of oscillatory activity provided initial insights into the complexities of the dynamic electrophysiological activity in brain regions during attentional deployment. Different distributions of active cortical regions have been found for alpha, beta, and gamma frequencies when measured over the whole delay period (Siegel et al., 2008). Siegel et al. (2008) examined the dynamics for two of these active regions and found that the temporal pattern of direction-specific attention activity within each region, and the relative timing between regions, also varied with frequency. Separate investigations of dynamics have been conducted for theta (Green and McDonald, 2008) and alpha band (Pantazis et al., 2009) activity, revealing that cortical areas with attention-related activity vary at each period during deployment. Comparison of these studies suggests that while there are commonalities, the active cortical regions at each stage differ for alpha and theta. Detailed determinations of which regions are involved at each period, and the nature of the electrophysiological processes in each region, is important for a full understanding of the functional properties of attentional deployment. The present study measures stimulus-locked, signal-averaged [event-related field (ERF)] direction-specific attention activity across multiple frontal, parietal, and sensory cortical regions at each of multiple time periods during deployment. We examined whether active cortical regions support an early covert shift to the cued location, similar to that reported in visual search (Buschman and Miller, 2007), and a late period that may provide a top-down controlled bias signal for the location and feature of the anticipated target. We also propose that between the early and late periods, there is coordination between frontal and parietal regions to make the transition from early spatial orienting to biasing of both space and feature.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Eight right-handed, neurologically normal volunteers (six male; age, 27.4 years; SD, 4.3 years) participated in the study. Three additional subjects were excluded due to excessive head movements (>5 mm) during MEG recording. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects, and the experiment was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of California, San Francisco.
Task and stimuli
Subjects were instructed to maintain fixation on a cross-hair located at the center of their visual field (Fig. 1). They performed a cued visual spatial attention task while their MEG was recorded. A brief central arrow cue (50 ms) instructed subjects to covertly deploy and hold their attention to the left or right lower quadrant in anticipation of a unilateral S2 stimulus to be presented 1 s later at either the cued or uncued location (50/50 probability of occurring at either location); that is, the cue was not predictive of S2 spatial location, but was 100% instructive regarding where to attend. If the S2 occurred at the cued location, subjects performed a go/no-go grating discrimination; if it occurred at the uncued location, it was to be ignored. The S2 gratings could be one of two types: a 5° tilt clockwise from vertical was a nontarget requiring no response, and a 20° tilt was a target requiring a button press with the right index finger (if it occurred at the cued location). This produced a 25% probability that the S2 would be a target stimulus at the cued location, thus requiring a response. The design produced an equal probability of receiving a relevant stimulus (i.e., S2 at the cued location) or an irrelevant distractor (i.e., S2 at the uncued location), and required performance of a feature discrimination at the relevant spatial location.
Task design: covert anticipatory shifts of visual spatial attention. On each trial, a central arrow cue (50 ms duration) directed subjects to covertly deploy and hold their attention to the left (←‹) or right (›→) lower visual quadrant (continuously marked by circular gray patches). Following a 1 s delay, a second stimulus (S2, 80 ms duration) was delivered at either the cued or uncued location (50/50 probability of occurring at either location). Subjects performed a go/no-go discrimination of the S2 grating orientation only if it occurred at the cued location and ignored any stimuli presented at the uncued location. The S2 stimuli were categorized into four types: (cued vs uncued location) by (target orientation vs standard orientation). A 5 or 20° clockwise tilt of the grating orientation from vertical constituted a standard or target feature, respectively. The design produced an equal probability of receiving a relevant stimulus (i.e., S2 at the cued location) or an irrelevant distractor (i.e., S2 at the uncued location) and required performance of a feature discrimination at the relevant spatial location. The size of the arrow cue is larger, and the contrast level of gratings is exaggerated for easier visualization.
The locations of the anticipated S2 stimuli were continuously marked by circular gray patches (50% greater luminance relative to background), 2.25° in diameter located in the left and right lower quadrants at 5.125° eccentricity along the diagonal from fixation to the center of the patch. The S2 grating stimuli were grayscale images of stationary sine-wave-modulated contrasts at 1.75 cycles per degree that filled the left or right circular patch for a duration of 85 ms. The peak-to-peak contrast modulation was from 10% above background luminance to 90% above background; consequently, the mean luminance was the same as the nonmodulated gray patch, i.e., 50% above background, thereby not creating a luminance change with the S2 onsets. The nontarget gratings were rotated clockwise from vertical by 5°, and the target gratings were rotated by 25°. This ensured that subjects could not determine whether the S2 was a target stimulus simply by detecting that it was off the vertical; they had to determine the degree of tilt from vertical. The discrimination difficulty resulted in sufficient variation in performance within and between subjects to conduct analyses of the relationship between performance and magnitude of anticipatory attention effects. Arrow cues (←< and >→) were 0.75° wide and 0.50° high, and were presented immediately above the fixation cross. The arrow cue was designed to convey directional information as a unitary entity. Both sides of the cue object convey the same directional information. For example, in a left cue (←<) both the left and right sides convey the same directional meaning (← and –<). Subjects reported that the cue is processed as a unitary entity, an arrow, that conveys directional information seemingly “automatically” (consistent with the literature on reflexive processing of arrows; Ristic et al., 2002). We used an arrow cue, because we wanted an unambiguous, highly overlearned and extremely rapidly processed cue to examine the possibility of rapid shifting. There was a 2150–2950 ms variable intertrial interval. Each trial consisted of an arrow cue (left/right direction was varied pseudorandomly across trials) followed 1 s later by an S2 stimulus, followed by a variable intertrial interval. Participants received 18 blocks of 50 trials per block (900 trials total), with each block lasting ∼130 s and breaks between blocks as needed to sustain good attention throughout the study. Stimuli were delivered via a liquid crystal projection system with a screen placed 36 cm from the nasion.
Data collection
MRI acquisition.
Anatomical MRIs were obtained for each participant to generate models of cerebral tissues for MEG source imaging and definition of cortical regions of interest (ROIs). The MRI data were acquired at 1.5 tesla, using a SENSE head coil (Philips Gyroscan Intera; Philips Medical Systems). A whole head volume was acquired in 200 axial slices with a three-dimensional gradient echo sequence (256 × 256 matrix; 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.2 mm; turbo factor, 180; shot interval, 3000 ms; TI, 769.6 ms; TE, 3.7 ms; TR, 7.9 ms; flip angle, 8°). For spatial coregistration of MRI data with the MEG sensor array, we marked the nasion and the left and right preauricular points using multimodality radiographic markers (MM 3002; IZI Medical Products).
MEG acquisition and preprocessing.
Continuous MEG signals were acquired using a 275-channel whole-cortex CTF Omega 2000 system (VSMedTech Systems). A third-order synthetic gradiometer correction was applied to remove far-field noise. The head location within the MEG helmet was recorded with three magnetic coils during the short breaks between blocks, and subjects with excessive head movements (>5 mm) were excluded from analyses. The MEG recording bandpass was 0–300 Hz, and data were sampled at 1200 Hz. Vertical and horizontal eye movements were measured with bipolar vertical and bipolar horizontal DC-recorded electroculograms. All trials were inspected for eye movements and other artifacts (MEG artifacts, muscle activity, eye blinks, etc.) and contaminated trials were discarded, leaving ∼700 trials per subject for analyses. The artifact-rejected MEG data were low-passed filtered at 40 Hz and averaged, using epochs from −200 to 1400 ms, with respect to cue onset to create ERFs for source imaging.
Data processing
MEG cortical source imaging.
A tessellated representation (40,000 vertices) of the cortical surface was constructed from the volumetric T1-weighted MR images of each subject, coregistered to the MEG data. The MR images were segmented using FreeSurfer (Dale et al., 1999). The spatial distributions of signal-averaged sensor waveforms (ERFs) were mapped to the cortical surface of each subject using a regularized L2 minimum norm inverse procedure (Hämäläinen et al., 1993; Dale et al., 2000; Darvas et al., 2004), with the publicly available BrainStorm Matlab toolbox (http://neuroimage.usc.edu/brainstorm/). The resulting map provides an estimate of cortical current density at every vertex for each time sample of each ERF. The cortical current magnitudes at successive time points during the experimental epoch (brain source waveforms) provide the dynamic activity information for further analyses.
Extraction of ROI-based MEG data.
We analyzed cortical current image maps in each subject using anatomically predefined ROIs. This a priori approach is desirable for its data reduction and its signal detection sensitivity because it summarizes activity over the vertices in each ROI and can detect weak brain activation that could be missed with methods that examine all vertices in the brain requiring severe statistical thresholding. We defined 11 regions commonly found across multiple fMRI and PET studies of visual spatial attention (Corbetta et al., 1998; Coull and Nobre, 1998; Gitelman et al., 1999; Kastner et al., 1999; Rosen et al., 1999; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Nobre et al., 2000b; Vandenberghe et al., 2000; Yantis et al., 2002; Molenberghs et al., 2007; Serences and Yantis, 2007). Each ROI was defined by deriving the superset of published Talairach coordinates for that region and determining the maximum range of values reported across studies in x, y, and z axes. The ROI locations and extents are shown in Figure 2, and the Talairach coordinates for the center of each ROI are given in Table 1. The Talairach coordinates of ROIs were mapped across individual subject's brains using surface-based morphing in FreeSurfer (Fischl et al., 1999a,b). We use relatively large ROIs (in keeping with the blurred nature of MEG images) that provide approximate locations of activity, which can be referred to the growing functional imaging literature for comparison. Due to the relatively low spatial resolution of MEG imaging, activity found in an ROI does not pinpoint the activity to that ROI nor restrict it to that ROI, but indicates that the activity arises in the vicinity of that ROI. The same interpretation issue applies to a data-driven MEG analysis approach because the results are also spatially blurred.
Anatomical ROIs. The locations and extents of the 11 ROIs (yellow patches) are illustrated from different views of the inflated cortex of the right hemisphere (although ROIs were used in both hemispheres, only the right is illustrated here for simplification of viewing). Light and dark gray correspond with gyri and sulci, respectively. Top left, Anterior view of right hemisphere. Top right, Posterior view of right hemisphere. Bottom left, Lateral view of right hemisphere. Bottom right, Medial view of right hemisphere. Fusiform, Fusiform gyrus; V1/V2, early-tier visual sensory cortices in the vicinity of V1/V2; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; FEF, frontal eye field; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; IPSa, anterior intraparietal sulcus anterior; LIP, lateral intraparietal area; IPSv, ventral intraparietal sulcus ventral; LO, lateral occipital region.
Talairach coordinates for ROIs
Each ROI contains multiple vertices from the tessellated cortical surface. To reduce the data and obtain a representative waveform for each ROI, we extracted the source waveforms for all vertices within each ROI, identified the 20 vertices having the largest peak amplitude, and derived the mean waveform of these 20 vertices, using the same vertices for every time point, thereby reducing the data to a single waveform representation for each ROI. A representative waveform was derived for each condition (cue left, cue right) in each of the 11 ROIs for the cue delay period (including a 200 ms baseline preceding the onset of the cue and 1000 ms after cue onset), and these were submitted to statistical analyses (see below, Analyses).
MEG cortical cross-talk and interpretive issues.
The cortical images of activity are spatially blurred due to the nature of the MEG inverse method. This results in spread of activity from one region into a neighboring region (cross talk), and the extent of spread depends upon the point-spread function in the area. Regions that are further apart will have less contamination from activity arising in the other area. While some of the regions used in this study are relatively close to each other, it should be noted that it is possible to detect activity arising in one region versus that of its neighbor when their activity differs in timing or in response to experimental conditions, thereby overcoming the common activity in each region (as is found in the present data).
Temporal definition of time periods used during cue–target delay.
To further reduce the high dimensionality of the data and speak directly to the large ERP literature, we defined four a priori time periods corresponding to commonly reported scalp ERP components during the delay following a cue. We defined the time periods, analogous to defining “components,” through an assessment of the range of postcue latencies reported in the literature for the following four attention-related scalp components (Harter et al., 1989; Hopf and Mangun, 2000; Nobre et al., 2000a; van Velzen et al., 2002; Praamstra et al., 2005; Slagter et al., 2005; Green and McDonald, 2006, 2010; Jongen et al., 2006; Simpson et al., 2006; van der Lubbe et al., 2006; Grent-‘t-Jong and Woldorff, 2007; Dale et al., 2008). (1) An early, short-duration process (positivity contralateral early) occurring during 125–175 ms; (2) a subsequent component, often referred to as the early directing attention negativity (EDAN), occurring between 250 and 350 ms; (3) a middle period associated with activity commonly called the anterior directing attention negativity (ADAN), occurring between 400 and 500 ms; and (4) a late period toward the end of the cue delay up to the onset of the target, which would correspond to 800–1000 ms in this study, often called the late attention directing positivity. Because the latency and duration of components varies somewhat across published studies within our a priori ranges, we divided each period into two equal duration windows to better define the timing of the component activity in the present study, resulting in eight windows.
Data analyses
Direction-specific anticipatory attention activity during attentional deployment.
We defined direction-specific attention activity as the difference in activity in a region when anticipatory attention is directed to the contralateral versus ipsilateral visual field location. Our hypothesis was that underlying each period during attentional deployment there is direction-specific attention activity (cue direction by hemisphere interaction) in a subset of cortical regions (ROIs). This was assessed by first submitting the magnitude of MEG cortical ROI activity to an ANOVA having four within-subject factors—cue direction (left, right), cortical hemisphere (left, right), cortical region (ROI), and temporal component (components)—and testing for a significant cue by hemisphere by ROI by component interaction. Finding this four-way interaction to be significant (F(60,420) = 2.734; p = 0.04) after Greenhouse–Geisser correction for sphericity, we then examined each of the components (time periods) and tested for significant cue by hemisphere interactions in each ROI without corrections for multiple comparisons (Fig. 3) and report these interaction effects in the Results section.
Method for deriving direction-specific anticipatory visual attention effects. Direction-specific attention effects on brain activity are defined in the ANOVA model as interaction effects between factors cue direction (left, right) and ROI-hemisphere (left, right). An example of such an interaction is graphed at the top. ROI activity is measured in the left and right hemisphere under two conditions: when spatial attention is cued toward or away from the visual field contralateral to the ROI hemisphere, corresponding to conditions in which the spatial location contralateral to the cortical region is attended or unattended, respectively. For example in the right hemisphere ROI, cue left–cue right conditions correspond to attended–unattended conditions. Comparison between the same conditions (cue left–cue right) for the left hemisphere should reveal the effect with opposite sign because this comparison corresponds to unattended–attended conditions. This interaction effect appears graphically as a nonzero slope between the values for the left and right hemispheres. An example is illustrated in the figure for IPSa. The IPSa MEG activity waveforms for each condition (cue left and cue right) are plotted in the middle. The a priori time window for defining the amplitudes entered into the ANOVA (400–500 ms in this example) is shown by the vertical box. (Note that to capture the activity predicted by the literature, each a priori time window is divided into two equal subwindows due to variance in the latencies from the literature. The data shown in the plot in the top panel are for the 400–450 ms subwindow.) The mean amplitude during the subwindow is determined for each attention condition, and the resultant value for subtracting cue left and cue right mean amplitudes for a hemisphere is plotted in the top panel, reflecting the attention effect in that hemisphere.
Correspondence between anticipatory attention activity and subsequent performance.
To determine whether anticipatory attentional modulations of cortical activity benefit subsequent behavioral performance, we conducted an analysis of the magnitude of attentional modulation in each cortical region versus the speed of reaction time (RT). This was accomplished by dividing trials into two groups: the fast group (trials with RTs less than the median RT) and the slow group (trials with RTs greater than the median RT). An ERF (the mean across trials) was obtained for each group of trials (fast and slow). A direction-specific attention modulation (the between-condition difference in ERF magnitude for attention cued to the contralateral hemifield vs ipsilateral hemifield) was calculated for fast and slow ERFs in each cortical region. The attention modulation values were entered into an ANOVA with two within-subjects factors: RT (fast, slow) and hemisphere (left, right). An ANOVA was performed for each of the four time windows used in the main analyses above.
Results
Behavioral results
Participants were required to respond only to target gratings presented at the cued location. Participants correctly detected about 80% of cued targets [81.5 ± 11.4%; the false negative rate was about 17% (17.3 ± 11.1%)] with <2% false positive responses (1.6 ± 1.8%). The target detection rate was similar for the left visual field (79.8 ± 14.2%) and right visual field (83.2 ± 10.4%; t(7) = −0.92, not significant). The mean reaction time for cued targets was 500.2 ± 79.2 ms. The reaction time for cued targets was significantly faster by about 20 ms for the right visual field (490.4 ± 80.7 ms) than for the left visual field (510.7 ± 79.9 ms; t(7) = 3.48; p < 0.05).
Flow of cortical activations preceding earliest (150–175 ms after cue) direction-specific attention effect
It is widely postulated that cue-driven voluntary deployment of spatial attention must proceed via bottom-up extraction of symbolic cue meaning that leads to top-down direction-specific attentional deployment processes. Yet, human cueing studies have not examined the initial temporal flow of activations from lower-tier visual regions to parietal and frontal control regions to determine whether control regions are activated sufficiently in advance of the first direction-specific attention effect to allow for communication between control regions and the sensory region that is modulated. To determine this, we performed an analysis of the first onset latencies in occipital, parietal, and frontal regions before the first attention effects in sensory cortex (during 150–175 ms in cuneus; see below). For each cortical region, the onset of event-related activity to cues was estimated as the first time point at which the magnitude exceeded a baseline threshold (i.e., the mean plus two SDs of activity occurring in the baseline period defined as −200 to +25 ms relative to cue onset). The sequence of cortical onset latencies is as follows: V1/V2, 66.5 ms; cuneus, 77.2 ms; inferior parietal lobe (IPL), 84.4 ms; frontal eye field (FEF), 94.8 ms; middle frontal gyrus (MFG), 107.3 ms. The results reveal a sequence of activations from sensory to parietal to frontal regions (Fig. 4) between 60 and 107 ms, indicating that there had been significant activation of sensory, parietal, and frontal regions at least 40 ms before the first direction-specific effect observed at 150 ms (see the following section for direction-specific latencies).
Early onsets of activity across cortical regions. The waveforms illustrate the initial onsets of activity to the cue stimulus from representative ROIs in occipital (V1/V2; cuneus), parietal (IPL), and frontal lobes (FEF, MFG). The waveforms show differences in onset timing consistent with conduction time between regions and indicate that bottom-up processing of cue information has occurred throughout these regions by ∼110 ms after cue.
Examination of multiple temporal periods during anticipatory deployment of visual spatial attention: direction-specific attention effects
Earliest direction-specific modulation of sensory cortex
Following the bilateral non-direction-specific activations described above, there was a statistically significant and direction-specific modulation of the cuneus (Fig. 5) in which the activity was larger in the hemisphere contralateral to the location indicated by the cue [cue by hemisphere interaction during 150–175 ms after cue, F(1,7) = 12.92, p = 0.009; post hoc analyses for cue effect (cue left vs cue right) within hemisphere, left hemisphere (LH), t = 2.64, p < 0.05; right hemisphere (RH), not significant]. No other direction-specific effects were found during this period.
Earliest (150–175 ms) direction-specific effect. The first statistically significant direction-specific attentional modulation of cortex was found in the occipital cuneus region during the 150–175 ms period following the cue. The cue by hemisphere interaction effect in the cuneus is illustrated in the right panel: the difference in MEG magnitude between the two attention conditions (cue left − cue right) is shown for the LH and RH cuneus regions.
Early period direction-specific attention effects in parietal and occipital regions
During the next temporal period (250–350 ms after cue), significant direction-specific effects were found in both parietal [lateral intraparietal area (LIP)] and occipital (cuneus) regions as illustrated in Figure 6(cue by hemisphere interaction, LIP, 250–300 ms, F(1,7) = 7.29, p = 0.03; cue effect within RH, t = 2.89, p < 0.05; 300–350 ms, F(1,7) = 8.95, p = 0.02; cue effect within LH, t = 2.05, p < 0.05; cuneus, 250–300 ms, F(1,7) = 8.36, p = 0.02, bilateral). There was a trend for cue by hemisphere interaction in the lateral occipital (LO) region (250–300 ms, F(1,7) = 5.49, p = 0.052).
Early period (250–350 ms): direction-specific attention system. The early period, 250–350 ms after cue, significant direction-specific attention effects occurred in a parietal and occipital system involving LIP and cuneus regions. In addition, there was a trend effect in the LO region (p = 0.052) shown in the plots of attention effects. The cue by hemisphere attention effects are illustrated in the right panels for the subwindows of the early period (250–300 and 300–350 ms).
Middle period direction-specific effects in prefrontal and parietal control regions
In contrast to the preceding periods (and the last period, see below), direction-specific effects during the middle period (400–500 ms after cue) involved prefrontal [superior frontal gyrus (SFG), MFG] and parietal [LIP, anterior intraparietal sulcus (IPSa), IPL] control regions without any directional effects in sensory regions (Fig. 7). Cue by hemisphere interactions were found in the SFG (400–450 ms, F(1,7) = 6.83, p = 0.03, bilateral) and the MFG (450–500 ms, F(1,7) = 7.70, p = 0.03, bilateral), as well as the LIP (400–450 ms, F(1,7) = 10.69, p = 0.01, bilateral), IPSa (400–450 ms, F(1,7) = 12.56, p = 0.009, bilateral; 450–500 ms, F(1,7) = 16.06, p = 0.005; cue effect within the LH, t = 3.33, p < 0.01), and IPL (400–450 ms, F(1,7) = 16.88, p = 0.005; cue effect within the RH, t = 2.05, p < 0.05).
Middle period (400–500 ms): direction-specific attention system. Direction-specific processing in the middle period (400–500 ms after cue) during anticipatory attentional deployment occurred in a different system involving prefrontal and parietal control regions—SFG, MFG, LIP and IPSa—and did not involve sensory regions.
Late sustained period: direction-specific effects in different parietal and occipital regions from the early period
The final period during anticipatory deployment (800–1000 ms after cue) involves direction-specific effects in a fourth set of cortical regions that is different in its cortical configuration from the earlier periods. Significant cue by hemisphere effects were found in LIP, IPSa, and ventral IPS (IPSv) (Fig. 8). In contrast to preceding periods having modulation only in dorsal occipital regions, significant cue by hemisphere effects were found in both dorsal (LO) and ventral (fusiform) occipital regions, consistent with attention-related modulation based on both location and feature aspects of the anticipated target (LIP, 800–900 ms, F(1,7) = 11.66, p = 0.01; cue effect within LH, t = 2.89, p < 0.01; 900–1000 ms, F(1,7) = 13.76, p = 0.008, cue effect within LH, t = 4.9, p < 0.001; IPSa, 800–900 ms, F(1,7) = 6.44, p = 0.04, cue effect within LH, t = 3.03, p < 0.01; 900–1000 ms, F(1,7) = 6.51, p = 0.04, cue effect within LH, t = 2.55, p < 0.05; IPSv, 800–900 ms, F(1,7) = 7.90, p = 0.03, bilateral; 900–1000 ms, F(1,7) = 6.80, p = 0.04, bilateral; LO region, 900–1000 ms, F(1,7) = 7.08, p = 0.03, cue effect within LH, t = 3.4, p < 0.01; fusiform, 800–900 ms, F(1,7) = 5.87, p = 0.046, bilateral; 900–1000 ms, F(1,7) = 6.20, p = 0.042, bilateral).
Late period (800–1000 ms): direction-specific attention system and frontal attention system. The final period during anticipatory attentional deployment leading up to the onset of the imperative stimulus (800–1000 ms after cue) involved two cortical systems. One system was related to direction-specific processes, and the other system was comprised of right hemisphere frontal regions (indicated by white patches and labels). This frontal system had late sustained activity that was significantly above precue baseline levels but was not direction specific, consistent with a top-down control anticipatory role. The direction-specific attention system includes a different configuration of parietal regions (LIP, IPSa, IPSv) relative to preceding periods, and, in contrast to the early period, having modulation only in dorsal occipital regions, this final system involves both dorsal (LO) and ventral (fusiform) occipital regions, consistent with attention-related modulation based on both location and feature aspects of the anticipated target. In the frontal system, the right hemisphere frontal regions, the FEF, SFG and MFG, have significantly increased levels of activity during the final period of anticipatory attention.
Late sustained activity in frontal regions
The direction-specific (cue by hemisphere) effects during the late sustained period occurred only in parietal and occipital regions and not in frontal regions. Frontal areas are commonly found in fMRI studies of anticipatory attention, and control regions can play a more general role that does not reflect a direction-specific effect. To determine whether frontal regions might be active at the same time that there were direction-specific activations in the parietal and occipital regions, we tested for significant increases in activity above baseline during the late window (800–1000 ms) for the three frontal regions (FEF, SFG, MFG). This was accomplished for each frontal region (FEF, SFG, MFG) by deriving the mean of the absolute value of activity during each of the two windows in the period (800–900 and 900–1000 ms) and obtaining the average of the two conditions (cue left, cue right) in each subject. We then tested whether the activity in each window was significantly greater than the mean of the 200 ms precue baseline window using a one-tailed t test [one-tailed because the postcue absolute value activity can only be greater (it cannot be smaller) than the mean of 0 of the baseline period]. There was significantly increased activity in FEF, SFG, and MFG in the right hemisphere (FEF, 800–900 ms, t(7) = −3.89, p < 0.01; 900–1000 ms, t(7) = −3.43, p < 0.05; SFG, 800–900 ms, t(7) = −5.54, p < 0.001; 900–1000 ms, t(7) = −6.33, p < 0.001; MFG, 800–900 ms, t(7) = −4.12, p < 0.01; 900–1000 ms, t(7) = −3.71, p < 0.01), and not in the left hemisphere (see Fig. 8).
Confirmation of a priori time periods s as representative time periods
The above set of results report small variations in onset or offset latency within the a priori time periods, consistent with the amount of temporal variation reported in the EEG literature (i.e., significant effects occurred in the first or second window or spanned both windows in the a priori period). To provide a qualitative assessment that the literature-based a priori windows are representative of the time periods during which there are significant direction-specific attention effects, we ran an exploratory ANOVA on successive windows across the epoch (25 ms windows for 100–200 ms; 50 ms windows for 200–600 ms; 100 ms windows for 600–1000 ms after cue). The timing of cue by hemisphere interaction effects closely replicate those found for the a priori time periods from the EEG literature, with two instances of small variations in offset after the end of the a priori windows, consistent with the amount of temporal variation reported across studies. During the middle period (a priori window 400–500 ms), effects in the MFG and IPSa appear to continue until 550 ms. Importantly, there are no isolated periods of attention-related effects occurring outside of the literature-derived a priori windows including the period from 550 to 800 ms.
Cross-talk issues
While the spatial spread of activity created by the inverse method makes it more difficult to resolve activity in neighboring regions, the results illustrate how neighboring regions can be selectively identified when their activity differentially varies over time or as a function of experimental condition. For example, during 250-350 ms, the LIP and cuneus have attention-related activity, whereas the IPSv does not, despite its location between LIP and cuneus (i.e., a neighbor to both regions). Even so, due to low spatial resolution, activity found in an ROI (e.g., LIP or cuneus) does not pinpoint the activity to that ROI nor restrict it to that ROI, but indicates that the activity arises in the vicinity of that ROI. In the present data, there are instances in which neighboring regions are active at the same time, however, because there are other times when they are independently active, we report all regions separately.
Correspondence between direction-specific anticipatory attention activity and subsequent performance
To determine whether these anticipatory attentional modulations of cortical activity benefit subsequent behavioral performance, we analyzed the magnitude of attentional modulation in each cortical region with respect to reaction time. Direction-specific attention modulation was calculated for fast and slow RTs and submitted to an ANOVA with two within-subjects factors: RT (fast, slow) and hemisphere (left and right). Fast and slow response times were not significantly different for targets in left and right visual fields [fast, left visual field, 427.5 ± 71.2 ms (SD); right visual field, 422.8 ± 65.3 ms (SD); slow, left visual fields, 581.0 ± 86.7 ms (SD); right visual field, 570.9 ± 96.7 ms (SD)]. Results revealed a correspondence between attentional modulation during each time period during deployment and the speed of target detection, with the critical region differing for each period. In the first period (125–175 ms), the magnitude of attentional modulation in the cuneus region was greater for fast trials (F(1,7) = 14.48, p < 0.01, 125–150 ms). In the second period (250–350 ms) greater attentional modulation in FEF was associated with faster target detection (F = 7.26, p < 0.05, 300–350 ms). Greater attentional modulation in IPSv during the middle period (400–500 ms) corresponded with faster RT (F = 6.99, p < 0.05, 400–450 ms). During the late period, there was an RT by hemisphere interaction in the LO region (F(1,7) = 11.46, p < 0.05, 800–900 ms; F = 14.47, p < 0.01, 900–1000 ms), and post hoc tests revealed that this was due to the correspondence between attentional modulation and RT occurring predominantly in the left hemisphere.
The effects in FEF and IPSv indicate that these regions were an important part of the attentional systems at the early and middle periods, respectively, although they did not achieve significant direction-specific effects in the analysis above (Examination of multiple temporal periods during anticipatory deployment of visual spatial attention: direction-specific attention effectssection). Because large direction-specific amplitudes correspond with faster RTs and small amplitudes correspond with slower RTs in these areas, the combination across all trials is likely to reduce the overall direction-specific effect as analyzed above.
Discussion
The present study used cortical MEG ERF activity to identify cortical regions that show direction-specific activity during four time periods associated with anticipatory deployment of visual spatial attention.
It is widely postulated that cue-driven voluntary deployment of spatial attention must proceed via bottom-up extraction of symbolic cue meaning that leads to top-down direction-specific attentional deployment. However, there has not been a demonstration of the initial flow of activation from sensory to control regions before the earliest direction-specific modulation of sensory cortex. Such a modulation might indicate the time by which the symbolic cue meaning has been extracted and top-down attentional modulation of the sensory representation for the cued location initiated. In the present study, the temporal sequence of activity proceeds from occipital V1/V2 to extrastriate and parietal regions, then to frontal and then prefrontal cortex from 66 to 110 ms after cue (supplementary Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). The first direction-specific modulation in the cuneus occurs at 150–175 ms (and the strength of this modulation corresponds with faster subsequent target discrimination). The difference in timing between the bottom-up processing and the modulation of cuneus is sufficient for integrative processes in frontal and parietal control regions to occur and perhaps influence the direction-specific modulation of the cuneus, although current results cannot confirm a direct influence. The direction-specific modulation in cuneus could reflect an attention-related process or reentrant/secondary processing (Bar et al., 2006) related to extraction of the directional meaning or some type of late sensory processing of the slight physical asymmetry in the stimulus.
After the initial modulation of sensory cortex at 150–175 ms after cue, examination of three subsequent time periods during the cue–target delay revealed significant attention effects in different cortical regions. The next time period during attentional deployment (250–350 ms after cue) involves the FEF, LIP, cuneus, and possibly LO region, and may be similar in function to rapid covert shifts of spatial attention found in visual search. The task relevance of the left and right markers on the screen is ambiguous before the cue, but following the cue, one of the markers becomes relevant and can be targeted with a covert shift of attention. The magnitude of direction-specific activity in FEF during this early time period was predictive of faster reaction times, but when activity from FEF was averaged over trials with fast and slow response times, the overall attention effect was not statistically significant. Human fMRI studies have shown FEF, LIP, cuneus, and LO region to be involved in shifting visual spatial attention (Hopfinger et al., 2000; Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Molenberghs et al., 2007; Asplund et al., 2010), and the timing of activity in the current study suggests that FEF, LIP, cuneus (and possibly LO region) are involved in the covert targeting of the task-relevant stimulus marker. This is consistent with the attentional control functions attributed to FEF and LIP from studies of covert shifts of attention (Colby and Goldberg 1999; Schall, 2002; Thompson et al., 2005; Buschman and Miller, 2009; Gregoriou et al., 2009) and with the top-down modulation of sensory regions by these control regions (Moore et al., 2003; Bressler et al., 2008). A burst of coherent gamma activity between widespread sensors over the head around this time in deployment (Doesburg et al., 2008) suggests that interactions between frontal, parietal, and sensory regions are involved. Consensus is building across studies of oscillatory and stimulus-locked ERF measures that attentional modulation can occur at a relatively early time in the deployment process (Green and McDonald, 2008; Siegel et al., 2008; Pantazis et al., 2009), and there is evidence for behavioral importance via correlations with performance in the superior parietal lobe (SPL), IPL (Green and McDonald, 2008), and LO region (Wyart and Tallon-Baudry, 2009), as well as from the relationship between FEF activity and performance in the present study.
Unlike the other periods during anticipatory attentional deployment, which show attention activity in control and sensory regions (suggesting top-down modulations of sensory representations), the middle period (400–500 ms) involves solely frontal and parietal control regions (SFG, MFG, LIP, IPSa, IPL), suggesting a different function involving coordination between frontal and parietal control regions. This coordination, following covert targeting of the relevant location, may serve to integrate control processes needed to shift attention with respect to both the immediate spatial instruction (cue left/right) and the overall task instruction (discriminate the target feature at the cued location). Dynamics of direction-specific attention modulations in parietal and frontal regions have been shown in one other study, using oscillatory measures in the theta band, whereby activity peaked in frontal areas (MFG, SFG) in this time frame, but not in parietal regions (SPL, IPL) (Green and McDonald, 2008). Instead, parietal activity peaked during early (SPL, IPL) and late (IPL) periods. It may be that our stimulus-locked signal-averaged ERF measures pick up a transient and consistently stimulus-locked process needed to coordinate activity between parietal and frontal control regions under certain conditions. The involvement of direction-specific effects in parietal regions during this time period also appears to be frequency dependent, with the SPL, IPL, and IPS active in alpha and beta bands (Siegel et al., 2008; Pantazis et al., 2009), but not in theta or gamma bands (Green and McDonald, 2008; Siegel et al., 2008). The findings for this and the previous time period identify brain regions that contribute to the commonly reported scalp effects, EDAN and ADAN, occurring in these time periods (Hopf and Mangun, 2000; Nobre et al., 2000a; Praamstra et al., 2005; Green and McDonald, 2006; Jongen et al., 2006; van der Lubbe et al., 2006; Dale et al., 2008).
The late period from 800 ms after cue until target arrival involves direction-specific modulation of parietal (IPSa, IPSv, LIP) and occipital (LO, fusiform) regions, as well as non-direction-specific activity in right hemisphere frontal regions (FEF, SFG, MFG). While the function of these nonspecific effects in frontal areas is unclear from this study alone, results are consistent with a role for frontal cortex in sustaining visual spatial attention in general (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Vandenberghe et al., 2001; Grent-‘t-Jong and Woldorff, 2007). The timing of coactivation with other regions suggests coordination with parietal control regions that are also thought to be involved in sustaining anticipatory visual spatial attention (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Silver et al., 2005; Serences and Yantis 2007; Saygin and Sereno 2008; Doesburg et al., 2009). This is also the only period in which both dorsal and ventral visual areas are modulated. Earlier time windows show attentional modulation only in dorsal sensory regions, suggesting primarily spatially related targeting, whereas the involvement of both dorsal and ventral regions in the final period leading up to target arrival suggests a more integrated biasing for space and feature. EEG/MEG studies have found posterior direction-specific anticipatory attention activity immediately preceding the imperative stimulus that may reflect a bias signal (Worden et al., 2000; Eimer et al., 2002; Praamstra et al., 2005; Sauseng et al., 2005; Slagter et al., 2005; Green and McDonald, 2006, 2010; Thut et al., 2006; van der Lubbe et al., 2006; Grent-‘t-Jong and Woldorff, 2007; Dale et al., 2008; for gamma-band evidence for decision bias, see also Wyart and Tallon-Baudry, 2009). There is some debate as to whether this late activity originates in parietal and/or occipital regions, and some studies have shown this activity to be related to performance, adding support for its role as a bias signal (Thut et al., 2006; Green and McDonald, 2008; Wyart and Tallon-Baudry, 2009). While the cortical origins are likely to depend in part on the task, recent studies, including the present one, now show such late activity, lasting until target arrival, in occipital regions (Green and McDonald, 2008) or both parietal and occipital regions (Siegel et al., 2008; Pantazis et al., 2009; present study), with performance-related modulations found in occipital regions [inferior occipital gyrus (IOG) (Green and McDonald, 2008) and LO (present study)].
In summary, this study identifies direction-specific cortical ERF activity from multiple frontal, parietal, and sensory regions, adding to an emerging characterization of the complex electrophysiological signals arising from cortical regions dynamically involved in anticipatory deployment of spatial attention. The results show attention effects in different regions in each of the temporal periods examined during deployment. An early period involving FEF, LIP, cuneus, and possibly the LO region may reflect a covert shift to the cued location marker similar to a process found in visual search. Late attention effects in parietal and both dorsal and ventral occipital regions are consistent with the notion of a baseline shift/bias signal. A middle period involves only frontal and parietal regions, and might reflect coordination between control regions to transition from the early covert spatial shift to the subsequent biasing of both spatial and feature representations. Each of the temporal periods examined in cued attentional deployment is functionally important for performance, as indicated by a correspondence between reaction time and the magnitude of direction-specific attention activity in different regions during each period.
Footnotes
This work was supported by NIH NINDS Grants R01 NS27900 and R01 NS45171 (G.V.S.), National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering Grants R01 EB002010 and R01EB009048 (R.M.L.), NIMH Grant 64204 (S.L.B.), and NIA Grant K01AG027369 (T.L.L.). We thank Drs. John Mosher, Sylvain Ballet, Thomas Ferree, Matti Hamalainen, Thomas Nichols, John Kornak, and Morgan Hough for helpful suggestions, statistical advice, and software developments, and Eleanor Wong for technical assistance.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Gregory V. Simpson at his present address, Brain Plasticity Institute, 1 Montgomery Street, 7th Floor, San Francisco, CA 94104. Greg.BrainPlasciticityInstitute{at}gmail.com